1. Introduction
The structure of individual variation in the subjectively assessed characteristics of daily rhythms appears to be multidimensional [
1,
2,
3,
4,
5,
6,
7,
8,
9,
10]. However, the variation in only one of several dimensions known under the term “morningness-eveningness” [
11] is usually of interest for studies of individual differences in sleep–wake behavior and habits [
12,
13,
14,
15]. In the framework of such a unidimensional approach, an individual can be assigned to one of two poles of this morningness-eveningness dimension and named either a morning or evening (chrono)type [
16] or nicknamed either a lark or owl, respectively. Usually, morningness-eveningness is assessed as a trait using a morning–evening preference scale (e.g., [
17,
18,
19]). Consequently, individuals occupying the poles of this continuous preference scale can be named morning and evening types. Despite this most popular trait-like approach to morningness-eveningness assessments, it is also often unidimensionally assessed as a state rather than a trait using data on current sleep times on free days (e.g., [
20,
21,
22,
23]).
The correlations between trait and state assessments (i.e., between a preference score and a current sleep timing) are expected to be positive in sign and substantial in magnitude. Therefore, the cases of discrepancy between these assessments are relatively rare [
24,
25,
26]. However, such exceptions cannot be fully excluded, and, therefore, it can be expected that there are individuals with discordant (conflicting) trait and state assessments of morningness-eveningness, i.e., who are classified as evening types based on their trait assessments and as morning types according to their state assessments or, conversely, who are classified as morning types according to their trait assessments and as evening types based on their state assessments. Moreover, neither trait nor state assessments can fully replace the objective measurements of the circadian phase of the rhythms marking the central circadian clock. It was shown that the trait and state assessments of chronotype correlate with the current circadian phase of the melatonin rhythm [
27,
28], but neither correlation was found to be strong [
27,
28], and chronotype categorizations based on either trait or state assessments demonstrated low concordance with that based on the circadian phase measurement [
28].
The questionnaire tools for either trait or state assessments of morningness-eveningness were implemented in a large number of studies testing the links of chronotype with health (e.g., [
29,
30,
31,
32,
33,
34,
35]), mood (e.g., [
36,
37,
38,
39,
40,
41,
42,
43,
44,
45,
46]), and sleep problems (e.g., [
47,
48,
49,
50,
51,
52,
53,
54,
55,
56,
57,
58,
59,
60,
61,
62]). The results of the vast majority of studies suggested that such problems were more frequently associated with evening than morning types.
If morningness-eveningness is assessed as a trait-like characteristic of an individual, it is expected to be stable, long-lasting, and internally caused; conversely, if it is assessed as a state-like characteristic, it can be temporal, short-lasting, and caused by external circumstances. Given that current sleep timing belongs to the state-like characteristics of chronotypes, the questions arise: Can interventions leading to an advance of sleep timing improve health, mood, and sleep in evening types, and can interventions delaying sleep timing compromise health, mood, and sleep in morning types? Due to intercorrelation between trait and state assessments, it is difficult to determine whether the associations between chronotype and these problems are internal or external in nature. Therefore, it is difficult to evaluate the prospects of improving health, mood, and sleep by correcting sleep timing, a state-like characteristic of morningness-eveningness. Light can be shed on the issue of the internal vs. external nature of these associations in a study of individuals with discordant trait and state assessments of morningness-eveningness. If such associations are internally caused, the problems with health, mood, and sleep are expected to be more likely reported by evening types with early sleep timing rather than morning types with late sleep timing. Alternatively, these problems are expected to be more likely reported by morning types with late sleep timing rather than evening types with early sleep timing in the case of externally caused associations.
The conflicting trait and state assessments of morningness-eveningness can be, at least partly, explained by limitations of the unidimensional approach to categorization of chronotypes, i.e., when only two—either evening or morning—types can be distinguished at the poles of a preference scale. The application of a multidimensional approach to chronotyping can suggest that individuals with discordant trait and state assessments belong to other than just two types, morning and evening. For example, LIVEMAN chronotyping was introduced to distinguish between six types rather than two chronotypes (lethargic, vigilant, afternoon, and napping types in addition to morning and evening types) [
6]. Consequently, these four other chronotypes can differ from morning and evening types in their pattern of associations with health, mood, and sleep problems. The difference in such associations can also be expected for those individuals with discrepant trait and state assessments of morningness-eveningness who were reclassified as lethargic, vigilant, afternoon, or napping types by means of this multidimensional chronotyping. Therefore, the associations with health, mood, and sleep problems of the individuals with discordant trait and state assessments of morningness-eveningness can resemble the associations of either morning or evening types or other types of the multidimensional classification of chronotypes.
Here, the associations between chronotype and health, mood, and sleep problems were explored in 1582 university students aged between 17 and 25 years. Like people of younger and older ages, they wake up relatively early on weekdays. On the other hand, they differ from younger and older people in reporting the latest weekend sleep timing (e.g., [
63,
64]). It can be speculated that some lark-students sleep late on weekends in order to remain in sync with their peers who practice late sleep timing. Conversely, some owl-students might sleep earlier on weekends to reduce the extent of displacement of their sleep relative to their advanced circadian sleep phase on weekdays. However, it is difficult to imagine university students who wake up early on weekends. Therefore, it can be expected that their trait and state assessments of morningness-eveningness disagree due to applying the unidimensional approach to chronotyping. These owl-students can belong to types other than evening and morning, and, consequently, they can differ from both morning and evening types in their pattern of associations with health, mood, and sleep problems.
Here, we asked the following questions about chronotypes with discordant trait and state characteristics of morningness-eveningness: 1. How many such chronotypes are there? 2. Can multidimensional chronotyping confirm unidimensional categorization of these chronotypes as either morning or evening types? 3. Can the patterns of associations of these chronotypes with health, mood, and sleep problems resemble the patterns established for chronotypes with concordant trait and state characteristics? 4. Can a model of sleep–wake regulation indicate an additional regulator of sleep times underlying the discrepancy between trait and state characteristics?
The following hypotheses were tested: 1. Chronotypes with discordant trait and state characteristics of morningness-eveningness are rare compared to chronotypes with concordant characteristics. 2. Morning or evening types with late or early sleep timing, respectively, are neither morning nor evening types of the multidimensional classification of chronotype. 3. When such multidimensional classification suggests that they are neither morning nor evening types, their associations with health, mood, and sleep concerns are different from the associations of chronotypes with concordant characteristics of morningness-eveningness. 4. Sleep times in chronotypes with discordant trait and state characteristics of morningness-eveningness can be explained by accounting for an additional parameter of underlying regulating processes in model-based simulations of their sleep–wake cycles.
2. Results
2.1. Division of Subsamples into Three Trait, Three State, and Five Trait+State Types
The mean ages ± standard deviations of 439 male and 1133 female university students were 19.7 ± 2.0 and 19.4 ± 1.5 years, respectively.
Table 1 includes the results of assigning students to chronotypes with concordant and discordant trait and state characteristics of morningness-eveningness. It contains the criteria applied for such an assignment to unidimensional trait types (3), unidimensional state types (3), and, finally, unidimensional trait+state types (5: Mm-, Me-, Neither-, Em-, and Ee-type).
The whole sample was subdivided into three trait subsamples using the sums of scores on scales designed to assess morning and evening/night components of morningness-eveningness (12-item M and E scales of the SWPAQ named “Morning Lateness” and “Evening Lateness”, respectively, and 10-item MS and NW scales of the SWAT named “Morning Sleepability” and “Nighttime Wakeability”, respectively). Each of these four scales consists of an equal number of positively and negatively worded statements (six pairs in the SWPAQ and five pairs in the SWAT). The responses to these statements were assigned as either +1 or −1 to refer to eveningness or morningness, respectively. Because each statement describes the habits or behaviors in a way that highlights eveningness or morningness, respectively, the sums of scores on two morningness-eveningness scales of each questionnaire (M+E and MS+NW) allowed the subdivision of the whole sample into subsamples of morning and evening types (M- and E-types, respectively) characterized by a positive or negative score, respectively. Further subdivisions using the scores from the scales of two questionnaires resulted in three trait subsamples (M-type with both M+E and MS+NW ≤ 0, other type, and E-type with both M+E and MS+NW > 0). Similarly, we subdivided the whole sample according to weekend bed- and risetimes, either not later or later than 12 a.m. and 9 a.m. (approximately one-third or two-thirds of the whole sample, respectively). Applying such criteria to bed- and risetimes provided the possibility of subdividing the whole sample into three state subsamples (m-type with bedtime not later than 12 a.m. and risetime not later than 9 a.m., other type, and e-type with bedtime later than 12 a.m. and risetime later than 9 a.m.). Finally, the combination of the three trait and three state subdivisions based on scores and sleep times, respectively, resulted in the subdivision of the whole sample into five subsamples: (1) Mm-type with M+E and MS+NW ≤ 0, bedtime not later than 12 a.m. and risetime not later than 9 a.m. (concordant morning-type assessments); (2) Me-type with M+E and MS+NW ≤ 0, but bedtime later than 12 a.m. and risetime later than 9 a.m. (discordant morning-type assessments); (3) Neither-type; (4) Em-type with M+E and MS+NW > 0, but with bedtime not later than 12 a.m. and risetime not later than 9 a.m. (discordant evening-type assessments); and (5) Ee-type with M+E and MS+NW > 0, bedtime later than 12 a.m. and risetime later than 9 a.m. (concordant evening-type assessments).
As expected, the number of evening-type students was greater than the number of morning-type students. Also, as expected, the number of concordant morning and evening types was greater than the number of discordant morning and evening types (
Table 1). The discordant trait and state characteristics of morningness-eveningness were reported by only 8.9% of the total sample of 1582 students. Every third of morning trait types reported late weekend sleep times (Me-type), while only every tenth of evening trait types reported early weekend sleep times (Em-type), 5.4% and 3.5% of the whole sample, respectively (
Table 1). While morning-type students frequently reported late weekend sleep timing, evening-type students rarely reported early weekend sleep timing. The difference between male and female students in the division of five subsamples of trait+state types was non-significant (χ
24 = 4.6,
p = 0.334).
2.2. Scale Scores and Sleep Times in Five Trait+State Types
Figure 1 illustrates the differences between five trait+state types (Mm-, Me-, Neither-, Em-, and Ee-types) in scores on morningness-eveningness scales (
Figure 1A) and sleep times (
Figure 1B). As expected, the extent of morningness-eveningness gradually increased in the following direction: Mm-type (extremely negative scores) → Me-type (negative scores) → Neither-type (near-zero scores) → Em-type (positive scores) → Ee-types (extremely positive scores;
Figure 1A). Early weekend sleep timing (bed- and risetime not later than 12 a.m. and 9 a.m., respectively) was reported by Mm- and Em-types, and late weekend sleep timing (bed- and risetime later than 12 a.m. and 9 a.m., respectively) was reported by Me- and Ee-types (
Figure 1B).
Figure 1.
Scores on morningness-eveningness scales and sleep times in five trait+state types. Scores on morningness-eveningness scales and sleep times on weekends (
B) were used for the subdivision of the whole sample into subsamples of unidimensional trait and state types, with the following subdivision into subsamples of trait+state types: Mm-, Me-, Neither-, Em-, and Ee-types. (
A) Scores on scales designed to assess morning and evening/night components of morningness-eveningness (two 12-item scales of SWPAQ, “Morning Lateness” and “Evening Lateness”, and two 10-item scales of SWAT, “Morning Sleepability” and “Nighttime Wakeability”); M+E and MS+NW: Sums of scores on two scales of the SWPAQ and SWAT. (
B) Rise- and bedtimes (scale on the left and right sides, respectively) on weekdays and weekends in five trait+state types. See the results of the statistical comparisons of five trait+state types in
Table 2 and
Table 3.
Figure 1.
Scores on morningness-eveningness scales and sleep times in five trait+state types. Scores on morningness-eveningness scales and sleep times on weekends (
B) were used for the subdivision of the whole sample into subsamples of unidimensional trait and state types, with the following subdivision into subsamples of trait+state types: Mm-, Me-, Neither-, Em-, and Ee-types. (
A) Scores on scales designed to assess morning and evening/night components of morningness-eveningness (two 12-item scales of SWPAQ, “Morning Lateness” and “Evening Lateness”, and two 10-item scales of SWAT, “Morning Sleepability” and “Nighttime Wakeability”); M+E and MS+NW: Sums of scores on two scales of the SWPAQ and SWAT. (
B) Rise- and bedtimes (scale on the left and right sides, respectively) on weekdays and weekends in five trait+state types. See the results of the statistical comparisons of five trait+state types in
Table 2 and
Table 3.
Similarity between the trait+state subdivisions was found for only one of six sleep times shown in
Table 3, time in bed on weekends (the last column on the right). This similarity implies that the difference between the five trait+state types in sleep duration did not contribute to their difference in weekend sleep timing. Since weekday bedtimes in one of five subdivisions (Em-types) were, on average, somewhat earlier than weekend bedtimes in this subdivision (
Figure 1B), it was necessary to include an additional parameter in the model simulating their sleep times (see
Section 2.5).
2.3. Multidimensional Chronotyping of Trait, State, and Trait+State Types
The multidimensional approach to chronotyping provides the possibility to evaluate differences between individuals other than sleep phase, such as differences in afternoon and daily averaged levels of activity (afternoon vs. napping type and lethargic vs. vigilant type, respectively) [
6].
Table 4 and
Table 5 contain the results of the comparison of counts of chronotypes distinguished by applying single-dimensional and multidimensional approaches for chronotyping (columns and lines, respectively). The division of students into either three trait, three state, or five trait+state types is based on distinguishing between only two extreme chronotypes, morning or evening. In contrast, the multidimensional LIVEMAN chronotyping includes several other options, i.e., morning and evening types are only two of seven types (abbreviated as LIVEMAN types: Lethargic, Inconclusive, Vigilant, Evening, Morning, Afternoon, and Napping). The lethargic type has a low level of alertness in the morning, afternoon, and evening; the vigilant type has a high level in the morning, afternoon, and evening; the evening type has a low level in the morning, moderate in the afternoon, and high in the evening; the morning type has a high level in the morning, moderate in the afternoon, and low in the evening; the afternoon type has a low level in the morning, high in the afternoon, and moderate in the evening; the napping type has a high level in the morning, low in the afternoon, and moderate in the evening; and the inconclusive type is different from any of the six mentioned types (see also the notes for
Table 4 and
Supplementary Materials). Therefore, a student assigned to the subsamples of morning types using single-dimensional chronotyping (i.e., morning trait type, morning state type, or morning trait+state type with either concordant or discordant trait and state assessments) can choose another of the morning-type options offered by the LIVEMAN classification of chronotypes. The results of LIVEMAN chronotyping of such morning types suggested that, nevertheless, morning-type students were less likely to choose other morning-type options provided by the LIVEMAN classification (
Table 4 and
Table 5). Therefore, none of the subsamples of morning types was an exception (
Table 5).
Similarly, a student assigned to the subsamples of evening types using single-dimensional chronotyping (i.e., evening trait type, evening state type, or evening trait+state type with either concordant or discordant trait and state assessments) can choose another evening-type option offered by the LIVEMAN classification. The results of LIVEMAN chronotyping of such evening types suggested that they are more likely to choose other evening-type options from the LIVEMAN classification when they are an evening trait type, evening state type, or evening trait+state type with concordant trait and state assessments (
Table 4 and
Table 5). However, one of the subsamples of evening types with discordant trait+state assessments (Em-types) was an exception (
Table 5).
Overall, these results suggested that a student assigned to a subsample of either morning or evening types using single-dimensional classifications (i.e., morning or evening trait type, morning or evening state type, or morning or evening trait+state type) chose the same option (either morning or evening type suggested by the LIVEMAN classification) rather than any of the six other options. The only exceptions were students from the subsample of evening types with early sleep timing (Em-types). They did not choose the evening type option suggested by the LIVEMAN classification as frequently. More frequently chosen options were lethargic and napping types (L- and N-types). Thus, the results of multidimensional classification for these Em-types contrasted with the results of classification for Me-types that were also choosing morning types from the LIVEMAN classification despite their late weekend sleep timing (
Table 5).
2.4. Associations Between Trait, State, and Trait+State Types and Health, Mood, and Sleep Problems
The following predictions about the associations between chronotypes and health, mood, and sleep concerns can be made from the results of the comparison between single- and multidimensional classifications. Since Me-types from the single-dimensional classification of trait+state types more frequently chose morning-type rather than the other six types of LIVEMAN classification, the pattern of their associations with concerns about health, mood, and sleep can show similarity with the pattern shown by any of the morning types, including Mm-types of the single-dimensional classification of trait+state types. In contrast, the pattern of Em-types from trait+state classification cannot resemble the pattern of evening types from LIVEMAN and other classifications. This pattern is expected to be, at least partly, dissimilar to the patterns shown by other evening types, including Ee-types from the single-dimensional classification of trait+state types. Since they are more likely to choose lethargic or napping types (L- and N-types), their pattern can be more similar to the patterns of these L- and N-types of the LIVEMAN classification.
In full agreement with these expectations, the results shown in
Table 6 suggest that the pattern of associations between Me-types and health, mood, and sleep concerns was very similar to the patterns of other morning types, including Mm-types of the trait+state typology. They are more likely than other types to be concerned about their sleep in the middle and end of the night but less likely to be concerned about their sleep in the beginning of the night. They were also not more concerned than other types about daytime sleepiness and depressed mood (
Table 6).
Moreover, in full agreement with the expectations, the pattern of Em-types of trait+state typology differed from the patterns of other evening types (i.e., Ee-types of trait+state typology and evening types of the trait, state, and LIVEMAN typologies). The pattern of Em-types only partially resembled the patterns of other evening types, and this resemblance can be explained by partial similarity between the patterns of evening types and lethargic or napping types. For instance, Em-, Ee-, L-, and N-types suffered from daytime sleepiness and depressed mood more frequently than expected. Em-types were similar to Mm-, Me-, M-, Neither-, L-, and V-types but were dissimilar to evening (trait, state, and Ee-) types of unidimensional classifications in reporting sleep problems in the middle of the night more frequently than expected. Em-, L-, and N-types, but not Ee-types, reported problems with sleep at the beginning of the night less often than expected. Em-types were similar to Ee- and other evening types with a lower frequency of concerns about sleep at the end of the night, but, unlike any of the other evening types, Em-types did not report poorer health compared to morning types. These results support the conclusion that the partial resemblance between the patterns of Em-types and other evening types, including Ee-types of the trait+state typology, can be explained by the partial similarity of the patterns of evening types and two other (lethargic and napping) types of the LIVEMAN classification (
Table 6).
Overall, the results of the comparison of patterns of associations between chronotypes and health, mood, and sleep concerns supported expectations about the patterns of Me- and Em-types of unidimensional trait+state typology (i.e., the two types with discordant trait and state assessments). Me-types were similar to the morning types of any other typology in this pattern, despite their discordant trait- and state-characteristics (i.e., late rather than early weekend sleep timing). In contrast, the pattern of Em-types with discordant trait- and state-characteristics was more similar to the patterns of chronology types other than evening types from the LIVEMAN typology (i.e., the patterns of L- and N-types).
2.5. An Additional Parameter in Simulations of Sleep Times Reported by Trait+State Types
Sleep times reported by five trait+state types were simulated to link the discordance between trait and state characteristics to the parameters of the underlying processes of sleep–wake regulation (
Table 7). The model-based simulations of sleep–wake cycles in daytime learners/workers usually assume that bedtimes are exclusively determined by the individual’s biological clock, while risetimes on weekdays are determined by the social clocks that conflict with the biological clocks. This assumption was supported by a simulation of sleep times in three of five trait+state types: Me-, Neither-, and Em-types (
Table 7).
The results of the Me- and Em-types suggested the necessity of incorporating an additional assumption in the model-based simulations of their sleep–wake cycles to account for a delay in the transition between wakefulness and sleep in these types. It seems that their bedtime cannot be exclusively determined by the biological (body) regulators of their sleep–wake cycle (as shown in
Figure 2A for Mm- and Ee-types). As shown in
Figure 2B, it is necessary to account for the possibility of prolongation of wakefulness beyond the bedtime set by their biological sleep–wake regulating mechanisms. Such self-deprivation of sleep in early night hours appears to reflect an additional influence of “mental (mind) clocks” on bedtimes.
In terms of the model, the previous simulations and the present simulations of sleep–wake cycles of Mm-, Neither-, and Ee-types are based on an assumption that the time of transition between wakefulness and sleep is determined by two internal (homeostatic and circadian) components of the mechanism of sleep–wake regulation. Therefore, bedtimes always coincide with
t2, that is, the time point at which the highest buildup (
Sd) is reached by the process of sleep–wake regulation (
S). The prolongation of wakefulness after this time point is not proposed for any bedtimes (either on weekdays or weekends) in the present simulations of sleep cycles reported by the types with concordant trait and state assessments (Mm- and Ee-types). The last line in
Table 7 and
Figure 2A shows that the model was capable of accurately simulating sleep times in these Mm- and Ee-types. However, the sleep cycles of Me- and Em-types characterized by discordant trait and state assessments cannot be accurately simulated without the additional assumption that wakefulness was prolonged after reaching
Sd. Their simulations required accounting for a kind of partial self-deprivation of either weekend or weekday sleep in either Me- or Em-types, respectively (
Table 7 and
Figure 2B). Thus, the prolongation of wakefulness after reaching the time point of the highest buildup (
Sd) of the process of sleep–wake regulation (
S) was additionally proposed on weekdays in Em-types (
Table 7 and
Figure 2B), while such a prolongation of wakefulness in Me-types was proposed on weekdays (
Table 7 and
Figure 2B).
Overall, given that weekday risetimes are set by social rather than biological (internal) clocks, they occur earlier than
ad lib weekend risetimes set by biological rather than social clocks. Consequently, the model predicts that weekday bedtimes after earlier weekday risetimes cannot occur later than weekend bedtimes after later
ad lib weekend risetimes. If weekday bedtimes occurred later than weekend bedtimes, these bedtimes cannot be predicted and simulated by the model, postulating that these times are exclusively determined by the internal biological mechanisms of sleep–wake regulation. In order to set weekday bedtimes later than weekend bedtimes, an additional assumption was included in the model. Since weekday bedtime cannot occur exactly at
t2 in Em-types (this is the time point at which the highest buildup,
, of the sleep–wake regulating process,
, is reached), a delay in the transition between the wake and sleep phases of the sleep–wake cycle (+h) was proposed. This proposition provided a model-based explanation for the discordant trait and state characteristics in these Em-types. Similarly, such a delay was proposed for Me-types, but on weekends rather than on weekdays. As mentioned in the introduction, students of these types might sleep late on free days because, for instance, they are trying to remain in sync with their peers who practice late sleep timing on these days. To remain active during the early night hours of these days, they can ignore the bedtime signal sent by their biological mechanisms of sleep–wake regulation (
Table 7 and
Figure 2B).
The results of the simulations were in agreement with the empirical results reported in
Table 2,
Table 3, and
Table 6. Since socially determined early wake-ups reduce weekday sleep duration, the profound reduction in the time in bed on weekdays in Em-types can explain their concern about disturbances in their sleep–wake cycle, such as daytime sleepiness (
Table 3). As expected, Em-types resembled L- and N-types of the LIVEMAN typology in this respect (
Table 6).
We also found that the differences between five trait+state types in their simulated circadian phases (
Figure 2 and
Table 7) were in agreement with the differences between these types in morningness-eveningness self-assessments (e.g.,
Figure 1A and
Table 2).
To summarize, the simulation results provided an explanation for the results of the comparison between single- and multidimensional classifications of chronotypes on the pattern of their associations with health, mood, and sleep concerns. Me-types from the single-dimensional trait+state typology most likely belonged to morning types rather than the other six types of the LIVEMAN typology. The simulations confirmed the morningness of these Me-types (an earlier rather than later circadian phase and, therefore, earlier sleep times set by their biological clocks). Consequently, the pattern of associations between these Me-types and health, mood, and sleep concerns was expected to show a similarity with the patterns shown by any other morning types, including Mm-types of the trait+state classification and morning types of the LIVEMAN classification. This expectation was supported by the study results (
Table 6). In contrast, the simulations did not confirm eveningness of Em-types from the single-dimensional trait+state typology. The simulations suggested an intermediate rather than evening circadian phase and, therefore, earlier rather than later sleep times set by their biological clocks. Consequently, this simulation result explained why Em-types did not resemble evening types from the LIVEMAN classification regarding the pattern of their associations with health, mood, and sleep concerns. This pattern only partly resembled the patterns of other evening types and showed much closer similarity to the patterns of lethargic or napping types from the LIVEMAN classification (
Table 6).
2.6. Summary of the Study Results as the Answers to Four Questions
Table 8 summarizes the answers to the questions of the present study about chronotypes with discordant trait and state characteristics of morningness-eveningness. Question #1: How many such chronotypes are there? Given that the correlations between trait and state assessments are positive in sign, chronotypes with discordant trait and state characteristics of morningness-eveningness (Me- and Em-types) were rare compared to chronotypes with concordant characteristics (Mm- and Ee-types), and the lowest rate of evening types with early weekend sleep timing (Em-types) contrasted with the highest rate of evening types with late weekend sleep timing (Ee-types). Question #2: Can multidimensional chronotyping confirm unidimensional categorization of these chronotypes as either morning or evening types? The multidimensional chronotyping did not confirm the unidimensional categorization of Em-types as evening types but did confirm the unidimensional categorization of Me-types as morning types despite their late weekend sleep timing. Question #3: Can the patterns of association between these chronotypes and health, mood, and sleep problems resemble the patterns established for chronotypes with concordant trait and state characteristics? Consequently, the pattern of associations between health, mood, and sleep problems and morning types with late weekend sleep timing (Me-types) resembled the patterns of morning types with concordant trait and state characteristics (including Mm-types), while the pattern of evening types with early weekend sleep timing (Em-types) did not resemble the patterns of evening types with concordant trait and state characteristics (including Ee-types). Question #4: Can a model of sleep–wake regulation indicate an additional regulator of sleep times underlying the discrepancy between trait and state characteristics? The model-based simulations confirmed an earlier circadian phase of morning types with late weekend sleep timing (Me-types), and this late weekend sleep timing was explained by the prolonged buildup phase of the sleep–wake regulation processes after reaching the bedtime set on weekends by these processes. In contrast, the simulations did not confirm a late circadian phase of evening types with early weekend sleep timing (Em-types); these types were more likely to have an intermediate circadian phase resembling that of afternoon and napping types, which allowed them to go to bed and wake up early on weekends.
3. Discussion
The questionnaire tools for trait and state assessments of morningness-eveningness were implemented in numerous studies exploring the associations between this dimension of individual chronobiological variation and health, mood, and sleep problems. The results were in favor of morning rather than evening types. In the present study, we addressed the following questions about morning and evening chronotypes with discordant trait and state characteristics of morningness-eveningness: 1. How many morning and evening types are there among university students? 2. Can the students belong to types other than morning or evening, as suggested by the multidimensional chronotyping? 3. Can these other types resemble morning and evening types with concordant trait and state characteristics regarding their pattern of associations with health, mood, and sleep concerns? 4. Can the conflicting trait and state characteristics be linked to the parameters of the underlying processes of sleep–wake regulation? We found that if trait-like assessments suggested morningness, it was supported by multidimensional chronotyping despite the atypical (late) timing of weekend sleep. In contrast, if trait-like assessments suggested eveningness, multidimensional chronotyping failed to support it. More likely, evening types who reported atypical (early) timing for weekend sleep were lethargic or napping types. Consequently, these evening types with early weekend sleep timing differed from typical evening types in health, mood, and sleep concerns. Their concerns more resembled the concerns of lethargic or napping types than the concerns of evening types. In contrast, morning types with late weekend sleep timing did not differ from other morning types in these concerns. It seems that weekend sleep mistiming did not significantly impact the health, mood, and sleep of these morning types. Simulations of sleep times reported by chronotypes with discordant trait and state characteristics of morningness-eveningness suggested that they can practice a voluntary prolongation of wakefulness to the early night hours, thus ignoring the bedtime signal from their internal biological clocks, either on weekends (morning types with late weekend sleep timing) or on weekdays (evening types with early weekend sleep timing).
This dismissal of biologically determined bedtimes revealed by the present simulations was previously addressed in the literature on the adverse effects of delayed sleep times under the terms “bedtime procrastination” [
65,
66,
67,
68,
69] and “mind-bedtime procrastination” [
70]. This procrastination was determined as “the volitional delay of sleep without external influences” [
71]. Prior to the introduction of these terms, the concept of the conflicting social and biological clocks was proposed [
72]. This concept postulates that weekday risetimes occur earlier than risetimes after
ad lib (e.g., weekend) sleep because social clocks determine early wake-ups in contrast to later weekend risetimes set by biological clocks. If the former risetimes occur due to forced awakening, the latter risetimes occur after
ad lib sleep. As for bedtimes, this concept postulates that they are solely determined by biological clocks, and therefore, people cannot voluntarily advance the endogenously determined weekday bedtimes to fully compensate for the reduction in sleep duration on weekdays [
72]. However, this concept of two conflicting clocks does not take into account the possibility of the contribution of “mind-bedtime procrastination” to bedtimes on weekdays and/or weekends. Although sleep onset cannot be successfully shifted ahead due to the resistance of biological clocks, it can, at least sometimes, lag behind the sleep onset set by these clocks due to “the volitional delay of sleep without external influences”. The results of model-based simulations suggested that bedtimes in at least one of five trait+state subsamples cannot be explained by the model without the incorporation of an additional assumption that sleep can be partially self-deprived due to the conflict between biological clocks and the tendency of “mind-bedtime procrastination” (“mental clocks”). Overall, the simulations indicated that if the wake-ups can be determined by the conflict between social and biological clocks, bedtimes can be, at least sometimes, determined by the conflict between biological (body) and “mental (mind)” clocks that are known under the terms “mind-bedtime procrastination” and “bedtime procrastination”. When the bedtime signal sent by these biological clocks is ignored, the wake phase of the sleep–wake cycle is prolonged beyond the biologically determined bedtime, or, in other words, sleep during early night hours is self-deprived.
In agreement with the concept of three conflicting clocks, our results suggested that bedtimes of evening-type students cannot be easily shifted ahead due to the resistance of their biological clocks. Consequently, these results did not support the speculation that owl-type students can practice early weekend sleep timing, particularly in an attempt to reduce the extent of displacement of their weekday sleep relative to their internal circadian phase. Instead, the multidimensional LIVEMAN chronotyping indicated that the subsample of evening types with early weekend sleep timing mostly included lethargic and napping types rather than evening and morning types. Consequently, the patterns of association between such chronotypes and health, mood, and sleep concerns resembled the patterns of lethargic and napping types. Since our results indicated that the single-dimensional approach to the assessment of trait-like individual variation can lead to misclassification of chronotypes, the implication of multidimensional approaches to chronotyping [
1,
4,
5,
6,
73,
74] can be recommended to explain the conflicting results of trait and state assessments of morningness-eveningness. These study results are in agreement with studies suggesting a wider interpretation of the terms “chronotype” and “circadian type” [
4,
5,
7,
8,
14,
15] in studies of multidimensional individual variation in the domain of chronobiology. The SIC and other tools designed for multidimensional chronotyping can be further implemented in research regarding associations between more than two chronotypes and sleep, mood, and health problems. At least, the first attempts to implement the SIC (e.g., [
75]) indicated the necessity of reconsidering the conventional view of such associations established in previous studies using the unidimensional approach to chronotyping.
We found that in the sample of university students, a third of morning types practiced late weekend sleep timing, which might be explained by the attempts of lark-type students to remain in sync with their peers who prefer late weekend sleep timing. The result suggesting that late weekend sleep timing did not change the morning type pattern of concerns about health, mood, and sleep appears to agree with the result of the comparison of these patterns before and during lockdown, which demonstrated the patterns’ persistence during lockdown [
57] despite the delay in sleep timing on weekdays and weekends [
76]. The previous and present results agree with the recent findings of a phenome-wide association study aimed at exploring the relationship between chronotype and 976 other phenotypes in 27,030 participants from the UK Biobank dataset. Eveningness was found to be associated with spending more time on the computer, TV, and mobile phone; playing computer games; a preference for attending pubs or social clubs; a higher frequency of consuming alcohol; active smoking; taking cannabis; tiredness and lethargy; negative emotions; depressed mood; mood swings; loneliness; and neuroticism [
77]. Therefore, it can be concluded that the associations revealed in such studies seem to be internal rather than external in nature, and, consequently, a simple shift in sleep timing is unlikely to succeed in modifying these associations.
Further research can be conducted to elaborate on the potential brain mechanisms responsible for the ability to overcome the biological need for getting sleep at a certain time of the day. It seems that, unlike the circadian and sleep–wake regulating mechanisms located in the oldest brain regions, the human ability for voluntary sleep deprivation can be linked to high-level processes produced by the cerebral cortex (e.g., decision-making, problem-solving, thinking, motivation, awareness, attention, learning, etc.). Further research can also explore the integration of the concept of three conflicting—social, “mental”, and biological—clocks in a holistic approach to health developed in the theoretical framework of the biopsychosocial model proposed by Engel [
78]. This model offers an improvement/alternative to the traditional biomedical model by accounting not only for biological factors but also for psychological and social factors relevant to subjective well-being and objective physical and mental health [
79]. It emphasizes the importance of the biological, psychological, and social environment for understanding various conditions. In particular, one of the foundational underpinnings of this model is a revised assumption of mind/body dualism that notoriously offers a scientific account for the influence of “immaterial” mental processes on material body processes regulating human behavior [
78,
79].
Several limitations of this study require acknowledgement. The application of a cross-sectional and non-repeated measures design was one such limitation. In addition, we did not evaluate the numerous factors affecting sleep in university students (e.g., home environment conditions, preparation for examinations and performing deadline-sensitive homework, pre-existing health conditions and adverse health behaviors, engagement in physical activity, following a special diet, etc.). The results obtained in the study of university students cannot be generalized across age and level of education. Moreover, sleep times did not inform about the phase positions of the physiological and hormonal markers of the circadian clocks. Similarly, sleep times cannot inform about the bedtime set by the internal biological mechanisms of sleep–wake regulation. However, the lack of such information about individual differences in the circadian phase and the extent of the delay in wakefulness relative to a biologically determined bedtime cannot challenge the conclusions of our study, i.e., a need to account for the conflict of the biological and “mental” clocks to explain the discrepancies between trait and state assessments. Future studies of this conflict can focus on longitudinal evaluation of objectively measured sleep–wake cycles with parallel measurements of individual circadian phase and genetic and epigenetic markers of chronotype, as well as accounting for the contribution of multiple external factors to sleep patterns established in natural settings.
5. Conclusions
In the present study of 1582 university students, multidimensional chronotyping was applied to support the results of single-dimensional chronotyping, and the associations between discordant trait and state assessments of morningness-eveningness and health, mood, and sleep concerns were evaluated. Multidimensional chronotyping confirmed the morningness of morning types with late weekend sleep timing. Consequently, the morning-type pattern of associations with health, mood, and sleep concerns was also confirmed for these morning types with late weekend sleep timing. Such results agree with the previously published results, indicating that late sleep timing cannot modify the pattern of associations between chronotype and health, mood, and sleep problems. Multidimensional chronotyping did not support the eveningness of evening types with early weekend sleep timing. These types mostly belonged to the lethargic and napping types. Consequently, their pattern of associations with health, mood, and sleep problems was less similar to those of evening types with concordant trait and state assessments and more similar to those of lethargic and napping types. Therefore, the implication of multidimensional approaches to the classification of chronotypes can be recommended for clarifying the causes of disagreement between trait and state assessments of morningness-eveningness. The model-based simulations of sleep–wake cycles in daytime learners/workers usually assume that bedtimes, unlike risetimes on weekdays, are exclusively determined by biological clocks, while risetimes on weekdays result from the conflict between biological and social clocks. However, the present results of model-based simulations of the sleep–wake cycles of students from the subsamples with discordant trait and state assessments of morningness-eveningness indicated a more complex conflict between three rather than two kinds of time regulators for sleep. The additional regulator (“mind clocks”) was previously conceptualized under the terms “bedtime procrastination” and “mind-bedtime procrastination”. The simulations suggested that if, in accordance with the concept of two conflicting clocks, social and biological, these clocks mostly determine weekday and weekend risetime, respectively, this concept fails to recognize the complexity of clocks governing bedtimes. Biological clocks cannot exclusively determine bedtimes on weekdays and weekends in chronotypes with discordant trait and state assessments of morningness-eveningness. The simulations of their sleep–wake cycles indicated that these bedtimes were more likely determined by an additional conflict of biological clocks with “mental clocks”, previously described as “mind-bedtime procrastination” or “bedtime procrastination”. The dismissal of the bedtime signal sent by biological clocks was proposed to occur in at least the subsamples with conflicting trait and state assessments of morningness-eveningness. As a result, sleep was self-deprived during early night hours (i.e., the wake phase of the sleep–wake cycle was prolonged beyond the biologically determined bedtime).