Next Article in Journal
Discursive Competition in the Tourist Platform Economy of a Large City (Madrid)
Previous Article in Journal
Primary Forestry Industry Cluster in Honduras: A SWOT–CAME Analysis
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Volunteering in Environmental Organizations and Subjective Well-Being: Evidence from a Nationally Representative, Longitudinal Dataset in the US

1
Department of Economics, University of Toledo, Toledo, OH 43606, USA
2
Department of Psychology, University of Toledo, Toledo, OH 43606, USA
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
World 2025, 6(3), 94; https://doi.org/10.3390/world6030094
Submission received: 28 May 2025 / Revised: 2 July 2025 / Accepted: 3 July 2025 / Published: 4 July 2025

Abstract

This study uses a nationally representative longitudinal dataset in the US to examine the long-term association of volunteering for environmental, recycling, and conservation groups with a person’s (a) willingness to continue to volunteer later in life and (b) several measures of their mental and physical well-being including perceived social status, optimism, psychological stress, suicidal thoughts and attempts, depressive symptoms and general self-reported physical health. By using Add Health data, we match responses to an environmental volunteerism question in Wave III (2002) with subjective well-being responses in Wave V (2016–2018) to examine the long-term association between these variables. After excluding missing responses, the analysis sample consists of 9800 individuals. After using linear survey regression analyses and several techniques based on propensity scores (stratification, weighting, matching) two key results emerged: first, being involved in environmental groups and organizations early in life showed a significant positive association with more hours spent on volunteering or community service work later in life; and second, people who volunteer in early adulthood are more optimistic, more sociable, have a higher perceived social status, display less stress and depressive symptoms.

1. Introduction

When asked, most people in the United States state that they care very much about the environment; for instance, according to a recent survey 69% favored the U.S. taking steps to become carbon neutral by 2050 [1]. Fewer people, however, translate this stated concern into real commitment through voluntary actions, i.e., volunteering to recycle, clean-up trash, improve conservation areas (see [2,3,4]). According to [4], this gap is not rare—they report that 72% of their respondents admitted that they observe a discrepancy between their actions and their intentions. Volunteering is not an automatic response to stated environmental concerns. So, what can explain the gap between such reported intentions and actions? Are those individuals who volunteer simply doing so as it contributes to a greater cause, or do their actions also improve their own well-being and increase their long-term commitment to the environment? While some evidence exists on how environmental concerns affect subjective well-being (e.g., [5,6,7]), little is known about how volunteering in environmental organizations, a distinct and well-defined type of pro-environmental behavior, impacts physical/mental well-being and translates into a long-term commitment to environment protection.
Herein we address this open question empirically and explore the link between environmental volunteering and measures of well-being. Our paper explores whether past volunteering in environmental organizations leads to greater mental and physical well-being as well as future volunteering. Using unique features provided by the National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent to Adult Health (Add Health) dataset, we measure a person’s level of care about the environment in terms of their self-reported volunteering of community service to environmental organizations. We evaluate the association of such volunteering in early adulthood (at ages 18–26) with several measures of physical and mental well-being later in life (at ages 32–42).
In more detail, this paper aims to examine the long-term adjusted association of pro-environmental behavior in terms of membership in environmental and conservation clubs earlier in life with measures of mental and physical well-being (perceived social status, optimism, neuroticism, and sociability, perceived stress scale, depressive symptoms, suicidal thoughts, physical health) and continuing volunteerism later in life. We use environmental volunteerism to measure pro-environmental behavior. This covariate comes from Add Health Wave III (2002) and is based on the question about volunteering in conservation, recycling, or environmental groups. We match responses to this environmental volunteerism question with subjective well-being responses in Wave V (2016–2018) to examine the long-term association between these variables. In other words, we analyze the long-term (~16 years) association of environmental volunteerism with individual well-being for US residents.
After excluding missing responses, we conducted our analysis with a sample size of 9800 individuals and our methodology is designed to investigate whether environmental volunteerism is related to individual well-being in the long run. Using regression and matching techniques based on propensity scores, we study the impact of volunteering to environmental organizations on an average person’s physical, mental, and emotional well-being in the long run by controlling for observable demographic and socio-economic confounders.
Our study improves on previous attempts to establish a link between environmental volunteerism and individual well-being in four specific ways. First, our measure of environmental concern is based on a very specific question about environmental volunteerism and this question simply asks whether the respondent has volunteered to environmental organizations. This is important because the quality of self-reported measures may differ [8,9] and, as discussed in [10], answers to frequently used questions in the form of “How often do you recycle paper?” can be imprecise and comparisons of responses across people may be problematic. While volunteerism for environmental groups variable that we use was self-reported by respondents, it is a relevant measure of active involvement in conservation and environmental organizations, as opposed to questions about general levels of care about the environment [5,6,11]. Second, we look at several established standardized, psychological measures of well-being such as the CES-D scale (Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale) and Life Orientation Test. Third, our data comes from a large nationally representative, longitudinal dataset. Fourth, unlike most relevant studies, we evaluate the association of volunteering with individual well-being in the long-term—approximately 16 years later. This long-term perspective matters because some health outcomes are likely to manifest over time.
Examining ongoing volunteer participation in environmental organizations is crucial for environmental policies aimed at changing people’s behaviors and habits [12]. Our analysis can guide efforts to increase volunteerism by emphasizing the otherwise discounted individual benefits of such actions.

2. Literature Review and Background

Past literature has focused on how individuals’ pro-environmental behavior is related to subjective well-being, and whether pro-environmental behavior promotes individuals’ subjective well-being and happiness [5,13,14,15,16,17,18,19,20]. For example, [5,14,16,18,19] examined subjective well-being in the context of climate change, natural hazards, environmental degradation, and disasters, whereas [7,15,17] examined subjective well-being in the context of self-perceived health analyses. Subjective well-being generally refers to how people experience and evaluate their lives and activities in their lives [21]. We define subjective well-being as the degree to which individuals experience positive or negative emotions, and how they evaluate their own lives [22].
Pro-environmental actions are sometimes perceived as costly, effortful, or otherwise unpleasant, potentially leading to a negative impact on subjective well-being (e.g., [23,24]). This implies a negative relationship between pro-environmental behavior and subjective well-being. However, an expanding body of research suggests the opposite—that engaging in pro-environmental actions may, in fact, enhance individuals’ subjective well-being [25].
It has been suggested that the link between pro-environmental behaviors and subjective well-being may be positive because engaging in such behaviors is perceived as meaningful [26,27]. The sense of meaningfulness associated with an action reflects the extent to which it is regarded as important, significant, and morally appropriate [28]. Engaging in meaningful activities fosters a positive self-perception, ultimately contributing to enhanced subjective well-being [27,29].
Our study focuses on a specific form of volunteerism: volunteering in environmental organizations. This focus is meaningful for several reasons [24,30]. First, environmental volunteerism represents a unique intersection of civic engagement and environmental concern, where individuals act not only out of altruism but also out of a commitment to ecological values and sustainability. This dual motivation may lead to distinct psychological benefits compared to other forms of volunteering. Second, environmental volunteering often involves physical, outdoor, and group-based activities—such as habitat restoration or community clean-ups—that are themselves linked to improved well-being through mechanisms like nature exposure, physical activity, and social connection. Finally, prior research on the relationship between volunteerism and subjective well-being has largely treated volunteerism as a general category, without accounting for the cause-specific nature of volunteering [5,6,7]. By examining environmental volunteerism specifically, our study contributes to a more differentiated understanding of how the context of volunteering influences well-being outcomes.
Several studies have shown that general volunteer activities are positively associated with subjective well-being, which is usually measured as self-rated happiness and life satisfaction (e.g., [7,31,32,33,34,35,36]). Among these, [31] specifically focused on public policies to promote volunteerism and consequently subjective well-being, [33,34] focused on voluntary activities and daily happiness, whereas [37] analyzed health benefits of volunteerism. It has been argued that volunteer activities can enhance happiness by providing psychological benefits, such as a sense of usefulness and finding meaning in life [37], and also by increasing levels of social integration through extended social participation and engagement (e.g., [38,39]).
Finally, in the economics literature, the economic benefits of volunteerism have been broadly identified in models examining public goods, private consumption, and investment [40,41,42,43,44]. The public goods model [40,41] focuses on altruistic benefit where the general motivation is to increase supply of public goods. In the private consumption model [42,43], self-value benefits are analyzed, and the general motivation includes joy from the act of volunteering, self-integration, social status, or social ethics. The investment model [44] discusses “exchange benefits” of volunteering such as gaining labor market experience, skills, and contacts and even signaling one’s ability to prospective employers. Microeconomic models along these lines are used to explain volunteer labor supply. Meier and Stutzer [45] provided broader theoretical considerations on the motivation to volunteer.
Our paper contributes to the literature by examining benefits of volunteer work in environmental organizations to the individuals themselves, in terms of their own mental and physical well-being as well as future volunteering in environmental organizations. We provide key insights into the strength, direction and robustness of the relation between environmental community work and subjective well-being.

3. Data and Methods

We use data from Add Health, a U.S. nationally representative school-based survey, which started with a sample of approximately 90,000 school-age adolescents enrolled in grades 7–12 in 1994–1995 in 132 schools nationwide [46]. A nationally representative subset of the respondents from the initial sample was re-interviewed at home in 1994–1995 (Wave I, n = 20,745, ages 12–19), in 1995–1996 (Wave II, n = 14,738, ages 13–20), in 2001–2002 (Wave III, n = 15,170, ages 18–26), in 2008–2009 (Wave IV, n = 15,701, ages 24–32) and in 2016–2018 (Wave V, n = 12,300, ages 32–42).
Our sample includes respondents interviewed in Wave III (2002) and Wave V (2016–2018). Among the 15,170 respondents who took part in Wave III interviews when respondents ranged in age from 18 to 26 years old (7140 males and 8030 females), 12,300 respondents participated in Wave V interviews conducted approximately 16 years later when respondents ranged from 32 to 42 years old (6187 males and 6113 females). In Wave V, these respondents answered questions about general (not only environmental) volunteering, physical and mental health, personality, social support, feelings, and experiences. We draw information on control variables from Wave I at-home interviews (1994–1995).

3.1. Outcome Variables

We investigate if environmental volunteerism is associated with subjective well-being that included measures of mental and physical well-being and continuing volunteering in Wave V. In our analyses, we did not exclude any outcome a priori, but used all available outcomes related to individual well-being. The outcome measures of mental health include perceived social status, measures of optimism and neuroticism (life orientation test), Perceived Stress Scale [47], suicidal ideation, and CES-D (Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale, [48]). Furthermore, we include the outcome of self-reported good physical health and a measure of continuing volunteerism.
Perceived social status ranges between 1 and 10 and is based on the question “Think of this ladder as representing where people stand in the United States. At the top of the ladder (step 10) are the people who have the most money and education, and the most respected jobs. At the bottom of the ladder (step 1) are the people who have the least money and education, and the least respected jobs or no job. Where would you place yourself on this ladder? Pick the number for the step that shows where you think you stand at this time in your life, relative to other people in the United States” [40].
To measure optimism and neuroticism, we created three binary indicator variables based on three items from the Life Orientation Test asked in Wave V which measure dispositional levels of optimism and neuroticism [49]: “How much do you agree or disagree with each statement about you as you generally are now, not as you wish to be in the future? (i) I am always optimistic about my future, (ii) I hardly ever expect things to go my way, (iii) Overall, I expect more good things to happen to me than bad”.
Wave V collected information on all four items of the Perceived Stress Scale such as “In the past 30 days, how often have you felt that you were unable to control the important things in your life” Each item was measured on a 0–4 scale (0 = Never, 1 = Almost never, 2 = Sometimes, 3 = Fairly often, 4 = Very Often). We summed responses, with summed scores ranging between 0 and 16 where higher scores indicate greater perceived stress (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.78).
Next, we created a variable indicating whether the respondent experienced suicidal thoughts or attempts in the past 12 months by combining affirmative responses to the questions “During the past 12 months, have you ever seriously thought about committing suicide?” and “During the past 12 months, how many times have you actually attempted suicide?” [40].
Add Health also collected responses on five items used in the CES-D scale (Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale) which measures depressive symptoms such as “During the past 7 days, I felt that I could not shake off the blues” [42]. The possible responses on each item ranged from 1 = never to 4 = always. We created a single depressive scale by summing responses, with the resulting scale ranging between 5 and 25 (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.82).
For physical health, we created a binary indicator of self-reported good physical health based on the question “How is your general physical health” where the indicator variable was coded 1 for the responses “excellent”, “very good” and “good,” or zero otherwise. Research has established that self-rated health has good predictive validity with respect to mortality [50,51].
Finally, we created a binary variable indicating continuing volunteerism in Wave V based on the question “In the past 12 months, about how many hours did you spend on volunteer or community service work?”.
Based on findings in prior literature on relationships between concerns about the environment and one’s well-being, we expected environmental volunteering to be positively related to measures of mental, and physical well-being and likelihood of continuing volunteering.

3.2. Independent Variables

Our main explanatory covariate was a binary indicator variable measuring environmental volunteerism in early adulthood. The covariate comes from Wave III, based on the question “Which of the following types of organizations have you been involved with in your volunteer or community service work in the last 12 months? Indicate all that apply: conservation, recycling, or environmental groups, such as the Sierra Club or the Nature Conservancy”. It is important to note that this environmental volunteerism question was only asked in Wave III and was not asked again in subsequent waves. This is the reason why we used Wave III for this question.
The other explanatory variables, which we used as statistical controls in multivariate analyses, come from Wave I and include basic demographic and socio-economic characteristics of the respondent and their family available in Wave I of Add Health: age, indicators for gender, race (white, Black), Hispanic ethnicity, Picture Vocabulary Test (PVT) as a proxy for cognitive abilities, log of yearly family pretax income, whether either parent had a college degree, and whether the adolescent lived with both biological parents. It is important to account for basic demographics such as race/ethnicity and cognitive abilities, which may determine socio-economic status, in multivariate analysis due to the known and persistent disparities in health [46].
Past literature has identified age, education, income, gender and other socio-economic factors as important factors that may impact levels of volunteerism and well-being and should be controlled in empirical analyses [5,7,52,53,54,55]. After restricting the sample to observations with valid Wave V sampling weights (n = 12,300), non-missing observations on the volunteering measure in Wave III (deleted 2082 observations), and non-missing observations on dependent variables (deleted additional 418 observations), our final analysis sample had n = 9800.
Add Health followed a strict privacy protocol where respondents answered questions on computers read through headphones, reducing social desirability bias when responding to the pro-environmental question compared to other surveys.

3.3. Methods

First, we used linear survey regression (linear probability model) to estimate covariate-adjusted associations of volunteering with conservation organizations in Wave III with four categories of outcome measures: mental, and physical well-being and likelihood of continuing volunteering.
Second, we used propensity score analysis, an alternative method of accounting for observed confounders by creating a matched or weighted sample in which distributions of measured covariates are similar between treated and untreated respondents. In our case, treated individuals were those who reported environmental volunteering and untreated were those who did not. Before conducting propensity score analyses, we checked for covariate balance by comparing standardized differences in means of the included covariates in the treatment model using the user-written Stata 14.2 command ‘pbalchk’ [56], and did not detect significant (>0.5 standard deviation) differences between treated and untreated respondents
In the first step of the propensity score analysis we used the logistic regression model and the independent variables to estimate propensity scores (predicted probabilities for each respondent of participation in conservation volunteering in Wave III, henceforth referred to as treated respondents). The use of logistic regression is a common choice for predicting propensity scores [57]. In the second step we conducted three types of analyses using estimated propensity scores in order to obtain average treatment effect (ATE) estimates of the effect of volunteering: stratification, weighting, matching. In addition, we used a combination of weighting and stratification.
During stratification, we stratified respondents into 10 quantiles (strata) based on the predicted propensity scores. The number of treated respondents ranged between 8 and 50 and the number of untreated respondents ranged between 930 and 975 across 10 strata, and standardized differences in means of observables (balance check) were not significant in any stratum. The estimates represent differences between treated and untreated respondents across 10 strata.
Next, during weighting we used estimated propensity scores to create sampling weights for linear regression adjustment. The sample used in these estimations was restricted to common support, i.e., to the range of propensity scores in which both treated and untreated respondents were observed. First, we present estimates using linear regression with sample weights equal to the inverse probability of treatment (IPT). Second, we present the same estimates with standardized mortality/morbidity ratio (SMR) weights. IPT weights change the distribution of confounders in both treated and untreated groups to be equal to the distribution in the entire sample. Estimates that use IPT weights compare expected outcomes when everyone received treatment to expected outcomes when no-one received treatment. In contrast, SMR weights only change the distribution of confounders for untreated respondents to match it to the distribution among treated respondents. The SMR weighted analysis compares outcomes for treated respondents with what their outcomes would be if they had remained untreated. The IPT and SMR weighted analyses produce the same estimates if treatment has the same effect on everyone; they produce different estimates if respondents likely affected by the treatment are more likely to receive the treatment [57].
In the third type of propensity score analysis, we use matching based on propensity scores. In “greedy matching”, every treated respondent is compared to every untreated respondent to find the closest match. This procedure continues until there are no more possible matches (Stata command ‘gmatch’ by [50]). In Coarsened Exact Matching (CEM) data are temporarily coarsened and exact matches are produced on these coarsened data, and then the analysis is computed on the un-coarsened, matched data [58]. Coarsened exact matching requires fewer assumptions and possesses more attractive statistical properties than other matching methods [52].
Finally, marginal mean weighting through stratification (MMWS) combines the above two methods—stratification and weighting based on the two different propensity-based weights—IPT and SMR [59,60].
All analyses were weighted using Wave V Add Health sampling weights, rendering estimates applicable to the underlying cohort.

4. Results

Table 1A presents descriptive statistics for all variables used in analyses. In Wave III, 267 respondents (2.7% of the sample) volunteered for environmental and conservation organizations (of the original 280 responses, 13 responses were deleted due to missing data on other variables; naturally, the environmental volunteering group is much smaller than the non-volunteering group, but we still had a large enough sample to estimate statistically significant effects and the advantage of having thousands of untreated individuals is that the matching techniques can find close matches for the 267 treated individuals.) This Wave III question asks about a specific type of volunteering—time given to environmental or conservation organizations. However, in Wave V respondents were asked a general volunteering question. We had 39.6% of the respondents performing general volunteering work in Wave V.
When the sample was stratified by environmental volunteering status in Wave III (Table 1B), respondents volunteering in environmental organizations scored significantly better (based on significance testing and Cohen’s d [61] on the well-being measures in Wave V: they had higher perceived social status, volunteered more than others, were more optimistic, felt less stressed, showed fewer depressive symptoms, were in better health and experienced less suicidal ideation and attempts.
Table 2 presents the results from linear survey regression (linear probability model). Table 3 presents the results from propensity score analyses. In each table, estimates are presented for mental and physical well-being and continuing volunteering.
In Table 3, Panel 1 presents estimates of ATE after stratification, and Panels 2 and 3 present estimates of ATE after using estimated propensity scores to create sampling weights for linear regression adjustment. Panel 2 presents estimates using linear regression with sample weights equal to the inverse probability of treatment (IPT); Panel 3 presents the same estimates but with standardized mortality/morbidity ratio (SMR) weights. Panels 4 and 5 implement matching based on propensity scores. Panel 4 implements “greedy matching”; Panel 5 implements Coarsened Exact Matching (CEM). Panels 6 and 7 present estimates using marginal mean weighting through stratification (MMWS) where Panel 6 uses MMWS weights and Panel 7 uses inverse probability of treatment weights.

5. Discussion of the Results

In this section, we interpret the results across three categories: (1) mental well-being, (2) physical health and future volunteering, and (3) comparisons with the previous literature.

5.1. Results for Measures of Mental Well-Being

The results from linear regression in Table 2 suggest that compared to respondents who did not volunteer in Wave III, those volunteering with environmental organizations earlier in life had significantly higher perceived social status and were more optimistic on the three measures of optimism/neuroticism. Propensity score results in Table 3 are qualitatively and quantitatively similar to results from survey linear regression in Table 2, although in some cases the effect of volunteering in Wave III lost statistical significance for some outcomes in some models.
Linear regression estimates in Table 2 also suggest that environmental volunteering in Wave III reduced perceived stress, reduced suicidal thoughts or attempts and reduced depressive symptoms. Estimates using propensity score analyses in Table 3 echo findings using linear regression in terms of the signs and magnitudes of estimates; however, most coefficients on volunteering lost their statistical significance in case of SMR weighted regressions (Panel 3), greedy matching (Panel 4) and coarsened exact matching (Panel 5). The smaller coefficients when using SMR weights in Panel 3 suggest that respondents whose mental well-being was more likely to improve due to conservation volunteering were less likely to participate in this volunteering.
It is important to note that there is no evidence in the literature that happier people would volunteer more and no empirical evidence of reverse causality between life satisfaction and voluntary activities [31,33,39]. Therefore, our results suggest that environmental volunteerism is likely to improve individual mental, and emotional well-being in the long run supporting prior research [20,21,22,26]: People who volunteer in early adolescence are likely to become more optimistic, more sociable, have higher perceived social status, display less stress and depressive symptoms.

5.2. Results for the Measures of Physical Health and Future Volunteering

According to linear survey regression in Table 2, environmental volunteering in Wave III was associated with greater likelihood of reporting being in good or better physical health and with a greater likelihood of volunteering later in life, in Wave V. The same result was found in all models using propensity scores in Table 3 with the coefficient on volunteering in Wave III being quantitatively similar across models. These results suggest that being involved in and volunteering time for environmental organizations earlier in life translates to better physical health and more hours spent on volunteering or community service work later in life.
We note that R-squared values in all of the models in Table 2 are low (<0.1), which indicates that the included basic demographic and socio-economic controls as well as the environmental volunteering together explain very little variation in the measures of mental and physical well-being and in future volunteering across the respondents in our sample.

5.3. Interpretation and Relation to Existing Studies

Our paper contributes to the literature by examining the long-term impact of environmental volunteerism—specifically, membership in environmental and conservation organizations earlier in life—on various outcomes, including subjective well-being, mental and physical health, and continued volunteerism in later life. Our results support existing studies that report a positive relationship between environmental concerns and subjective well-being (e.g., [5,6,7,20,21,22,26]). However, our contribution lies in demonstrating how concrete pro-environmental behaviors—such as volunteering in environmental organizations—impact well-being and foster sustained environmental engagement.
Our study explicitly distinguishes between holding environmental concerns and engaging in environmental volunteering. The latter aligns more closely with the concept of pro-environmental behavior, which refers to deliberate actions aimed at reducing individuals’ negative environmental impact [2]. By focusing on environmental volunteerism, we address the value–action gap, emphasizing behavior over stated concern. Moreover, unlike studies that rely on broadly defined measures of volunteering due to data limitations (e.g., [7]), our analysis draws on specific data on environmental organization membership, allowing for a more targeted investigation. Finally, our study evaluates the effects of volunteering on well-being over a 16-year period, offering a long-term perspective that is especially relevant for outcomes—such as mental and physical health—that may develop gradually over time.

6. Concluding Remarks

Our study is the first using a nationally representative dataset of US residents to investigate our associations using quasi-experimental methods and matching techniques. Our study contributes to understanding the link between volunteerism and individual well-being using a unique dataset. We find that environmental volunteerism is consistently associated with improvements in subjective, mental, and physical health outcomes.
According to our interpretation, these results matter for environmental policy designed to change people’s behavior. The existence of a value–action gap implies that pro-environmental behavior is not an automatic result of environmental concerns, and there is a gap between people’s perception of environmental problems and possible actions [2,62,63,64]. The volunteering in environmental organizations variable that we used in our study is more aligned with a pro-environmental behavior concept describing activities aimed at reducing the negative impact individuals confer on the environment. Consequently, it might be plausible to think that the value–action gap can shrink if more people understand the positive association of environmental community service and volunteerism with their own well-being. If a person knows that volunteering in environmental organizations is good for their own well-being and the environment, we can expect to see more behavioral change towards environmental volunteerism. To promote sustained engagement in voluntary work, policymakers can complement structural opportunities with targeted informational and educational campaigns that emphasize the personal benefits of volunteering, including improvements in well-being. Policy instruments such as public service announcements, social marketing, awards, and recognition programs can be strategically employed to encourage participation. Even when the primary policy objective is to increase volunteer rates rather than enhance individual well-being per se, highlighting the positive psychological effects of volunteering can serve as an effective strategy for encouraging engagement.
We end by considering three limitations to our study. First, any observed behavior can stem from selection based on unobserved personality traits that may be unaccounted for by covariates. For example, if people with higher levels of life satisfaction are more likely to participate in voluntary activities (reverse causality), then positive effects found in our and other studies may be overstated. However, ref. [39] presented robust evidence that volunteering has a significantly positive impact on well-being, and found no evidence that this positive relationship was driven by reversal causality, when examining the well-being impact of volunteering. This was also the main assumption in earlier studies as it is unclear why happier individuals should also worry more about the environment [31,33]. In the absence of experimental data, it is more difficult to identify causality between environmental volunteerism and individual well-being. While we controlled for possible confounders and individual-specific heterogeneity, observational studies come with some skepticism on the identification of causal effects. However, we note that unless it is a natural experiment, experimental approaches in this context come with their own problems. While our study does not establish causality, it nonetheless advances a step further and adjusts the difference in outcomes for effects of several important covariates that might cause selection bias. Furthermore, this is the only longitudinal dataset for the US asking about environmental volunteering and measures of well-being years after.
Second, the fact that our measure of volunteering for environmental groups was self-reported may be concerning. However, its meaning is straightforward and behaviorally specific because it is based on a discrete yes/no question querying involvement with conservation, recycling, or environmental groups, such as the Sierra Club or Nature Conservancy. While being a relatively more specific and relevant measure of pro-environmental behavior as opposed to questions asking about general levels of care about the environment, it may still be subject to measurement error. However, this binary measure is less prone to measurement error compared to other measures differentiating between levels of intensity of volunteering.
Third, some studies pointed out issues with comparing values of ordinal variables across people and time, and researchers do not have a full consensus on how to interpret these subjective well-being measures [65,66]. However, our study contributes to understanding the link between environmentalism and well-being by using a unique dataset and established psychological measures. Future research should examine how the intensity of volunteerism influences subjective well-being. Survey designs that capture variation in volunteering intensity would offer deeper insights into the relationship between environmental volunteerism and well-being.

Author Contributions

O.S. and J.D.E. contributed to writing and editing of the entire manuscript; A.A. contributed to data analysis and writing and editing of the entire manuscript. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The use of Add Health data for this study received an exempt status by the Institutional Review Board of the University of Toledo.

Informed Consent Statement

This research used secondary data and did not involve human participants and/or animals in research; no informed consent was needed.

Data Availability Statement

This research uses data from Add Health, a program project directed by Kathleen Mullan Harris and designed by J. Richard Udry, Peter S. Bearman, and Kathleen Mullan Harris at the University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill, and funded by grant P01-HD31921 from the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development, with cooperative funding from 23 other federal agencies and foundations. Special acknowledgment is due Ronald R. Rindfuss and Barbara Entwisle for assistance in the original design. Information on how to obtain the Add Health data files is available on the Add Health website (http://www.cpc.unc.edu/addhealth) (accessed on 7 June 2025). No direct support was received from grant P01-HD31921 for this analysis. Information on how to reproduce the analysis is available from the corresponding author on request. The authors of this manuscript are not affiliated with Add Health program project and only analyze data originally collected by Add Health.

Acknowledgments

We thank Jason Shogren for his valuable comments on this paper.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors confirm that they have no competing interests to declare.

References

  1. Pew Research Center 2023. What the Data Says About Americans’ Views of Climate Change. Pew Research Center: Washington, DC, USA, 9 August 2023. Available online: https://www.pewresearch.org/short-reads/2023/08/09/what-the-data-says-about-americans-views-of-climate-change/ (accessed on 22 November 2023).
  2. Chai, A.; Bradley, G.; Lo, A.; Reser, J. What time to adapt? The role of discretionary time in sustaining the climate change value–action gap. Ecol. Econ. 2015, 116, 95–107. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Kormos, C.; Gifford, R. The validity of self-report measures of proenvironmental behavior: A meta-analytic review. J. Environ. Psychol. 2014, 40, 359–371. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Kennedy, E.H.; Beckley, T.M.; McFarlane, B.L.; Nadeau, S. Why we don’t “walk the talk”: Understanding the environmental values/behaviour gap in Canada. Hum. Ecol. Rev. 2009, 16, 151–160. [Google Scholar]
  5. Ferrer-i-Carbonell, A.; Gowdy, J.M. Environmental degradation and happiness. Ecol. Econ. 2007, 60, 509–516. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Binder, M.; Ward, F. The structure of subjective well-being: A vector autoregressive approach. Metroeconomica 2013, 64, 361–400. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Binder, M.; Blankenberg, A.K. Environmental concerns, volunteering and subjective well-being: Antecedents and outcomes of environmental activism in Germany. Ecol. Econ. 2016, 124, 1–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Lange, F.; Steinke, A.; Dewitte, S. The Pro-Environmental Behavior Task: A laboratory measure of actual pro-environmental behavior. J. Environ. Psychol. 2018, 56, 46–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Kaiser, F.G.; Merten, M.; Wetzel, E. How do we know we are measuring environmental attitude? Specific objectivity as the formal validation criterion for measures of latent attributes. J. Environ. Psychol. 2018, 55, 139–146. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Lange, F.; Dewitte, S. Measuring pro-environmental behavior: Review and recommendations. J. Environ. Psychol. 2019, 63, 92–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Urban, J.; Ščasný, M. Exploring domestic energy-saving: The role of environmental concern and background variables. Energy Policy 2012, 47, 69–80. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Welsch, H.; Kühling, J. Using happiness data for environmental valuation: Issues and applications. J. Econ. Surv. 2009, 23, 385–406. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Frey, B.S.; Stutzer, A. What can economists learn from happiness research? J. Econ. Lit. 2002, 40, 402–435. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Rehdanz, K.; Maddison, D. Climate and happiness. Ecol. Econ. 2005, 52, 111–125. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Li, Y.; Ferraro, K.F. Volunteering and depression in later life: Social benefit or selection processes? J. Health Soc. Behav. 2005, 46, 68–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Welsch, H. Environment and happiness: Valuation of air pollution using life satisfaction data. Ecol. Econ. 2006, 58, 801–813. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Borgonovi, F. Doing well by doing good. The relationship between formal volunteering and self-reported health and happiness. Soc. Sci. Med. 2008, 66, 2321–2334. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  18. Sekulova, F.; van Den Bergh, J.C. Climate change, income and happiness: An empirical study for Barcelona. Glob. Environ. Change 2013, 23, 1467–1475. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Li, Z.; Folmer, H.; Xue, J. To what extent does air pollution affect happiness? The case of the Jinchuan mining area, China. Ecol. Econ. 2014, 99, 88–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Yuan, L.; Shin, K.; Managi, S. Subjective well-being and environmental quality: The impact of air pollution and green coverage in China. Ecol. Econ. 2018, 153, 124–138. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. National Research Council. Subjective Well-Being: Measuring Happiness, Suffering, and Other Dimensions of Experience; National Academies Press: Washington, DC, USA, 2013. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Diener, E.; Oishi, S.; Lucas, R.E. Personality, culture, and subjective well-being: Emotional and cognitive evaluations of life. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2003, 54, 403–425. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Epstein, A. ‘97% of Climate Scientists Agree’ Is 100% Wrong Forbes. 2015. Available online: https://www.forbes.com/sites/alexepstein/2015/01/06/97-of-climate-scientists-agree-is-100-wrong/ (accessed on 7 June 2025).
  24. Zawadzki, S.J.; Steg, L.; Bouman, T. Meta-analytic evidence for a robust and positive association between individuals’ pro-environmental behaviors and their subjective wellbeing. Environ. Res. Lett. 2020, 15, 123007. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Kasser, T. Living both well and sustainably: A review of the literature, with some reflections on future research, interventions and policy. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. A Math. Phys. Eng. Sci. 2017, 375, 20160369. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Venhoeven, L.A.; Bolderdijk, J.W.; Steg, L. Why acting environmentally-friendly feels good: Exploring the role of self-image. Front. Psychol. 2016, 7, 1846. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Venhoeven, L.A.; Bolderdijk, J.W.; Steg, L. Why going green feels good. J. Environ. Psychol. 2020, 71, 101492. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. van der Werff, E.; Steg, L.; Keizer, K. It is a moral issue: The relationship between environmental self-identity, obligation-based intrinsic motivation and pro-environmental behaviour. Glob. Environ. Change 2013, 23, 1258–1265. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Taufik, D.; Bolderdijk, J.W.; Steg, L. Going green? The relative importance of feelings over calculation in driving environmental intent in the Netherlands and the United States. Energy Res. Soc. Sci. 2016, 22, 52–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Stern, P.C. Toward a coherent theory of environmentally significant behaviour. J. Soc. Issues 2000, 56, 407–424. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Binder, M.; Freytag, A. Volunteering, subjective well-being and public policy. J. Econ. Psychol. 2013, 34, 97–119. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Han, S. Social capital and subjective happiness: Which contexts matter? J. Happiness Stud. 2015, 16, 241–255. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Binder, M. Volunteering and life satisfaction: A closer look at the hypothesis that volunteering more strongly benefits the unhappy. Appl. Econ. Lett. 2015, 22, 874–885. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Gimenez-Nadal, J.I.; Molina, J.A. Voluntary activities and daily happiness in the United States. Econ. Inq. 2015, 53, 1735–1750. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Lane, T. How does happiness relate to economic behaviour? A review of the literature. J. Behav. Exp. Econ. 2017, 68, 62–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Lee, M.A. Volunteering and happiness: Examining the differential effects of volunteering types according to household income. J. Happiness Stud. 2019, 20, 795–814. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Piliavin, J.A.; Siegl, E. Health benefits of volunteering in the Wisconsin longitudinal study. J. Health Soc. Behav. 2007, 48, 450–464. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Rodríguez-Pose, A.; Von Berlepsch, V. Social capital and individual happiness in Europe. J. Happiness Stud. 2014, 15, 357–386. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Magnani, E.; Zhu, R. Does kindness lead to happiness? Voluntary activities and subjective well-being. J. Behav. Exp. Econ. 2018, 77, 20–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Menchik, P.L.; Weisbrod, B.A. Volunteer labor supply. J. Public Econ. 1987, 32, 159–183. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Andreoni, J. Impure altruism and donations to public goods: A theory of warm-glow giving. Econ. J. 1990, 100, 464–477. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Freeman, R.B. Working for nothing: The supply of volunteer labor. J. Labor Econ. 1997, 15 Pt 2, S140–S166. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Roy, K.; Ziemek, S. On the economics of volunteering (No. 31). In ZEF Discussion Papers on Development Policy; Center for Development Research ZEF: Bonn, Germany, 2000. [Google Scholar]
  44. Ziemek, S. Economic analysis of volunteers’ motivations—A cross-country study. J. Socio-Econ. 2006, 35, 532–555. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Meier, S.; Stutzer, A. Is volunteering rewarding in itself? Economica 2008, 75, 39–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Harris, K.M.; Halpern, C.T.; Biemer, P.; Liao, D.; Dean, S.C. Add Health Wave V documentation: Sampling and Mixed-Mode Survey Design; Add Health: Chapel Hill, NC, USA, 2019. [Google Scholar]
  47. Cohen, S.; Kamarck, T.; Mermelstein, R. A global measure of perceived stress. J. Health Soc. Behav. 1983, 24, 385–396. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Radloff, L.S. The CES-D scale: A self report depression scale for research in the general population. Appl. Psychol. Meas. 1977, 1, 385–401. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Scheier, M.F.; Carver, C.S.; Bridges, M.W. Distinguishing optimism from neuroticism (and trait anxiety, self-mastery, and self-esteem): A reevaluation of the Life Orientation Test. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1994, 67, 1063. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  50. Schnittker, J.; Bacak, V. The increasing predictive validity of self-rated health. PLoS ONE 2014, 9, e84933. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  51. Lorem, G.; Cook, S.; Leon, D.A.; Emaus, N.; Schirmer, H. Self-reported health as a predictor of mortality: A cohort study of its relation to other health measurements and observation time. Sci. Rep. 2020, 10, 4886. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Wilson, J.; Musick, M. The effects of volunteering on the volunteer. Law Contemp. Probl. 1999, 62, 141. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Huang, J.; Van den Brink, H.M.; Groot, W. A meta-analysis of the effect of education on social capital. Econ. Educ. Rev. 2009, 28, 454–464. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Carpenter, J.; Myers, C.K. Why volunteer? Evidence on the role of altruism, image, and incentives. J. Public Econ. 2010, 94, 911–920. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Meyer, A. Does education increase pro-environmental behavior? Evidence from Europe. Ecol. Econ. 2015, 116, 108–121. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Lunt, M. “PBALCHK: Checking Covariate Balance”. 2013. Available online: https://personalpages.manchester.ac.uk/staff/mark.lunt/propensity.html (accessed on 25 April 2023).
  57. Lunt, M. Propensity Analysis in Stata Revision: 1.1. Document. 2014. Available online: https://personalpages.manchester.ac.uk/staff/mark.lunt/propensity_guide.pdf (accessed on 7 June 2025).
  58. Blackwell, M.; Iacus, S.; King, G.; Porro, G. CEM: Coarsened exact matching in Stata. Stat. Stata 2009, 9, 524–546. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Huang, I.; Frangakis, C.; Dominici, F.; Diette, G.B.; Wu, A.W. Application of a propensity score approach for risk adjustment in profiling multiple physician groups on asthma care. Health Serv. Res. 2005, 40, 253–278. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  60. Linden, A. MMWS: Stata Module for Implementing Mean Marginal Weighting Through Stratification; Statistical Software Components s457886; Boston College Department of Economics: Chestnut Hill, MA, USA, 2014. [Google Scholar]
  61. Cohen, J. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences, 2nd ed.; Lawrence Erlbaum: Hillsdale, NJ, USA, 1988. [Google Scholar]
  62. Blake, J. Overcoming the ‘value-action gap’ in environmental policy: Tensions between national policy and local experience. Local Environ. 1999, 4, 257–278. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Kaiser, F.G.; Wölfing, S.; Fuhrer, U. Environmental attitude and ecological behaviour. J. Environ. Psychol. 1999, 19, 1–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Barr, S. Are we all environmentalists now? Rhetoric and reality in environmental action. Geoforum 2004, 35, 231–249. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. MacKerron, G. Happiness economics from 35,000 feet. J. Econ. Surv. 2012, 26, 705–735. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Bond, T.N.; Lang, K. The sad truth about happiness scales. J. Political Econ. 2019, 127, 1629–1640. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Table 1. Descriptive statistics.
Table 1. Descriptive statistics.
(A) Descriptive Statistics for Everyone, n = 9800.
Variable LabelMeanSDMinMax
Outcome measures of subjective well-being
Social status in W5, 1–10socstatus5.4941.874110
I am always optimistic about my future, W5optimism10.7860.41001
I hardly ever expect things to go my way, W5optimism20.1760.38101
Overall, I expect more good things to happen to me than bad, W5optimism30.7690.42101
Outcome measures of mental health
Perceived Stress Scale 4 (PSS-4) score: 0–16, W5pss45.0232.995016
Suicidal thoughts or attempts in past 12 months, W5suicideal0.07200.25801
Sum of 5 depression items, W5cesditems7.3462.522520
Outcome measure of physical health
Good or better health, W5ghealth0.8680.33901
Outcome measure of continuing volunteering
Volunteered in the past 12 months, W5volunteer0.3960.48901
Measure of environmental volunteering
Conservation volunteer past 12 months (0 or 1), W3conserv0.02700.16301
Other covariates
Age, W1 15.011.7311119
Male 0.4220.49401
Black 0.2020.40201
Hispanic 0.1430.35001
Picture Vocabulary Test score, W1 101.014.3410141
Log annual family income, W1 3.6160.851−2.3036.907
Both biological parents present, W1 0.5560.49701
Mom or dad has college degree, W1 0.3710.48301
(B) Descriptive statistics by conservation volunteering status in Wave III, n = 9800.
Mean, Volunteered (n = 267)Mean, Not Volunteered (n = 9533)DifferenceCohen’s d
Outcome measures of subjective well-being
Social status in W5, 1–106.1245.4770.647 **0.345
I am always optimistic about my future, W50.8430.7850.058 *0.141
I hardly ever expect things to go my way, W50.1120.178−0.065 **−0.173
Overall, I expect more good things to happen to me than bad, W50.8540.7670.087 **0.207
Outcome measures of mental health
Perceived Stress Scale 4 (PSS-4) score: 0–16, W54.5175.037−0.520 **−0.174
Suicidal thoughts or attempts in past 12 months, W50.0450.072−0.028 *−0.105
Sum of 5 depression items below, W57.0307.355−0.325 *−0.129
Outcome measure of physical health
Good or better health, W50.9400.8660.075 **0.218
Outcome measure of continuing volunteering
Volunteered in the past 12 months, W50.5840.3900.194 **0.397
Measure of environmental volunteering
Conservation volunteer past 12 months (0 or 1), W31.0000.0001.0006.135
Other covariates
Age, W114.81615.016−0.200−0.116
Male0.4870.4200.066 *0.136
Black0.1240.204−0.081 **−0.199
Hispanic0.1010.144−0.043 *−0.123
Picture Vocabulary Test score, W1106.494100.8135.681 **0.396
Log annual family income, W13.8143.6100.204 **0.240
Both biological parents present, W10.6070.5550.0520.105
Mom or dad has college degree, W10.5470.3660.181 **0.375
Statistical significance for difference in means: * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01.
Table 2. Estimates from linear survey regression of the effect of conservation volunteering in Wave III on well-being and volunteering outcomes in Wave V, n = 9800.
Table 2. Estimates from linear survey regression of the effect of conservation volunteering in Wave III on well-being and volunteering outcomes in Wave V, n = 9800.
SocstatusOptimism1Optimism2Optimism3Pss4SuicidealCesditemsGhealthVolunteer
Conservation volunteer, W30.49 **0.09 **−0.05 *0.11 **−0.57 **−0.05 **−0.58 **0.09 **0.20 **
(0.14)(0.03)(0.02)(0.04)(0.20)(0.01)(0.16)(0.01)(0.04)
Age, W10.03−0.000.000.01 *−0.02−0.01 *−0.00−0.000.01 **
(0.02)(0.00)(0.00)(0.00)(0.03)(0.00)(0.03)(0.00)(0.00)
Male0.020.020.02−0.02 *−0.37 **0.01−0.12−0.02−0.08 **
(0.06)(0.01)(0.01)(0.01)(0.09)(0.01)(0.07)(0.01)(0.01)
Black−0.170.08 **−0.000.08 **0.29 *−0.020.03−0.030.02
(0.09)(0.02)(0.01)(0.02)(0.13)(0.01)(0.13)(0.02)(0.02)
Hispanic0.090.030.030.02−0.03−0.01−0.06−0.01−0.00
(0.09)(0.02)(0.02)(0.02)(0.13)(0.01)(0.15)(0.02)(0.02)
Picture Vocabulary Test score, W10.01 **−0.00 **−0.00 **0.00−0.010.00−0.000.00 **0.00 **
(0.00)(0.00)(0.00)(0.00)(0.00)(0.00)(0.00)(0.00)(0.00)
Log annual family income, W10.19 **0.01−0.02 *0.02−0.06−0.01 *−0.100.010.00
(0.05)(0.01)(0.01)(0.01)(0.07)(0.01)(0.07)(0.01)(0.01)
Both biological parents present, W10.20 **0.01−0.03 *0.01−0.19−0.02 **−0.24 **0.020.03 *
(0.06)(0.01)(0.01)(0.01)(0.10)(0.01)(0.07)(0.01)(0.01)
Mom or dad has college degree, W10.57 **0.03 *−0.06 **0.05 **−0.42 **0.00−0.22 *0.06 **0.10 **
(0.07)(0.01)(0.01)(0.02)(0.11)(0.01)(0.10)(0.01)(0.02)
R squared0.070.010.030.010.020.010.010.020.05
Note: Regression coefficients and SE in parentheses. Statistical significance for difference in means: * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01. See Table 1A for the definition of the outcome variables shown in column headings.
Table 3. Estimates of average treatment effects (ATE) of volunteering in Wave III on measures of well-being and volunteering in Wave V using propensity scores, n = 9800.
Table 3. Estimates of average treatment effects (ATE) of volunteering in Wave III on measures of well-being and volunteering in Wave V using propensity scores, n = 9800.
SocstatusOptimism1Optimism2Optimism3Pss4SuicidealCesditemsGhealthVolunteer
Panel A: Stratifying by propensity score in 10 strata
Conservation volunteer, W30.50 **0.09 **−0.05 *0.11 **−0.58 **−0.05 **−0.59 **0.09 **0.20 **
(0.15)(0.03)(0.03)(0.03)(0.20)(0.01)(0.16)(0.01)(0.04)
Panel B: Using propensity scores for inverse-probability-weighted (IPT) regression adjustment, observations restricted to common support
Conservation volunteer, W30.41 **0.09 **−0.050.11 **−0.56 **−0.05 **−0.55 **0.09 **0.19 **
(0.15)(0.03)(0.03)(0.03)(0.21)(0.01)(0.16)(0.01)(0.04)
Panel C: Using propensity scores for weighted (SMR) regression adjustment, observations restricted to common support
Conservation volunteer, W30.35 **0.06 **−0.030.08 **−0.33−0.03 *−0.210.05 **0.15 **
(0.11)(0.02)(0.02)(0.02)(0.17)(0.01)(0.14)(0.02)(0.03)
Panel D: Greedy matching using propensity scores with caliper = 0.0001
Conservation volunteer, W30.400.07−0.11 *0.17 **−0.39−0.05−0.49 *0.05 *0.27 **
(0.23)(0.04)(0.04)(0.05)(0.32)(0.03)(0.24)(0.02)(0.06)
Panel E: Coarsened Exact Matching using propensity scores
Conservation volunteer, W30.43 *0.09 *−0.040.08 *−0.21−0.03−0.330.08 **0.20 **
(0.17)(0.04)(0.03)(0.04)(0.25)(0.02)(0.20)(0.02)(0.05)
Panel F: Marginal mean weighting (MMWS weights) through stratification using propensity scores
Conservation volunteer, W30.460.11 **−0.08 **0.06−0.53 *−0.04 *−0.55 **0.11 **0.24 **
(0.24)(0.03)(0.03)(0.06)(0.22)(0.02)(0.18)(0.01)(0.05)
Panel G: Marginal mean weighting (IPTW weights) through stratification using propensity scores
Conservation volunteer, W30.52 **0.11 **−0.08 **0.07−0.60 **−0.04−0.58 **0.11 **0.24 **
(0.20)(0.03)(0.03)(0.05)(0.22)(0.02)(0.17)(0.01)(0.05)
Note: Regression coefficients and SE in parentheses. Statistical significance for difference in means: * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01. See Table 1A for the definition of the outcome variables shown in column headings. All models include independent variables from Table 2.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Sapci, O.; Amialchuk, A.; Elhai, J.D. Volunteering in Environmental Organizations and Subjective Well-Being: Evidence from a Nationally Representative, Longitudinal Dataset in the US. World 2025, 6, 94. https://doi.org/10.3390/world6030094

AMA Style

Sapci O, Amialchuk A, Elhai JD. Volunteering in Environmental Organizations and Subjective Well-Being: Evidence from a Nationally Representative, Longitudinal Dataset in the US. World. 2025; 6(3):94. https://doi.org/10.3390/world6030094

Chicago/Turabian Style

Sapci, Onur, Aliaksandr Amialchuk, and Jon D. Elhai. 2025. "Volunteering in Environmental Organizations and Subjective Well-Being: Evidence from a Nationally Representative, Longitudinal Dataset in the US" World 6, no. 3: 94. https://doi.org/10.3390/world6030094

APA Style

Sapci, O., Amialchuk, A., & Elhai, J. D. (2025). Volunteering in Environmental Organizations and Subjective Well-Being: Evidence from a Nationally Representative, Longitudinal Dataset in the US. World, 6(3), 94. https://doi.org/10.3390/world6030094

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop