Over- and Under-Estimation of Travel Time on Commute Trips: GPS vs. Self-Reporting
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Literature Review
3. Hypotheses
3.1. Temporal Relevance
- Arrival flexibility is a set of four binary variables that represent the subjects’ workplace arrival time constraints. The categories are: had to be there at the work start time; may arrive within 20 min of the work start time; may arrive within 60 min of the work start time; may arrive at any time past the work start time. The first category is the base case (source: surveys). Travel time perception could also be a function of late and early arrival penalties regarding the work start time (soft rather than hard constraints), which future research could disentangle.
- Type of trip is a binary variable indicating whether the trip originates from home (1 = from home to work) or from work (0 = from work to home) (source: GPS and surveys).
3.2. Temporal Uncertainty and Temporal Expectancies
- Expected travel time of the trip is the travel time (minutes) a subject indicates as expected to arrive at their destination. This differs from the reported travel times, as those are based on the subject’s estimate of the actual travel time of the trip (source: surveys).
- Traffic information is a binary variable indicating whether a subject received any type of pre-trip travel information. 1 = received information; 0 = did not receive information (source: surveys).
- Trips on Interstate bridges is a binary variable indicating whether a subject crossed the Mississippi River using any of the Interstate bridges (source: GPS).
3.3. Task Complexity and Absorption and Attentional Deployment
- Relative discontinuity is the sum of changes in street hierarchy (i.e., discontinuity) divided by the trip length. A change in street hierarchy is defined by the change of segment speed (source: GPS).
- Proportion of limited access roads is obtained by diving the trip length of the trip on limited access roads to the total length of the trip (source: GPS).
- Proportion of signalized arterials is obtained by dividing the trip length of the trip on signalized arterials to the total length of the trip (source: GPS). It is unitless.
- Circuity is the ratio of the network distance of a path P to the Euclidean distance of the origin and destination corresponding to the path P. This measure captures the inefficiency in the network from a traveler’s perspective. It is calculated on the actual commute routes of each trip taken by each subject (source: GPS) It is unitless.
3.4. Affective Elements
- Congestion level represents the subjects’ description of their travel experience with regards to the experienced congestion during their trips. The categories are low, medium, and high. The first category is the base case (source: surveys). Future research could provided alternative objective rather than subjective definitions using GPS data vs. free-flow travel time.
- Stress level represents the subjects’ description of their travel experience with regards to their stress level during their trips. The categories are low, medium, and high. The first category is the base case (source: surveys).
- Fear of driving on the I-35W bridge and other bridges in the vicinity identifies the subjects that admitted they avoid bridges (including the I-35W bridge, Washington Ave bridge, and 10th Street bridge), because of the fear of bridge collapse (source: surveys).
3.5. Socio-Economics and Demographics
- Gender is a binary variable; 1 = male; 0 = female (source: surveys).
- Income is a set of three binary variables: low income ([$0, $49,999]), medium income ([$50,000, $99,999]), and high income ([$100,000, ∞+)]). The first category is the base case (2008 U.S. dollars) (source: surveys).
4. Materials and Methods
5. Statistical Models
5.1. Linear Regression Models
- : functional forms for the covariates.
- : the dependent variable defined in Equation (1).
- : the vector of continuous covariates described in the Hypotheses Section.
- : the vector of categorical covariates described in the Hypotheses Section.
- : the vector of parameters to be estimated.
- i.i.d. for all i.
- i.i.d. for all n and i.
5.2. Logistic Regression Model
- : functional forms for the covariates.
- : the dependent variable defined in Equation (4).
- : the vector of continuous covariates described in the Hypotheses Section.
- : the vector of categorical covariates described in the Hypotheses Section.
- : The vector of parameters to be estimated.
- i.i.d. for all i.
5.3. Statistical Hypothesis Testing
5.4. Statistical Estimation
6. Results
7. Discussion and Concluding Remarks
Author Contributions
Funding
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Levinson, D.; Harder, K.; Bloomfield, J.; Carlson, K. Waiting Tolerance: Ramp Delay vs. Freeway Congestion. Transp. Res. Part F Traffic Psychol. Behav. 2006, 9, 1–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Levinson, D.; Harder, K.; Bloomfield, J.; Winiarczyk, K. Weighting Waiting: Evaluating the Perception of In-Vehicle Travel Time Under Moving and Stopped Conditions. Transp. Res. Rec. J. Transp. Res. Board 2004, 1898, 61–68. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Peer, S.; Knockaert, J.; Koster, P.; Verhoef, E.T. Over-reporting vs. overreacting: Commuters’ perceptions of travel times. Transp. Res. Part A Policy Pract. 2014, 69, 476–494. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lagune-Reutler, M.; Guthrie, A.; Fan, Y.; Levinson, D.M. Transit Riders’ Perception of Waiting Time and Stops’ Surrounding Environments; Transportation Research Board: Washington, DC, USA, 2016. [Google Scholar]
- Fan, Y.; Guthrie, A.; Levinson, D. Waiting time perceptions at transit stops and stations: Effects of basic amenities, gender, and security. Transp. Res. Part A Policy Pract. 2016, 88, 251–264. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wu, X.; Levinson, D.; Liu, H. Perception of waiting time at signalized intersections. Transp. Res. Rec. J. Transp. Res. Board 2009, 2135, 52–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zhang, L.; Xie, F.; Levinson, D. Illusion of motion: Variation in value of travel time under different freeway driving conditions. Transp. Res. Rec. J. Transp. Res. Board 2009, 2135, 34–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ribeiro, M.D.; Larrañaga, A.M.; Arellana, J.; Cybis, H.B. Influence of GPS and self-reported data in travel demand models. Procedia -Soc. Behav. Sci. 2014, 162, 467–476. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Varotto, S.F.; Glerum, A.; Stathopoulos, A.; Bierlaire, M.; Longo, G. Mitigating the impact of errors in travel time reporting on mode choice modelling. J. Transp. Geogr. 2017, 62, 236–246. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Varela, J.M.L.; Börjesson, M.; Daly, A. Quantifying errors in travel time and cost by latent variables. Transp. Res. Part B Methodol. 2018, 117, 520–541. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Díaz, F.; Cantillo, V.; Arellana, J.; de Dios Ortúzar, J. Accounting for stochastic variables in discrete choice models. Transp. Res. Part B Methodol. 2015, 78, 222–237. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jackson, W.B.; Jucker, J.V. An empirical study of travel time variability and travel choice behavior. Transp. Sci. 1982, 16, 460–475. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Daly, A.; Ortuzar, J.D. Forecasting and data aggregation: Theory and practice. Traffic Eng. Control 1990, 31, 632–643. [Google Scholar]
- Arellana, J.; Daly, A.; Hess, S.; de Dios Ortúzar, J.; Rizzi, L.I. Development of Surveys for Study of Departure Time Choice: Two-Stage Approach to Efficient Design. Transp. Res. Rec. 2012, 2303, 9–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Cirillo, C.; Maness, M.; Serulle, N.U. Measuring value of travel time and travel time variability in the presence of managed lanes: Results from a pilot stated preference survey on the Capital Beltway. Transp. Lett. 2014, 6, 23–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Yildirimoglu, M.; Kahraman, O. Searching for empirical evidence on traffic equilibrium. PLoS ONE 2018, 13, e0196997. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Tang, W.; Levinson, D.M. Deviation between Actual and Shortest Travel Time Paths for Commuters. J. Transp. Eng. Part A Syst. 2018, 144, 04018042. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zhu, S.; Levinson, D. Do people use the shortest path? An empirical test of Wardrop’s first principle. PLoS ONE 2015, 10, e0134322. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Parthasarathi, P.; Hochmair, H.; Levinson, D. Network structure and spatial separation. Environ. Plan. B Plan. Des. 2012, 39, 137–154. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Parthasarathi, P.; Levinson, D.; Hochmair, H. Network structure and travel time perception. PLoS ONE 2013, 8, e77718. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Carrion, C.; Levinson, D. A Model of Bridge Choice Across the Mississippi River in Minneapolis. In Network Reliability in Practice: Selected Papers from the Fourth International Symposium on Transportation Network Reliability; Levinson, D., Liu, H., Bell, M., Eds.; Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 2012. [Google Scholar]
- Zhu, S. The Roads Taken: Theory and Evidence on Route Choice in the Wake of the I-35W Mississippi River Bridge Collapse and Reconstruction. Ph.D. Thesis, University of Minnesota, Minnesota, MN, USA, 2010. [Google Scholar]
- Zakay, D. On prospective time estimation, time relevance and temporal uncertainty. In Time, Cognition and Action; Macar, F., Poutas, I., Friedman, W., Eds.; Kluwer Academic: Dordrecht, The Netherlands, 1992; pp. 109–119. [Google Scholar]
- Block, R.; Zakay, D. Models of psychological time revisited. In Time and Mind; Helfrich, H., Ed.; Hogrefe and Huber: Boston, MA, USA, 1996; pp. 171–195. [Google Scholar]
- Angrilli, A.; Cherubini, P.; Pavese, A.; Manfredini, S. The influence of affective factors on time perception. Percept. Psychophys. 1997, 59, 972–982. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Langer, J.; Wapner, S.; Werner, H. The effect of danger upon the experience of time. Am. J. Psychol. 1961, 74, 94–97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Thayer, S.; Schiff, W. Eye-contact, facial expression, and the experience of time. J. Soc. Psychol. 1975, 95, 117–124. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Schachter, S.; Singer, J. Cognitive, social, and physiological determinants of emotional state. Psychol. Rev. 1962, 69, 379–399. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Fox, R.; Bradbury, P.; Hampton, I. Time judgment and body temperature. J. Exp. Psychol. 1967, 75, 88–96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tipples, J. Time flies when we read taboo words. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 2010, 17, 563–568. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Thomas, E.; Weaver, W. Cognitive processing and time perception. Percept. Psychophys. 1975, 17, 363–367. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jones, M.; Boltz, M. Dynamic attending and responses to time. Psychol. Rev. 1989, 96, 459–491. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Boltz, M. Time estimation and expectancies. Mem. Cognit. 1993, 21, 853–863. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Glicksohn, J.; Mourad, B.; Pavell, E. Imagination, absorption and subjective time estimation. Imagin. Cognit. Personal. 1991, 11, 167–176. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tellegen, A.; Atkinson, G. Openness to absorbing and self-altering experiences (“absorption”), a trait related to hypnotic susceptibility. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 1974, 83, 268–277. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Madalina, S. Cognitive Mechanisms Involved in the Subjective Time Perception. Ph.D. Thesis, Babes-Bolyai University, Cluj-Napoca, Romania, 2011. [Google Scholar]
- Peer, S.; Koster, P.; Verhoef, E. The perception of travel time variability. In Proceedings of the 4th International Symposium on Tranportation Network Reliability, Minneapolis, MN, USA, 22–23 July 2010. [Google Scholar]
- Carrion, C.; Levinson, D. Uncovering the influence of commuters’ perception on the reliability ratio. Transp. Find. 2019. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Parthasarathi, P. Network Structure and Travel. Ph.D. Thesis, University of Minnesota, Minnesota, MN, USA, 2011. [Google Scholar]
- Parthasarathi, P.; Levinson, D.; Hochmair, H. Network Structure and Travel Time Perception. In Proceedings of the International Association of Travel Behavior Research Conference, Toronto, ON, Canada, 15–19 July 2012. [Google Scholar]
- TBI. 2000 Travel Behavior Inventory Home Interview Survey: Data and Methodology; Metropolitan Council of the Twin Cities Area: St. Paul, MN, USA, 2003. [Google Scholar]
- Zhu, S.; Levinson, D.; Liu, H.; Harder, K. The traffic and behavioral effects of the I-35W Mississippi River Bridge Collapse. Transp. Res. Part A 2010, 44, 771–784. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Xie, F.; Levinson, D. Measuring the Structure of Road Networks. Geogr. Anal. 2007, 39, 336–356. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Baldauf, D.; Burgard, E.; Wittmann, M. Time perception as a workload measure in simulated car driving. Appl. Ergon. 2009, 40, 929–935. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Block, R.A.; Hancock, P.A.; Zakay, D. How cognitive load affects duration judgments: A meta-analytic review. Acta Psychol. 2010, 134, 330–343. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Carrion, C. Travel Time Perception Errors: Causes and Consequences. Ph.D. Thesis, University of Minnesota, Minnesota, MN, USA, 2013. [Google Scholar]
- Census (2006–2008) American Community Survey 3-Year Estimates, Minneapolis-St. Paul-Bloomington, MN-WI Metropolitan Statistical Area. Available online: http://factfinder.census.gov/ (accessed on 25 November 2009).
- Vierordt, K. Der Zeitsinn nach Versuchen; H Laupp.: Tubingen, Germany, 1868. [Google Scholar]
- Cameron, A.C.; Trivedi, P.K. Microeconometrics: Methods and Applications; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2005. [Google Scholar]
- Wooldridge, J. Econometric Analysis of Cross Section and Panel Data, 2nd ed.; MIT Press: London, UK, 2010. [Google Scholar]
- Johnston, J.; DiNardo, J. Econometric Methods; McGraw-Hill: New York, NY, USA, 1997. [Google Scholar]
- Greene, W. Econometric Analysis, 7th ed.; Prentice-Hall: Upper Saddle River, NJ, USA, 2012. [Google Scholar]
- Cramer, J. Econometric Applications of Maximum Likelihood Methods; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 1986. [Google Scholar]
Sample | Dataset | Census | |
---|---|---|---|
Sex | Male | 43.93% | 49.40% |
Female | 56.07% | 50.60% | |
Age | (Mean, Std. Deviation) | (52.38, 9.91) | (34.47, 20.9) |
Education | 11th grade or less | 0.00% | 9.40% |
High School | 18.03% | 49.60% | |
Associate | 22.54% | 7.70% | |
Bachelors’ | 51.56% | 23.20% | |
Graduate or Professional | 7.86% | 10.10% | |
Household Income | $49,999 or less | 15.26% | 5.20% |
$50,000–$74,999 | 23.47 | 23.30% | |
$75,000–$99,999 | 36.53% | 14.60% | |
$100,000–$149,999 | 21.16% | 11.00% | |
$150,000 or more | 3.58% | 5.90% | |
Race | Black/African American | 11.33% | 6.20% |
White or Caucasian | 81.73% | 87.70% | |
Others | 6.93% | 6.10% |
Cobb–Douglas () (Equation (1)) | Logistic () (Equation (4)) | |||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Underestimated | Overestimated | |||||||||
Hyp | Variable | Coef. | (T-Stats) | Sig. | Coef. | (T-Stats) | Sig. | Coef. | (T-Stats) | Sig. |
Temporal Relevance | ||||||||||
U | Arrival flexibility: Within 20 min of start time (1 = In; 0 = Out) | () | 0.059 | (1.07) | 0.096 | (0.29) | ||||
U | Arrival flexibility: Within 60 min of start time (1 = In; 0 = Out) | () | 0.061 | (1.00) | 0.33 | (0.83) | ||||
U | Arrival flexibility: At any time beyond start time (1 = In; 0 = Out) | () | ** | () | () | |||||
O |
Type of trip (1 = from home to work; 0 = from work to home) | () | () | 0.35 | (1.49) | |||||
Temporal Uncertainty and Temporal Expectancies | ||||||||||
U | Expected travel time | () | *** | |||||||
U | Expected travel time | () | *** | () | *** | |||||
O | Traffic information (1 = received; 0 = otherwise) | () | () | * | 0.65 | (1.84) | * | |||
U | Interstate bridge (1 = used; 0 = otherwise) | () | ** | () | () | ** | ||||
Task Complexity and Absorption and Attentional Deployment | ||||||||||
O | Relative discontinuity | 0.33 | (1.18) | |||||||
O | Relative discontinuity | () | ** | () | ** | |||||
U | Proportion of limited access roads | () | ||||||||
U | Proportion of limited access roads | 0.0058 | (0.48) | 0.0018 | (0.10) | |||||
O | Proportion of signalized arterials | 9.06 | (2.56) | ** | ||||||
O | Proportion of signalized arterials | 0.073 | (3.44) | ** | 0.11 | (3.20) | ** | () | * | |
O | Circuity | 4.86 | (3.28) | ** | ||||||
O | Circuity | 0.076 | (0.84) | 0.67 | (5.72) | *** | () | ** | ||
Affective Elements | ||||||||||
O | Medium congestion level (1 = In; 0 = Out) | () | 0.0041 | (0.08) | () | ** | ||||
O | High congestion level (1 = In; 0 = Out) | () | 0.031 | (0.24) | () | ** | ||||
O | Medium stress level (1 = In; 0 = Out) | () | () | () | ||||||
O | High stress level (1 = In; 0 = Out) | () | ** | () | 0.589 | (0.73) | ||||
U |
Fear of bridges (1 = In; 0 = Out) | 0.097 | (1.35) | () | () | |||||
Socioeconomics and Demographics | ||||||||||
Gender [1= Male; 0 = Female] | () | () | () | |||||||
Income [$50,000, $99,999] (1 = In; 0 = Out) | 0.073 | (0.75) | () | () | 0.84 | (1.07) | ||||
Income [$100,000, ∞+) (1 = In; 0 = Out) | 0.11 | (1.02) | () | 1.42 | (1.66) | |||||
Intercept/scale constant | 1.81 | (11.05) | *** | 2.13 | (8.66) | *** | 03.37 | () | * | |
Dispersion parameter () | 0.19 | (30.15) | *** | 0.25 | (22.58) | *** | ||||
Random effect (,) | 0.24 | (8.66) | *** | 0.28 | (7.42) | *** | 1.76 | (6.07) | *** | |
Intercept log likelihood | ||||||||||
Final log likelihood | 49.254 | |||||||||
Likelihood ratio index | 0.23807827 | |||||||||
Akaike Information Criterion (AIC) | 179.7286 | 927.2543 | ||||||||
Bayesian Information Criterion (BIC) | 129.074 | 263.7704 | 1036.797 | |||||||
Number of observations | 528 | 337 | 865 | |||||||
Number of subjects | 60 | 60 | 64 |
© 2019 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Carrion, C.; Levinson, D. Over- and Under-Estimation of Travel Time on Commute Trips: GPS vs. Self-Reporting. Urban Sci. 2019, 3, 70. https://doi.org/10.3390/urbansci3030070
Carrion C, Levinson D. Over- and Under-Estimation of Travel Time on Commute Trips: GPS vs. Self-Reporting. Urban Science. 2019; 3(3):70. https://doi.org/10.3390/urbansci3030070
Chicago/Turabian StyleCarrion, Carlos, and David Levinson. 2019. "Over- and Under-Estimation of Travel Time on Commute Trips: GPS vs. Self-Reporting" Urban Science 3, no. 3: 70. https://doi.org/10.3390/urbansci3030070
APA StyleCarrion, C., & Levinson, D. (2019). Over- and Under-Estimation of Travel Time on Commute Trips: GPS vs. Self-Reporting. Urban Science, 3(3), 70. https://doi.org/10.3390/urbansci3030070