Next Article in Journal
Determination of Pyrrolizidine Alkaloids in Dietary Sources Using a Spectrophotometric Method
Previous Article in Journal
Functional Radical Cervical Dissection for Differentiated Thyroid Cancer: The Experience of a Single Center
 
 
Journal of Mind and Medical Sciences is published by MDPI from Volume 12 Issue 1 (2025). Previous articles were published by another publisher in Open Access under a CC-BY (or CC-BY-NC-ND) licence, and they are hosted by MDPI on mdpi.com as a courtesy and upon agreement with Valparaiso University (ValpoScholar).
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Personality Dimensions and Attributional Styles in Individuals With and Without Gender Dysphoria

by
Siamak Khodarahimi
1,*,
Hassan Ali Veiskarami
2 and
Maria Mercedes Ovejero Bruna
3
1
Eghlid Branch, Islamic Azad University, Eghlid 73815-114, Fars Province, Iran
2
Lorestan University, Khorramabad, Iran
3
Faculty of Psychology, Complutense University, Madrid, Spain
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
J. Mind Med. Sci. 2018, 5(2), 284-293; https://doi.org/10.22543/7674.52.P284293
Submission received: 31 July 2018 / Revised: 31 August 2018 / Accepted: 30 September 2018 / Published: 3 October 2018

Highlights

  • Patients with gender dysphoria have significantly higher neuroticism and lower agreeableness.
  • Patients with gender dysphoria have significantly lower positive attributional style.

Abstract

:

Highlights

  • Patients with gender dysphoria have significantly higher neuroticism and lower agreeableness.
  • Patients with gender dysphoria have significantly lower positive attributional style.

Abstract

This research investigates personality dimensions and attributional styles among individuals with and without gender dysphoria in relationship to gender, educational level, and ethnicity. Participants were 60 men and women with and without gender dysphoria. A demographic sheet and two inventories were used. Results showed that patients with gender dysphoria had significantly higher neuroticism and lower agreeableness compared with individuals without gender dysphoria. No significance differences in extraversion, openness to experience, and conscientiousness (based on the “big five” personality model) were found between those with and without gender dysphoria. Also, individuals without gender dysphoria had higher positive attributional styles compared to patients with gender dysphoria. Finally, there were significant effects for gender and ethnicity on personality dimensions, but not for gender, ethnicity, or the ethnicity by gender interaction on the attributional styles.

Introduction

Sexuality is, and its variations are, products of an individual’s genetic sex, gender identity, gender roles, and sexual orientation. Historically, the concept of gender dysphoria has progressed from homosexuality, transsexualism, and gender identity disorder to the current terminology of psychiatry and clinical psychology [1,2,3]. Gender dysphoria is related to a marked incongruence between one’s experienced/ expressed gender and his/her assigned gender, and is associated with clinically significant distress or impairment in social, occupational, or other important areas of functioning [4,5,6]. In practice, gender dysphoria is a diagnostic term that allows individuals access to care and helps to minimize jeopardy in social, occupational, or legal areas; furthermore, gender dysphoria is distinguished from sexual orientation [7,8]. Overall, contemporary studies have shown an increasing incidence of referrals for gender dysphoria diagnosis and a higher rate of psychiatric and developmental problems in those individuals with gender dysphoria [9]. Also, the prevalence of male-to-female transition is consistently higher than female-to-male transition in adults with gender dysphoria [10]. Nevertheless, controversy yet surrounds the evidence regarding the prevalence of coexisting or comorbid psychopathology in patients with gender dysphoria [10,11]. In contrast with the literature on gender dysphoria and concurrent psychopathology in the Western countries, there is little evidence about its non-disordered personality correlates in general. Thus, this study investigates the role of gender dysphoria on personality dimensions and attributional styles, and determines the roles of gender, ethnicity, and the educational level within a sample of Iranian adults.

Personality, attributional styles and gender dysphoria

Personality and character correlates of homosexuality, transsexualism, and gender identity disorder as precursor labels of gender dysphoria have been investigated in many studies, with few types of personality disorders evident in individuals with homosexuality, transsexual, and gender identity disorder [12,13,14]. In gay, lesbian, bisexual, and transgender individuals, Grant, Flynn, Odlaug and Schreiber (2012) indicated that about 93.8% exhibited at least one personality disorder (i.e. borderline, obsessive- compulsive and avoidant personality disorders) [15,16,17]. Duišin and colleagues (2014) showed that persons with gender dysphoria have significantly more personality disorders compared to individuals in a control group, particularly with respect to paranoid and avoidant personality disorders [18,19]. Settineri, Merlo, Bruno and Mento (2015) also indicated that the majority of individuals with gender dysphoria showed personality disorders such as paranoia [20], with a prevalence of comorbid personality disorders being about 60% in adults with gender dysphoria [10]. Barisic, Duisin, Djordjevic, Vujovic and Bizic (2017) have indicated that the combination of low neuroticism and psychoticism, and high agreeableness and extroversion were good predictors of a successful outcome for post- operative transition in patients with gender dysphoria after undergoing gender re-assignment surgery. However, there has been a lack of non-Western perspectives regarding the relationship between gender dysphoria and non-disordered personality dimensions and attributional styles in adults [21].

Theoretical approaches of Gender Dysphoria

Gender identity is influenced by a combination of biology, socialization, and culture [22]. According to the biological approach, the way the fetus develops and functions depends on its innate sensitivity to particular hormones, prenatal brain-sexing, and the availability of the relevant hormones in brain [23,24,25]. Both experimental and clinical investigations of neurological and hormonal substrates of gender dysphoria in adults involve some degree of biological gender differentiation in men’s and women’s psychological functioning [10,26]. From the biological approach, the benefits of clinical treatment of gender dysphoria are translated into a reduction of social exclusion, and the process of sex- reassignment surgery using biomedical treatment may decrease family psychopathology or stress [27].
From a socio-cultural perspective, family and social learning theories suggest that role-modeling of gender distinction, gender-specific behaviors, and the negative effect of a disturbed relationship with one or both parents may play a part in the development of gender dysphoria [28,29,30,31]. Overall, socio-cultural theories assume that gender differences in gender dysphoria may be influenced by self-perceived gender roles, gender socialization, and the presence of socio-structural power differentials within a culture [32]. Also, individuals with gender dysphoria may suffer from social bias, discrimination, and prejudice, and their basic civil justice might be denied due to stereotypes that are reinforced by labels of mental illness [33,34]. In case of gender dysphoria, Berlin (2016) suggested that both nature and nurture may influence feelings of self- expression and gender identity, particularly in Middle Eastern cultures.
From a psychological perspective, the embryonic markers of gender identity may emerge very early in development [35,36]. Psychological theories suggest that child-rearing and mother-blame may be related to gender dysphoria, and they conceptualize an etiology related to over-involved mothers and distant fathers, as these may disturb typical gender socialization in males [37]. According to Zucker and Bradley (1995), the common psychological trait in mothers of boys with gender dysphoria is the need to be nurtured by a female child [38]. Corbett (1999) suggested that parental influence and social reinforcement are major variables related to gender dysphoria development [39]. This approach speculates that confusion about one’s individual personality and sexual identity affects the ability to be intimate, thereby interfering with sexual development [40,41,42].
From a psychological perspective, psychosocial factors are often conceptualized as having a perpetuating rather than causal role on the emergence of gender dysphoria [10,43]. Lopez, Stewart and Jacobson- Dickman (2016) concluded that those who are validated in their gender dysphoria by supportive family and social environments have more favorable psychological outcomes [44]. With respect to gender-linked roles and psychosocial capabilities or resources, the psychological perceptive predicts that these variables may play a role in the differences between individuals with and without gender dysphoria with regard to personality traits and attributional styles [45,46].

Materials and Methods

The present study

According to a biopsychosocial perspective [47,48], biological differences, modes of familial interaction, and social and cultural factors might produce gender roles conflicts that emerge as gender dysphoria in childhood; in turn, these factors may influence their personality dimensions and attributional styles. Specifically, the present study suggests that complex interactions involving socialization with gender roles in familial and social contexts, the early experience of dysfunctional gender roles, power of gender roles in a person's family life, social stigma and biases, and instrumental outcomes of gender roles may influence personality domains (i.e., neuroticism, extraversion, openness to experience, agreeableness and conscientiousness) and attributional styles (i.e. negative and positive attributional styles) in patients with gender dysphoria.
From an integrative conceptual perspective, the present study assumes that individuals with gender dysphoria encounter numerous psychosocial constraints during their sexual maturity toward their gender identity within a culture. As such, these constraints may influence their personality and attributional style in a negative way. This study further assumes that social stigma, prejudice, and discrimination toward individuals with gender dysphoria and gender non-conformity may also influence their personality dimensions—based on the “Big Five” personality model—and attributional style [43,44,45,46]. This study thus hypothesizes that in this sample of Iranian adults: (a) the personality dimensions and attributional styles would differ in individuals with and without gender dysphoria; and (b) gender, educational level, and ethnicity—factors related to biological and socio-cultural development—could exert a significant role on personality dimensions and attributional styles.

Participants

The sample consisted of 60 men (30 individuals with gender dysphoria and 30 without gender dysphoria) from Shiraz City, Fars province; Iran. Gender classification of was based on their biological sex in this study. This sample further consisted of 30 males and 30 females (15 individuals with gender dysphoria and 15 individuals without gender dysphoria in each group). The mean and standard deviation of age for individuals with and without gender dysphoria were 28.84 (S=1.24) and 30.35 (S=1.64) respectively. The educational level ranged from diploma (N=30) and skill degree (N=18) to bachelor degree (N=12). The ethnicity of this sample was included Fars (N=43), and Turkish (N=17); all participants were Muslims.

Assessment Instruments

A demographic questionnaire gathered information about the participants’ status, gender, educational level, and ethnicity. Two standardized inventories were applied: (1) the NEO Personality Inventory-Revised (NEO PI-R), and (2) the Attributional Style Questionnaire (ASQ).
The NEO Personality Inventory-Revised (NEO PI- R; Costa & McCrae, 1992) consists of 240 personality items and three validity items. The NEO PI-R is based on the Five-Factor model and measures the interpersonal, motivational, emotional, and attitudinal styles of adults and adolescents. The NEO PI-R was designed to provide a general description of five domains in the normal personality relevant to clinical situations. These domains include: Neuroticism, Extraversion, Openness to Experience, Agreeableness, and Conscientiousness. Internal consistency coefficients range from .86 to .95 for domain scales; stability coefficients ranging from .51 to .83 have been found in three-year, six-year, and seven-year longitudinal studies based on the original NEO-PI factors. Test-retest reliability over 6 years for the factors are: N= .83, E= .82, O= .83, A= .63, C= .79 [44]. The reliability and validity of the NEO-PI-R have been confirmed by sever¬al studies in the Iranian population [49]. The reliability of the NEO PI-R domains using Cronbach’s alpha internal consistency in this study was: N= .93, E= .88, O= .87, A= .88, C= .89.
The Attributional Style Questionnaire (ASQ; Peterson et al., 1982) is the most widely used psychometric instrument for measuring attributional style. It contains 12 hypothetical events, 6 of them describing positive events (‘you meet an old friend who compliments you at your appearance’) and 6 describing negative events (‘you go out on a certain date and it goes badly’). Events are divided afterwards into an equal number of achievement and interpersonal contexts. The perceived cause of such event is rated along the dimensions of locus (that is due to the person or the situation), stability (likely or unlikely to occur again), and globality (limited in its influence or widespread) using seven-point scales. Scores can be computed for any dimension related to positive and negative events [45]. Test-retest coefficient was calculated at .64. The reliability and validity of ASQ has been supported in many studies [50,51,52]. Psychometric properties of ASQ have confirmed in Iranian samples [53]. The reliability of the ASQ for positive and negative subscales using Cronbach’s alpha internal consistency in this study is .87 and .86.

Procedure

According to Wilson, VanVoorhis, and Morgan (2007), sample size was considered adequate for comparisons between individuals with and without gender dysphoria [54]. All participants with gender dysphoria in the clinical group were recruited from several outpatient psychological clinics. Individuals with gender dysphoria were independently diagnosed based on clinical criteria by two clinical psychologists. The presence of gender dysphoria in clinical sample was further affirmed by a team of medical professionals. This clinical group was selected applying the purposive sampling method within an ex post facto design in which the sample is selected to include people of interest and exclude those who do not suit the purpose [55,56]. Individuals without gender dysphoria were screened for psychiatric disorders as diagnosed by two clinical psychologists and, after their approval, they were recruited for this study. A clinical interview was used to screen the control group for psychopathology. Individuals in the control group were matched to the clinical sample based on their biological sex, ethnicity, the level of education, and age. Therefore, the two samples were equivalent on several relevant demographic variables. As a part of the ethical code for this research, the authors guaranteed confidentiality. After reading the Informed Consent Form, participants had the opportunity to ask questions about the study giving informed consent, after which the questionnaires were administered.

Results

Table 1 presents mean and standard deviations for each group for the analyzed variables. To verify the first hypothesis regarding differences between gender dysphoric and non-dysphoric groups, seven t-tests for independent samples were computed to compare means on all variables of interest. Findings indicated that individuals without gender dysphoria had higher agreeableness, t(58) =-3.54, p <.001 and higher positive attributional style, t(58) =-4.14, p <.0001 than individuals with gender dysphoria. However, individuals with and without gender dysphoria did not differ on neuroticism, t(58) = 1.80, p <.07; extraversion, t(58) = -1.24, p <.22; openness to experience, t(58) =-.14, p <.88; conscientiousness, t(58) = 1.01, p <.31, and negative attribution style, t(58) = -.05, p <.95.
To test the second hypothesis involving the roles of gender, education, and ethnicity, multivariate analysis of covariance (MANCOVA) was conducted by participants’ status as the fixed variable, and gender, educational level, and ethnicity as covariates, and the five personality dimensions and positive and negative attribution styles as dependent variables. An overall multivariate effect was found for participants status; Wilks’ k = .435; F (7, 43) =7.61; p = .0001, gender; Wilks’ k = .301; F (7, 43) =13.57; p <.0001, educational level; Wilks’ k = .836; F (7, 43) =1.14; p <.355; ethnicity; Wilks’ k = .680; F (7, 43) =2.75; p <.01; gender*ethnicity interaction; Wilks’ k = .838; F (7, 43) =1.21; p <.365. These analyses indicate that while ethnicity and gender were significant, neither education nor the gender by ethnicity interaction affected the dependent variables. Tests of between subjects effects for ethnicity only showed significant differences on neuroticism, F (1, 59) =5.11, p<.02, (Table 2 and Table 3).

Discussions

Results for the first hypothesis regarding the Big Five personality model showed that individuals with gender dysphoria had significantly higher neuroticism and lower agreeableness scores compared to individuals without gender dysphoria, but no differences on extraversion, openness to experience, and conscientiousness dimensions of five big personality model. These findings are congruent with the previous literature regarding the potential roles of social, familial, and childhood factors on gender roles personality factors in individuals with showing sexual diversity, as represented by homosexuality, transsexualism, gender identity disorder, gender dysphoria, and their personality dysfunctions [57,58,59].
In line with Rosqvist, Nordlund and Kaiser [30], this study suggests that gender dysphoria, as an atypical developmental process, may influence personality dimensions in Iranian society. The present study also suggests that if one considers gender dysphoria as part of a developmental process then it is reasonable to assume that this atypical identity style may influence the lenses of people with gender dysphoria for self- exploration and introspection within their social and cultural contexts; particularly when their gendered- identity roles are not socially acceptable in that particular culture. Individuals with gender dysphoria may continuously attempt to overcome the socio- cultural barriers of their real world from the perspective of their own unusual gender identity styles. Thus, biopsychological personal dysfunctions and socio- cultural restrictions may amplify neuroticism and reduce agreeableness while also decreasing their positive attributional style.
The present findings are congruent with predictions of Big Five personality model in general life transitions [60]. According to this model [44], personality is associated with a range of main life outcomes like gender identity and gender-linked roles. The theory predicts that gender identity relates to the psychological characteristics of being either male or female, in other words the self-perceptions of masculinity and femininity, variables that likely influence individual’s personality dimensions. Such assumptions may account for the higher neuroticism and lower agreeableness found in individuals with gender dysphoria. These results are also consistent with the literature that suggests the influence of identity disorder on personality dimensions in abnormal ways [18,21]. Such personality characteristics highlight the enduring effect of gender identity and socio-cultural conflict during gender socialization in childhood and adulthood adjustment in men and women with gender dysphoria.
Furthermore, results of the first hypothesis in attributional style indicated that individuals with gender dysphoria had significant lower positive attributional style than participants in the control group, while no differences were found in negative attributional style. These findings are consistent with predictions of attributional style conceptualizations and gender identity in general [61,62,63]. In line with Maier, Peterson, and Schwartz (2000), the attributional reformulation of interpersonal and gender-linked roles by those with gender dysphoria may be influenced by their socio- cultural context in a culture [43]. Specifically, patients with gender dysphoria often encounter many negative social stigmas because of their atypical needs and life style, and in turn, they may use more negative attributional styles for solving of their interpersonal and general life problems. According to attributional theory, patients with gender dysphoria when realizing their non- conforming gender identity may experience social exclusion in a culture; and this process may in part induce a negative attributional style for dealing with problems.
Results from the second hypothesis demonstrated significant effects for influences of gender and ethnicity on personality dimensions in this study. Tests of between subjects’ effects for gender showed that males had significantly higher scores on neuroticism compared to females; and women had significantly higher scores on extraversion than men. These findings are consistent with earlier investigations that supported the role of biological sex on big five personality dimensions [64,65,66]. Further, tests of between subjects’ effects for ethnicity only showed significant differences on neuroticism; and individuals with Fars ethnicity had greater neuroticism than participants with Turkish ethnicity, a finding consistent with previous research on ethnicity and the big five personality dimensions [67,68]. Thus, both gender and ethnicity can influence personality dimensions beyond that of gender identity orientation through socialization and social recognition procedures which take place during the childhood development in familial and social contexts. The present study speculates that the development and effects of gender dysphoria are cultural bound. Finally, the test of the second hypothesis did not support roles for gender and ethnicity on attributional style [43,62], effects that may be explained in the light of situational or non- persistent causal interpretations of events in this sample. Given the lack of multicultural differences on gender, ethnicity and attributional style, this subject warrants further investigation particularly within a cross-cultural context.

Conclusions

In conclusion, this study contributes to the understanding of gender psychology as it demonstrates how gender dysphoria may influence personality and attributional styles in a specific cultural context. Furthermore, the study may offer applications for addressing social policies for individuals with gender dysphoria. And these results may be useful for clinicians and educators who educate, diagnose, and treat patients with gender dysphoria in that they highlight the relevance of cultural factors in understanding sexual diversity.
The present study is limited by the small sample size and by the fact that it relied only on self-report inventories. Further investigations should apply both experimental and longitudinal designs for the purpose of better understanding sociocultural factors in the development of gender dysphoria by using both psychological and biological measures in men and women from different cohorts and cross-cultural samples.

Conflicts of Interest disclosure

There are no known conflicts of interest in the publication of this article, and there was no financial support that could have influenced the outcomes. The manuscript was read and approved by all authors.

Compliance with Ethical Standards

Any aspect of the work covered in this manuscript that has involved human patients has been conducted with the ethical approval of all relevant bodies and that such approvals are acknowledged within the manuscript.

References

  1. Pichot, P. DSM-III: The 3d edition of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders from the American Psychiatric Association. Rev Neurol (Paris). 1986, 142, 489–499. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  2. Cohen-Kettenis, P.T.; Gooren, L.j. Transsexualism: A review of etiology, diagnosis and treatment. J Psychosom Res. 1999, 46, 315–333. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Ehrbar, R.D. Consensus from differences: Lack of professional consensus on the retention of the gender identity disorder diagnosis. International Journal of Transgenderism. 2010, 12, 60–74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Hird, M.J. Unidentified pleasures: Gender identity and its failure. Body & Society. 2002, 8, 39–54. [Google Scholar]
  5. Schrock, D.; Reid, L.; Boyd, E.M. Transsexuals’ embodiment of womanhood. Gender & Society. 2005, 19, 317–335. [Google Scholar]
  6. Vstarcevic, V.S.; Rossi Monti, M.; D'Agostino, A.; Berle, D. Will DSM-5 make us feel dysphoric? Conceptualisation(s) of dysphoria in the most recent classification of mental disorders. Aust N Z J Psychiatry. 2013, 47, 954–960. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. American_Psychiatric_Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders, 5th ed.; American_Psychiatric_Association: Washington, DC, USA, 2013; ISBN-13: 978-0890425558. [Google Scholar]
  8. Garcia-Falgueras, A. Gender dysphoria and body integrity identity disorder: Similarities and differences. Psychology. 2014, 5, 160–165. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Chen, M.; Fuqua, J.; Eugster, E.A. Characteristics of referrals for gender dysphoria over a 13-year period. J Adolesc Health. 2016, 58, 369–371. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  10. Zucker, K.J.; Lawrence, A.A.; Kreukels, B.P. Gender dysphoria in adults. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2016, 12, 217–247. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  11. Cole, C.M.; O'boyle, M.; Emory, L.E.; Meyer, W.J., III. Comorbidity of gender dysphoria and other major psychiatric diagnoses. Arch Sex Behav. 1997, 26, 13–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. American_Psychiatric_Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders, 4th ed.; tex t rev; American_Psychiatric_Association: Washington, DC, USA, 2000; ISBN-13: 978-0890420256. [Google Scholar]
  13. Bodlund, O.; Armelius, K. Self-image and personality traits in gender identity disorders: An empirical study. J Sex Marital Ther. 1994, 20, 303–317. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Bodlund, O.; Kullgren, G.; Sundbom, E.; Höjerback, T. Personality traits and disorders among transsexuals. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 1993, 88, 322–327. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Jonas, C.H. An objective approach to the personality and environment in homosexuality. The Psychiatric Quarterly. 1944, 18, 626–641. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Fleming, M.; Cohen, D.; Salt, P.; Jones, D.; Jenkins, S. A study of pre-and postsurgical transsexuals: MMPI characteristics. Arch Sex Behav. 1981, 10, 161–170. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  17. Rosen, A.C. Brief report of MMPI characteristics of sexual deviation. Psychol Rep. 1974, 35, 73–74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  18. Grant, J.E.; Flynn, M.; Odlaug, B.L.; Schreiber, L. Personality disorders in gay, lesbian, bisexual, and transgender chemically dependent patients. Am J Addict. 2011, 20, 405–411. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  19. Duišin, D.; Batinić, B.; Barišić, J.; Djordjevic, M.L.; Vujović, S.; Bizic, M. Personality disorders in persons with gender identity disorder. ScientificWorldJournal. 2014, 2014, 809058. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Settineri, S.; Merlo, E.M.; Bruno, A.; Mento, C. Personality Assessment in Gender Dysphoria: Clinical observation in psychopathological evidence. Mediterranean Journal of Clinical Psychology. 2016, 4, 1–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Barisic, J.; Duisin, D.; Djordjevic, M.; Vujovic, S.; Bizic, M. Influence of basic personality dimensions on adaptation processes and quality of life in patients with gender dysphoria in post transitory period. In Proceedings of the 2nd Contemporary Trans Health in Europe: Focus on Challenges and Improvements, Belgrade, Serbia, 6–8 April 2017. [Google Scholar]
  22. Lopez, X.; Stewart, S.; Jacobson-Dickman, E. Approach to Children and Adolescents with Gender Dysphoria. Pediatr Rev. 2016, 37, 89–96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Ettner, R. Care of the elderly transgender patient. Curr Opin Endocrinol Diabetes Obes. 2013, 20, 580–584. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Kreukels, B.P.C.; Köhler, B.; Nordenström, A.; Roehle, R.; Thyen, U.; Bouvattier, C.; de Vries, A.L.C.; Cohen-Kettenis, P.T.; dsd-LIFE group. Gender Dysphoria and Gender Change in Disorders of Sex Development/Intersex Conditions: Results From the dsd-LIFE Study. J Sex Med. 2018, 15, 777–785. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. MacLusky, N.J.; Naftolin, F. Sexual differentiation of the central nervous system. Science. 1981, 211, 1294–1302. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  26. Hines, M.; Constantinescu, M.; Spencer, D. Early androgen exposure and human gender development. Biol Sex Differ. 2015, 6, 3. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  27. Zucker, K.J.; Wood, H.; Singh, D.; Bradley, S.J. A developmental, biopsychosocial model for the treatment of children with gender identity disorder. J Homosex. 2012, 59, 369–397. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  28. Haldeman, D.C. Gender atypical youth: Clinical and social issues. School Psychology Review. 2000, 29, 192–200. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Kessler, S.J.; McKenna, W. Gender construction in everyday life: Transsexualism (abridged). Feminism & Psychology. 2000, 10, 11–29. [Google Scholar]
  30. Rosqvist, H.B.; Nordlund, L.; Kaiser, N. Developing an authentic sex: Deconstructing developmental– psychological discourses of transgenderism in a clinical setting. Feminism & Psychology. 2014, 24, 20–36. [Google Scholar]
  31. Yost, M.R.; Smith, T.E. Transfeminist psychology. Feminism & Psychology. 2014, 24, 145–156. [Google Scholar]
  32. Schmitt, D.P.; Long, A.E.; McPhearson, A.; O'brien, K.; Remmert, B.; Shah, S.H. Personality and gender differences in global perspective. Int J Psychol. 2017, 52 (Suppl. S1), 45–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Lev, A.I. Gender dysphoria: Two steps forward, one step back. Clinical social work journal. 2013, 41, 288–296. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Winters, K. Gender madness in American psychiatry: Essays from the struggle for dignity; GID Reform Advocates: Dillon, CO, 2008; ISBN-13: 978-1439223888. [Google Scholar]
  35. Berlin, F.S. A conceptual overview and commentary on gender dysphoria. J Am Acad Psychiatry Law. 2016, 44, 246–252. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  36. Martin, C.L.; Ruble, D.N.; Szkrybalo, J. Cognitive theories of early gender development. Psychol Bull. 2002, 128, 903–933. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  37. Stoller, R.J. The mother's contribution to infantile transvestic behaviour. Int J Psychoanal. 1966, 47, 384–395. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  38. Zucker, K.J.; Bradley, S.J. Gender identity disorder and psychosexual problems in children and adolescents. Can J Psychiatry. 1990, 35, 477–486. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  39. Rottnek Matthew. In Sissies and tomboys: Gender nonconformity and homosexual childhood; 1999; ISBN 9780814774847.
  40. Lev, A.I. Transgender emergence; Haworth Clinical Practice Press: New York, NY, USA, 2004. [Google Scholar]
  41. McBee, C. Towards a more affirming perspective: Contemporary psychodynamic practice with trans and gender nonconforming individuals; The University of Chicago School of Social Service Administration (Org); AdvocatesV Forum; 2013. [Google Scholar]
  42. Stein, E. The mismeasure of desire: The science, theory, and ethics of sexual orientation; Oxford University Press: Demand, 2001; ISBN 9780195142440. [Google Scholar]
  43. Maier, S.F.; Peterson, C.; Schwartz, B. From helplessness to hope: The seminal career of Martin Seligman. In The science of optimism and hope: Research essays in honor of Martin EP Seligman; 2000; pp. 11–37. [Google Scholar]
  44. Costa, P.T.; McCrae, R.R. NEO PI-R Professional Manual; Psychological Assessment Resources. Inc: Odessa, FL, USA, 1992. [Google Scholar]
  45. Peterson, C.; Semmel, A.; Von Baeyer, C.; Abramson, L.Y.; Metalsky, G.I.; Seligman, M.E. The attributional style questionnaire. Cognitive therapy and research. 1982, 6, 287–299. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Poropat, A. The relationship between attributional style, gender and the Five-Factor Model of personality. Personality and individual differences. 2002, 33, 1185–1201. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. McLeod, S. Biological theories of gender. Simply Psychology 2014. Available online: https://www.simplypsychology.org/gender-biology.html.
  48. Nieder, T.O.; Richter-Appelt, H. Tertium non datur– either/or reactions to transsexualism amongst health care professionals: The situation past and present, and its relevance to the future. Psychology & Sexuality. 2011, 2, 224–243. [Google Scholar]
  49. Khodarahimi, S.; Rezaye, A.M. The effects of psychopathology and personality on substance abuse in twelve-step treatment programme abstainers, opiate substance abusers and a control sample. Heroin Addict Relat Clin Probl. 2012, 14, 35–48. [Google Scholar]
  50. Carver, C.S. How should multifaceted personality constructs be tested? Issues illustrated by self- monitoring, attributional style, and hardiness. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1989, 56, 577. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  51. Smith, C.P. Motivation and personality: Handbook of thematic content analysis; Cambridge University Press; ISBN 0-521-40052-X.
  52. Hodson, G.; Earle, M. Conservatism predicts lapses from vegetarian/vegan diets to meat consumption (through lower social justice concerns and social support). Appetite. 2018, 120, 75–81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  53. Fallon, M.A.; Careaga, J.S.; Sbarra, D.A.; OʼConnor, M.F. Utility of a Virtual Trier Social Stress Test: Initial Findings and Benchmarking Comparisons. Psychosom Med. 2016, 78, 835–840. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  54. VanVoorhis, C.W.; Morgan, B.L. Understanding power and rules of thumb for determining sample sizes. Tutorials in Quantitative Methods for Psychology. 2007, 3, 43–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Given, L.M. The Sage encyclopedia of qualitative research methods; Sage Publications, 2008; ISBN 978-1-4129-4163-1. [Google Scholar]
  56. Tongco, M.D.C. Purposive sampling as a tool for informant selection. Ethnobotany Research and applications. 2007, 5, 147–158. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Bleiberg, E.; Jackson, L.; Ross, J.L. Gender identity disorder and object loss. J Am Acad Child Psychiatry. 1986, 25, 58–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  58. Di Ceglie, D. Gender identity disorder in young people. Advances in Psychiatric Treatment. 2000, 6, 458–466. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Stoller, R.J. The hermaphroditic identity of hermaphrodites. The Journal of nervous and mental disease. 1964, 139, 453–457. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. McCrae, R.R.; Costa, P.T. Validation of the Five- Factor Model of Personality Across Instruments and Observers. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1987, 52, 81–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  61. Mahasneh, D.A.M.; Al-Zoubi, D.Z.H.; Batayeneh, D.O.T. The Relationship between Attribution Styles and Personality Traits, Gender and Academic Specialization among the Hashemite University Students. International Journal of Business and Social Science. 2013, 4, 286–295. [Google Scholar]
  62. Peterson, C.; Seligman, M.E.; Vaillant, G.E. Pessimistic explanatory style is a risk factor for physical illness: A thirty-five-year longitudinal study. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1988, 55, 23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  63. Wegner, D.M.; Vallacher, R.R. Implicit psychology: An introduction to social cognition; Oxford University Press, 1977; ISBN-13: 978-0195022292. [Google Scholar]
  64. Budaev, S.V. Sex differences in the Big Five personality factors: Testing an evolutionary hypothesis. Personality and individual differences. 1999, 26, 801–813. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Khodarahimi, S. Perfectionism and five-big model of personality in an Iranian sample. International Journal of Psychology and Counselling. 2010, 2, 72–79. [Google Scholar]
  66. Weisberg, Y.J.; DeYoung, C.G.; Hirsh, J.B. Gender differences in personality across the ten aspects of the Big Five. Front Psychol. 2011, 2, 178. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  67. Löckenhoff, C.E.; Terracciano, A.; Bienvenu, O.J.; Patriciu, N.S.; Nestadt, G.; McCrae, R.R.; Eaton, W.W.; Costa, P.T., Jr. Ethnicity, education, and the temporal stability of personality traits in the East Baltimore Epidemiologic Catchment Area study. J Res Pers. 2008, 42, 577–598. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  68. Khodarahimi, S. Psychopathy Deviate Tendency and Personality Characteristics in an Iranian Adolescents and Youth Sample: Gender Differences and Predictors. European Journal of Mental Health. 2012, 7, 90–101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Table 1. Mean and Standard Deviations of Personality Dimensions and Attribution Styles in Clinical and Control Groups and the Total Sample.
Table 1. Mean and Standard Deviations of Personality Dimensions and Attribution Styles in Clinical and Control Groups and the Total Sample.
Jmms 05 00042 i001
Table 2. Tests of Between Subjects Effects for Gender Differences in Personality Dimensions and Attribution Style.
Table 2. Tests of Between Subjects Effects for Gender Differences in Personality Dimensions and Attribution Style.
Jmms 05 00042 i002
Table 3. Tests of Between Subjects Effects for Ethnic Differences in Personality Dimensions and Attribution Style.
Table 3. Tests of Between Subjects Effects for Ethnic Differences in Personality Dimensions and Attribution Style.
Jmms 05 00042 i003

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Khodarahimi, S.; Veiskarami, H.A.; Ovejero Bruna, M.M. Personality Dimensions and Attributional Styles in Individuals With and Without Gender Dysphoria. J. Mind Med. Sci. 2018, 5, 284-293. https://doi.org/10.22543/7674.52.P284293

AMA Style

Khodarahimi S, Veiskarami HA, Ovejero Bruna MM. Personality Dimensions and Attributional Styles in Individuals With and Without Gender Dysphoria. Journal of Mind and Medical Sciences. 2018; 5(2):284-293. https://doi.org/10.22543/7674.52.P284293

Chicago/Turabian Style

Khodarahimi, Siamak, Hassan Ali Veiskarami, and Maria Mercedes Ovejero Bruna. 2018. "Personality Dimensions and Attributional Styles in Individuals With and Without Gender Dysphoria" Journal of Mind and Medical Sciences 5, no. 2: 284-293. https://doi.org/10.22543/7674.52.P284293

APA Style

Khodarahimi, S., Veiskarami, H. A., & Ovejero Bruna, M. M. (2018). Personality Dimensions and Attributional Styles in Individuals With and Without Gender Dysphoria. Journal of Mind and Medical Sciences, 5(2), 284-293. https://doi.org/10.22543/7674.52.P284293

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop