Sharing Sensory Knowledge: Edwidge Danticat’s Breath, Eyes, Memory
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Scientific and Literary Studies of Mental Imagery Evoked by Language
3. Craft Knowledge of How Fiction Engages Readers’ Senses
Fiction writing requires analyzing the structure of one’s own sensory imagination. O’Connor reports that a friend “learned from Flaubert that it takes at least three activated sensuous strokes to make an object real” (O’Connor 1969, p. 69). While readers vary in what activates their imaginations, the three-strokes idea is worth testing. Writers’ craft advice offers a rich source for experimental hypotheses. As writers apply each other’s craft guidance, the fiction they create may be a more valuable source still.The nature of fiction is in large measure determined by the nature of our perceptive apparatus. The beginning of human knowledge is through the senses, and the fiction writer begins where human perception begins. He appeals through the senses, and you cannot appeal to the senses with abstractions.
4. Evoking a World Through an Imagining Mind in Breath, Eyes, Memory
As Sophie’s body responds to this new space, Danticat emphasizes somatosensory sensations that do not figure in traditional sensory hierarchies—proprioception (awareness of bodily position and movement), interoception (deep bodily sensations, such as the need to urinate), and temperature contrasts (warm floor vs. cool water). Two touch sensations suggest changes in Sophie’s life—she is unused to the feel of her mother’s hand and linoleum under her bare feet. Martine’s Brooklyn apartment emerges as Sophie’s body senses its joylessness.The sun stung my eyes as it came through the curtains. I slid my hand out of [my mother’s] to go to the bathroom. The grey linoleum felt surprisingly warm under my feet. I looked at my red eyes in the mirror while splashing cold water over my face.
In this blast of smells, sounds, movements, and sights, somatosensory modalities strike a keynote amplified by pain. The sun slaps Sophie, and music rakes her body. She imagines painted animals looking for relief, just as she is. Everything Sophie perceives has been influenced by the van driver’s sexual harassment of her throughout the ride. As in Martine’s apartment, Sophie’s sensations and emotions emerge together. One can hardly imagine one dimension of Sophie’s “feelings” without imagining them all.The sun, which was once god to my ancestors, slapped my face as though I had done something wrong. The fragrance of crushed mint leaves and stagnant pee alternated in the breeze. Body-raking konpa blared from the car radio as passengers hopped off the colorful van in which I had spent the last four hours. The sides of the van were painted in steaming reds, from cherry scarlet to crimson blood. Giraffes and lions were sketched over a terra cotta landscape, as though seeking a tint of green.(p. 91)
Danticat conveys Sophie’s close bond to Martine by showing how Sophie’s imagination works. The references to color, sound, and movement let readers perceive the scene through a distant camera, but “pounding”, “stunned”, and “face in the dirt” suggest that Sophie’s imagining mind is in her mother’s body—much as Martine’s mind is in Sophie’s.He had a black bandanna over his face so she never saw anything but his hair, which was the color of eggplants. He kept pounding her until she was too stunned to make a sound. When he was done, he made her keep her face in the dirt, threatening to shoot her if she looked up.(p. 138)
Danticat describes a series of actions, each of which may evoke images—the sound of flesh tearing, the hardness of the pestle, the brightness of blood on a pale sheet. Sophie discards the tool that ripped her “veil”, but her mind returns to her mother’s finger. Through the pain Danticat works to convey, readers may sense that trauma persists in Sophie’s body.My flesh ripped apart as I pressed the pestle into it. I could see the blood slowly dripping onto the bed sheet. I took the pestle and the bloody sheet and stuffed them into a bag. It was gone, the veil that always held my mother’s finger back every time she tested me.(p. 87)
Martine’s rape occurred in a cane field,27 and Danticat uses the same verb, “pound”, to describe her violation and Sophie’s fight. Sophie’s brutal struggle with a living foe (which strikes back) appeals to readers’ imaginations in ways that no abstract description could. Danticat invites readers to hear the cracks, watch the stalk lean, and smell the dirt on its exposed roots. She has crafted the passage so that any sights, sounds, or smells readers imagine will likely blend with impressions of Sophie’s movements, pain, and pounding heart. “Ou libere?” asks Sophie’s grandmother. “Are you free?” (p. 238). Artistically, Breath, Eyes, Memory asks readers to feel the answer.I took off my shoes and began to beat a cane stalk. I pounded it until it began to lean over. I pushed over the cane stalk. It snapped back, striking my shoulder. I pulled at it, yanking it from the ground. My palm was bleeding.(p. 238)
Crafted to set sensations dancing from one’s surface to one’s core, this creative passage suggests how love feels to Sophie. As in Danticat’s depictions of Sophie’s worst moments, her sensations emerge from descriptions of actions. Sophie’s joy comes not just from her own pleasure but from her sense of what Brigitte may be feeling.When I was pregnant, Joseph would play his saxophone for us, alone in the dark. He would put the horn very close to my stomach and blow in a soft whisper. Brigitte would come alive inside me, tickling like a feather under my skin. Joseph would press his ear against my stomach to hear her every move.(p. 108)
Sophie’s description of these night horrors starts with vision but comes alive through movement, sound, and somatosensory modalities. Sophie does not just watch; she intervenes, so that the wetness, thrashing, and biting come to be grounded in her own bodily experiences. In many senses, Martine’s nightmares are also Sophie’s (Rossi 2005–2006, p. 210).I had seen her curled up in a ball in the middle of the night, sweating and shaking as she hollered for the images of the past to leave her alone. Sometimes the fright woke her up, but most of the time, I had to shake her awake before she bit her finger off, ripped her nightgown, or threw herself out of a window.(pp. 195–96)
5. The Value of Craft Knowledge for Scientific Research
6. Conclusions
Funding
Data Availability Statement
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
1 | Interdisciplinary scholar Ziqi Jin argues that valuable insights can be gained by joining creative writers’ craft knowledge with the knowledge built by rhetorical narratologists and psychologists of aesthetics (Jin 2024, pp. 19, 60–61; Jin, forthcoming). |
2 | I thank the editor who pointed out that, in readers who have never experienced trauma as Sophie has, imagination may create inter-individual experiences in which readers’ autobiographical memories merge with the events Danticat describes. Such inter-individual experiences could enable transformative learning. |
3 | Psychologists Joseph Henrich, Steven J. Heine, and Ara Norenzayan present evidence that behavioral researchers have made unwarranted “broad claims about human psychology and behavior” based on experiments conducted with Western, educated, industrialized, rich, democratic (“WEIRD”) participants such as healthy young college undergraduates (Henrich et al. 2010, p. 61). |
4 | Cognitive literary scholar Marco Bernini proposes that models of cognition created by innovative writers such as Samuel Beckett can serve scientists by offering “scaffolding tools” to investigate mental experiences (Bernini 2021, p. vii). |
5 | Neuroscientist Nina Kraus reports that in the cochlea of the human auditory system, inner hair cells transduce wave patterns into electrical signals, while outer hair cells respond to feedback from higher processing centers, which are in communication with other sensory systems. As the inner hair cells respond to the environment, the outer hair cells are “hearing the brain” (Kraus 2021, p. 66). |
6 | I use the term “modal” rather than “sensory” in order to include the motor system, which interacts so closely with the sensory modalities. |
7 | In the imagery debates of the 1970s–1990s, Stephen M. Kosslyn and his colleagues contended that brains process visual mental images much as they do visual perceptions, whereas Zenon Pylyshyn and his collaborators argued that brains encode sensory information as language-like propositions (Kosslyn et al. 2006; Pylyshyn 2003). These debates could not be resolved with the behavioral experiments Kosslyn and Pylyshyn’s groups were conducting, but since the mid-1990s, neuroimaging evidence has favored Kosslyn’s position. Emily T. Troscianko points out that both Kosslyn’s and Pylyshyn’s models involve abstract, “propositional representations”, and that an enactivist view of perception based on environmental interactions better explains how mental images work (Troscianko 2013, pp. 183–85). |
8 | Two further experiments in Zwaan and Taylor’s 2006 study, which investigated the time course of comprehension, required participants to read rather than listen to sentences. Zwaan and his colleagues are well aware of the different cognitive demands of comprehending spoken and written language (Zwaan and Taylor 2006, pp. 4–5). |
9 | Troscianko calls literary “instructions to imagine” more evocative of “cognitive realism” than scientific prompts in mental imagery studies because the literary cues invite readers to explore an environment, not just envision it (Troscianko 2013, pp. 195, 182). |
10 | Cognitive literary scholar G. Gabrielle Starr, who analyzes multimodal imagery, observes that “when data from one sense is presented, the entire multisensory image is triggered. The connections that multisensory imagery can form are rapid and strong” (Starr 2010, p. 289). |
11 | Drawing on phenomenology as well as Damasio’s “as-if” hypothesis, cognitive literary scholar Paul Armstrong argues that “a simulation is an as-relation, both like and not like what it recreates” (Armstrong 2020, p. 7). See also pp. 118–21. |
12 | Seeking to honor and learn from neurodivergent readers’ experiences, Matthew Rubery argues that “there is no standard way of reading; there are ways of reading” (Rubery 2022, p. 6). |
13 | Lawrence Barsalou predicts that “large individual differences in grounded cognition should be the rule, not the exception” (Barsalou 2020, p. 11). Charles Spence and Ophelia Deroy remind readers that “profound individual differences in the vividness of visual mental imagery have been a core feature of the phenomenon ever since researchers first started to investigate it” (Spence and Deroy 2013, p. 162). |
14 | Emily Troscianko has recommended experiments that prompt mental imagery based on an understanding of imagination as interactive rather than pictorial (Troscianko 2013, p. 195). Marco Bernini has founded a Narrative and Cognition Lab in which scientists and literary scholars exchange ideas about potential experiments. |
15 | Auyoung observes that “the motor actions that Tolstoy specifies are designed to activate a disproportionate amount of the reader’s sensorimotor knowledge in a seemingly effortless way” (Auyoung 2018, p. 31). |
16 | My discussion of craft knowledge relies on learning opportunities in the Warren Wilson Master of Fine Arts Program, where I earned an MFA degree in fiction in 2015–2017. |
17 | Fiction writer Brian Evenson has discussed how incorporating aspects of narrative theory could enrich creative writing instruction (Evenson 2010). |
18 | Jim Grimsley, personal communication. |
19 | The book project, Cognitive Craft, on which this essay draws has its roots in my MFA thesis of 2017. I thank the advisor who opened my mind by reporting an unnamed colleague’s response to my thesis proposal, which had mentioned analyzing passages that evoked unisensory imagery. |
20 | Ifeona Fulani argues that in Breath, Eyes, Memory, sympathy is “the strategy—by which Danticat crosses cultural barriers to gain access to the imagination of the non-Haitian, non-Caribbean reader” (Fulani 2005, p. 77). |
21 | Anežka Kuzmičová has proposed that “a particularly effective way” to evoke blended, multimodal imagery in readers is to make a “dynamizing reference to bodily movement or touch directly after the object or immediate environment has been described in visual or other exteroceptive terms” (Kuzmičová 2012, p. 40). |
22 | Jana Evans Braziel argues that daffodils, which appear throughout Breath, Eyes, Memory, suggest the “pressing” and “squashing” forces of colonialism, since colonists brought daffodils to Haiti (Braziel 2003, p. 119). Braziel compares Danticat’s daffodil references to those of Jamaica Kincaid in her novel Lucy (1990), whose young protagonist must memorize Wordsworth’s poem about daffodils (Braziel 2003). |
23 | Bessel Van der Kolk characterizes trauma as an interaction between body and mind in which “the body keeps the score” (Van der Kolk 2014). Among the literary scholars who have analyzed Breath, Eyes, Memory as a trauma narrative are Donette A. Francis (2004), Jennifer C. Rossi (2005–2006), Sharrón Eve Sarthou (2010), and Kaiama L. Glover (2015, p. 77). Glover finds that Danticat “insist[s] on the impact of trauma on the bodies and the minds of her characters”. |
24 | Heather Hewett argues that Breath, Eyes, Memory “makes a powerful connection between testing and rape” (Hewett 2009, p. 133). Sharrón Eve Sarthou calls testing “a form of maternal rape” (Sarthou 2010, p. 107). |
25 | People who cannot escape physical abuse often “double” to survive trauma. Judith Herman proposes that “the helpless person escapes from her situation not by action in the real world but rather by altering her state of consciousness” (Herman [1992] 1997, p. 42). Gwen Bergner points out that in Haiti, “doubling” has a cultural basis in the Vodou notion of the divided self (Bergner 2017, p. 527). |
26 | Stephanie Couey argues that the Western medical label “bulimia” does not convey the full meaning of Sophie’s purging, which shows her bodily response to centuries of slavery, colonial violence, and U. S. neocolonial food practices (Couey 2023, pp. 163–64). |
27 | Literary scholars point out that besides recalling Martine’s rape, the Haitian sugarcane fields bring to mind colonial enslavement and Sophie’s grandfather’s death from overwork (Bergner 2017, p. 539; Thompson 2017, p. 74). |
28 | Sophie fills her narrative with stories told by her mother, grandmother, and aunt, so that Breath, Eyes, Memory “incorporates the multiple voices of other women” (Hewett 2009, p. 129). Sophie cannot tell her own story without telling her mother’s, since they are “inextricably connected as captives in the web of patriarchal power” (Johnson 2005–2006, p. 157). When adult Sophie returns to Haiti, her narrative shifts from internal to external as she confronts her relatives, trying to learn their perspectives (Rossi 2005–2006, p. 210). Through Sophie’s collective storytelling, all the women in her family become “agents of their discourse” rather than “agents of pain” (James Alexander 2011, p. 387). |
29 | If traumatic events “shatter the sense of connection between individual and community”, Breath, Eyes, Memory depicts a woman becoming herself by restoring these bonds (Herman [1992] 1997, p. 55). Danticat represents Sophie’s trauma not as an “individual experience” but a “collective plight shared by postcolonial women” (Francis 2004, p. 85). |
30 | Tim Bayne and Charles Spence assert that “any explanatory model of multisensory integration will need to extend across multiple levels of analysis and draw on the resources of a number of disciplines” (Bayne and Spence 2015, p. 609). |
31 | G. Gabrielle Starr reflects that “shaping or reshaping what we see, hear, taste or touch is one of the central acts artists may undertake” (Starr 2024, p. 114). |
32 | Frederik Schröer and Laura Otis have argued that non-Western understandings of human emotions as relational, emerging through interactions with people and environments, resonate with an understanding of cognition as embodied, embedded, enacted, and extended (Schröer and Otis 2023, p. 138). |
References
- Armstrong, Paul B. 2020. Stories and the Brain: The Neuroscience of Narrative. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Auyoung, Elaine. 2018. When Fiction Feels Real: Representation and the Reading Mind. New York: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Barsalou, Lawrence W. 2008. Grounded Cognition. The Annual Review of Psychology 59: 617–45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Barsalou, Lawrence W. 2020. Challenges and Opportunities for Grounding Cognition. Journal of Cognition 3: 1–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Baxter, Charles. 2008. Burning Down the House: Essays on Fiction. Minneapolis: Graywolf Press. [Google Scholar]
- Bayne, Tim, and Charles Spence. 2015. Multisensory Perception. In The Oxford Handbook of Philosophy of Perception. Edited by Mohan Matthen. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 603–20. [Google Scholar]
- Bergner, Gwen. 2017. Danticat’s Vodou Vernacular of Women’s Human Rights. American Literary History 29: 521–45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bernini, Marco. 2021. Beckett and the Cognitive Method: Mind, Models, and Exploratory Narratives. Cognition and Poetics Series. New York: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Berry, Wendell. 1977. The Unsettling of America. San Francisco: Sierra Club Books. [Google Scholar]
- Braziel, Jana Evans. 2003. Daffodils, Rhizomes, Migrations: Narrative Coming of Age in the Diasporic Writings of Edwidge Danticat and Jamaica Kincaid. Meridians 3: 110–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Brosch, Renate. 2017. Experiencing Narratives: Default and Vivid Modes of Visualization. Poetics Today 38: 255–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Burke, Michael, and Emily T. Troscianko. 2013. Mind, Brain, and Literature: A Dialogue on What the Humanities Might Offer the Cognitive Sciences. Journal of Literary Semantics 42: 141–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Caracciolo, Marco. 2013. Embodiment at the Crossroads: Some Open Questions between Literary Interpretation and Cognitive Science. Poetics Today 34: 233–53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Caracciolo, Marco. 2017. The Experientiality of Narrative: An Enactivist Approach. Berlin: De Gruyter. [Google Scholar]
- Caracciolo, Marco, and Karin Kukkonen. 2021. With Bodies: Narrative Theory and Embodied Cognition. Columbus: Ohio State University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Cave, Terrence. 2017. Situated Cognition: The Literary Archive. Poetics Today 38: 235–53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Couey, Stephanie. 2023. Bulimia as Postcolonial Trauma in Edwidge Danticat’s Breath, Eyes, Memory. MELUS 48: 163–85. [Google Scholar]
- Damasio, Antonio. 1999. The Feeling of What Happens: Body and Emotion in the Making of Consciousness. New York: Harcourt. [Google Scholar]
- Danticat, Edwidge. 2015. Breath, Eyes, Memory. New York: Soho Press. First published 1994. [Google Scholar]
- Dutriaux, Léo, Naomi E. Clark, Esther K. Papies, Christoph Scheepers, and Lawrence W. Barsalou. 2023. The Situated Assessment Method (SAM2): Establishing Individual Differences in Habitual Behavior. PLoS ONE 18: 1–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Esrock, Ellen J. 2004. Embodying Literature. Journal of Consciousness Studies 11: 79–89. [Google Scholar]
- Esrock, Ellen J. 2018. Einfühlung as the Breath of Art: Six Modes of Embodiment. Cognitive Processing 19: 187–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Esrock, Ellen J. 2019. Body Forth in Narrative. In Narrative Complexity: Cognition, Embodiment, Evolution. Edited by Marina Grishakova and Maria Poulaki. Lincoln, NE: University of Nebraska Press, pp. 270–90. [Google Scholar]
- Evenson, Brian. 2010. Across the Curriculum: Creative Writing. In Teaching Narrative Theory. Edited by David Herman, Brian McHale and James Phelan. New York: Modern Language Association, pp. 70–78. [Google Scholar]
- Fitch, Janet. 2020. Writing from the Senses. Paper presented at Community of Writers Course, Virtual, October 9–11. [Google Scholar]
- Francis, Donette A. 2004. “Silences too Horrific to Disturb”: Writing Sexual Histories in Edwidge Danticat’s Breath, Eyes, Memory. Research in African Literatures 35: 75–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fulani, Ifeona. 2005. Caribbean Women Writers and the Politics of Style: A Case for Literary Anancysm. Small Axe 9: 64–79. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Glover, Kaiama L. 2015. “You Have Said Things You Should not Have Said”: Trauma, Healing, and Treacherous Self-Telling in Stories of Haitian Women. Francosphères 4: 71–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hauk, Olaf, Ingrid Johnsrude, and Friedemann Pulvermüller. 2004. Somatotopic Representation of Action Words in Human Motor and Premotor Cortex. Neuron 41: 301–7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Henrich, Joseph, Steven J. Heine, and Ara Norenzayan. 2010. The WEIRDest People in the World? Behavioral and Brain Sciences 33: 61–135. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Herman, Judith. 1997. Trauma and Recovery: The Aftermath of Violence. New York: Basic Books. First published 1992. [Google Scholar]
- Hewett, Heather. 2009. Mothering across Borders: Narratives of Immigrant Mothers in the United States. Women’s Studies Quarterly 37: 121–39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hillman, David, and Ulrika Maude. 2015. Introduction. In The Cambridge Companion to the Body in Literature. Edited by David Hillman and Ulrika Maude. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 1–9. [Google Scholar]
- James Alexander, Simone A. 2011. M/othering the Nation: Women’s Bodies as Nationalist Trope in Edwidge Danticat’s Breath, Eyes, Memory. African American Review 44: 373–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jin, Ziqi. 2024. Crafting a Shared Universe: An Interdisciplinary Poetics of Narrative. Ph.D. dissertation, University of Washington, Seattle, WA, USA. [Google Scholar]
- Jin, Ziqi. Forthcoming. What Craft Advice, Rhetorical Narratology, and Psychology of Aesthetics Can Do for Each Other. Narrative.
- Johnson, Newtona. 2005–2006. Challenging Internal Colonialism: Edwidge Danticat’s Feminist Emancipatory Enterprise. Obsidian III 6–7: 147–66. [Google Scholar]
- Kern-Stähler, Annette, and Elizabeth Robertson. 2023. Literature and the Senses: An Introduction. In Literature and the Senses. Edited by Annette Kern-Stähler and Elizabeth Robertson. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 1–27. [Google Scholar]
- Kosslyn, Stephen M., William L. Thompson, and Giorgio Ganis. 2006. The Case for Mental Imagery. New York: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Kraus, Nina. 2021. Of Sound Mind: How Our Brain Constructs a Meaningful Sonic World. Cambridge: MIT Press. [Google Scholar]
- Kuzmičová, Anežka. 2012. Presence in the Reading of Literary Narrative: A Case for Motor Enactment. Semiotica 189: 23–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kuzmičová, Anežka. 2014. Literary Narrative and Mental Imagery: A View from Embodied Cognition. Style 48: 275–93. [Google Scholar]
- Lacey, Simon, and Krishnankutty Sathian. 2020. Visuo-Haptic Object Perception. In Multisensory Perception: From Laboratory to Clinic. Edited by Krishnankutty Sathian and Vilayanur S. Ramachandran. Cambridge: Academic Press, Elsevier, pp. 157–78. [Google Scholar]
- Lacey, Simon, Randall Stilla, and Krishnankutty Sathian. 2012. Metaphorically Feeling: Comprehending Textural Metaphors Activates Somatosensory Cortex. Brain and Language 120: 416–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lacey, Simon, Randall Stilla, Gopikrishna Deshpande, Sinan Zhao, Careese Stephens, Kelly McCormick, David Kemmerer, and Krishnankutty Sathian. 2017. Engagement of the Left Extrastriate Body Area During Body-Part Metaphor Comprehension. Brain and Language 166: 1–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Lawson, Rebecca, and Simon Lacey. 2013. Summary and Some Future Directions. In Multisensory Imagery. Edited by Simon Lacey and Rebecca Lawson. Berlin and Heidelberg: Springer, pp. 421–28. [Google Scholar]
- Lobnik, Mirja. 2016. Sounding Ecologies in Arundhati Roy’s The God of Small Things. Modern Fiction Studies 62: 115–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lobnik, Mirja. 2023. Multisensory Entanglements in Edwidge Danticat’s Claire of the Sea Light and Chimamanda Ngozi Adichie’s Purple Hibiscus. In Literature and the Senses. Edited by Annette Kern-Stähler and Elizabeth Robertson. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 478–92. [Google Scholar]
- Loichot, Valérie. 2013. The Tropics Bite Back: Culinary Coups in Caribbean Literature. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press. [Google Scholar]
- Miall, David S. 2006. Literary Reading: Empirical and Theoretical Studies. New York: Peter Lang. [Google Scholar]
- Miall, David S. 2015. Temporal Aspects of Literary Reading. In Investigations into the Phenomenology and the Ontology of the Work of Art: What Are Artworks and How Do We Experience Them? Edited by Peer F. Bundgaard and Frederik Stjernfelt. Berlin and Heidelberg: Springer, pp. 15–30. [Google Scholar]
- Newen, Albert, Shaun Gallagher, and Leon De Bruin. 2018. 4E Cognition: Historical Roots, Key Concepts, and Central Issues. In The Oxford Handbook of 4E Cognition. Edited by Albert Newen, Shaun Gallagher and Leon De Bruin. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 3–16. [Google Scholar]
- O’Connor, Flannery. 1969. The Nature and Aim of Fiction. In Mystery and Manners. Edited by Sally and Robert Fitzgerald. New York: Farrar, Straus, Giroux. [Google Scholar]
- Otis, Laura. 2015. Rethinking Thought: Inside the Minds of Creative Scientists and Artists. New York: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Pallasmaa, Juhani. 2012. The Eyes of the Skin: Architecture and the Senses, 3rd ed. Hoboken: John Wiley & Sons. [Google Scholar]
- Pulvermüller, Friedemann. 2005. Brain Mechanisms Linking Language and Action. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 6: 576–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pylyshyn, Zenon. 2003. Seeing and Visualizing: It’s Not What You Think. Cambridge: MIT Press. [Google Scholar]
- Ramachandran, Vilayanur S., Zeve Marcus, and Chaipat Chunharas. 2020. Bouba-Kiki: Cross-Domain Resonance and the Origins of Synesthesia, Metaphor, and Words in the Human Mind. In Multisensory Perception: From Laboratory to Clinic. Edited by Krishnankutty Sathian and Vilayanur S. Ramachandran. Cambridge: Academic Press, Cambridge: Elsevier, pp. 1–40. [Google Scholar]
- Rokotnitz, Naomi. 2017. Goosebumps, Shivers, Visualization, and Embodied Resonance in the Reading Experience: The God of Small Things. Poetics Today 38: 273–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rossi, Jennifer C. 2005–2006. “Let the Words Bring Wings to Our Feet”: Negotiating Exile and Trauma through Narrative in Danticat’s Breath, Eyes, Memory. Obsidian III 6–7: 203–20. [Google Scholar]
- Rubery, Matthew. 2022. Reader’s Block: A History of Reading Differences. Stanford: Stanford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Sarthou, Sharrón Eve. 2010. Unsilencing Défilés Daughters: Overcoming Silence in Edwidge Danticat’s Breath, Eyes, Memory and Krik? Krak! The Global South 4: 99–123. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sathian, Krishnankutty. 2012. Cross-Modal Recruitment of Sensory Cortical Regions. In The New Handbook of Multisensory Processing. Edited by Barry E. Stein. Cambridge: MIT Press, pp. 729–35. [Google Scholar]
- Sathian, Krishnankutty, and Simon Lacey. 2007. Journeying beyond Classical Somatosensory Cortex. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology 61: 254–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Scarry, Elaine. 1985. The Body in Pain: The Making and Unmaking of the World. New York: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Scarry, Elaine. 1999. Dreaming by the Book. New York: Farrar, Straus, Giroux. [Google Scholar]
- Schröer, Frederik, and Laura Otis. 2023. Feeling Environments: Emotions beyond Human Interiority. Geschichte und Gesellschaft 49: 138–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Seilman, Uffe, and Steen F. Larsen. 1989. Personal Resonance to Literature: A Study of Remindings While Reading. Poetics 18: 165–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sennett, Richard. 2008. The Craftsman. New Haven: Yale University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Shepard, Roger N. 1978. The Mental Image. American Psychologist 33: 125–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Spence, Charles, and Ophelia Deroy. 2013. Crossmodal Mental Imagery. In Multisensory Imagery. Edited by Simon Lacey and Rebecca Lawson. Berlin and Heidelberg: Springer, pp. 157–83. [Google Scholar]
- Starr, G. Gabrielle. 2010. Multisensory Imagery. In Introduction to Cognitive Cultural Studies. Edited by Lisa Zunshine. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press, pp. 275–91. [Google Scholar]
- Starr, G. Gabrielle. 2013. Feeling Beauty: The Neuroscience of Aesthetic Experience. Cambridge: MIT Press. [Google Scholar]
- Starr, G. Gabrielle. 2014. Theorizing Imagery, Aesthetics, and Doubly Directed States. In The Oxford Handbook of Cognitive Literary Studies. Edited by Lisa Zunshine. New York: Oxford University Press, pp. 246–68. [Google Scholar]
- Starr, G. Gabrielle. 2024. “Go and Catch a Falling Star”: Embodiment, Cognition, and Imagery. In The Routledge Companion to Literature and Art. Edited by Neil Murphy, W. Michelle Wang and Cheryl Julia Lee. London and New York: Routledge, pp. 107–21. [Google Scholar]
- Stein, Barry E., Terrence R. Stanford, and Benjamin A. Rowland. 2020. Multisensory Integration and the Society for Neuroscience: Then and Now. The Journal of Neuroscience 40: 3–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Thompson, Zoë Brigley. 2017. Happiness (or not) after Rape: Hysterics and Harpies in the Media versus Killjoys in Black Women’s Fiction. Journal of Gender Studies 26: 66–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Troscianko, Emily T. 2013. Reading Imaginatively: The Imagination in Cognitive Science and Cognitive Literary Studies. Journal of Literary Semantics 42: 181–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Troscianko, Emily T. 2014. First-Person and Second-Generation Perspectives on Starvation in Kafka’s “Ein Hungerkünstler”. Style 48: 331–48. [Google Scholar]
- Van der Kolk, Bessel. 2014. The Body Keeps the Score: Brain, Mind, and Body in the Healing of Trauma. New York: Penguin. [Google Scholar]
- Vessel, Edward A., Gabrielle G. Starr, and Nava Rubin. 2012. The Brain on Art: Intense Aesthetic Experience Activates the Default Mode Network. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 6: 1–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Willems, Roel M. 2013. Can Literary Studies Contribute to Cognitive Neuroscience? Journal of Literary Semantics 42: 217–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wolf, Maryanne. 2007. Proust and the Squid: The Story and Science of the Reading Brain. New York: Harper Collins. [Google Scholar]
- Zwaan, Rolf A. 2014. Embodiment and Language Comprehension: Reframing the Discussion. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 18: 229–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zwaan, Rolf A., and Lawrence J. Taylor. 2006. Seeing, Acting, Understanding: Motor Resonance in Language Comprehension. Journal of Experimental Psychology 135: 1–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2025 by the author. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Otis, L.C. Sharing Sensory Knowledge: Edwidge Danticat’s Breath, Eyes, Memory. Literature 2025, 5, 10. https://doi.org/10.3390/literature5020010
Otis LC. Sharing Sensory Knowledge: Edwidge Danticat’s Breath, Eyes, Memory. Literature. 2025; 5(2):10. https://doi.org/10.3390/literature5020010
Chicago/Turabian StyleOtis, Laura Christine. 2025. "Sharing Sensory Knowledge: Edwidge Danticat’s Breath, Eyes, Memory" Literature 5, no. 2: 10. https://doi.org/10.3390/literature5020010
APA StyleOtis, L. C. (2025). Sharing Sensory Knowledge: Edwidge Danticat’s Breath, Eyes, Memory. Literature, 5(2), 10. https://doi.org/10.3390/literature5020010