Next Article in Journal
Psychological Predictors of COVID-19-Related Anxiety in Vulnerable Groups
Previous Article in Journal
“I Believe More in the Ability of the Small Person to Make Big Changes”: Innovation and Social Entrepreneurship to Promote Public Health in Israel
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Assessing System Justification in Czech Population Using the System Justification Scale

INESAN (Institute for Evaluations and Social Analyses), Sokolovská 351/25, 18600 Prague, Czech Republic
Eur. J. Investig. Health Psychol. Educ. 2023, 13(9), 1801-1814; https://doi.org/10.3390/ejihpe13090131
Submission received: 29 August 2023 / Revised: 11 September 2023 / Accepted: 12 September 2023 / Published: 13 September 2023

Abstract

:
System justification (SJ) is an important construct in social psychology that has received considerable attention over the past three decades. At the empirical level, system justification is examined by means of a specially developed System Justification Scale (SJS), which is designed to explain how individuals accept justice, whether they consider a given social order to be fair, how they evaluate the conditions in the country in which they live, how they accept social change, or to what extent they express compliance with established rules. System justification involves not only those who benefit from the existing social order, but also those who are disadvantaged. In their case, system justification mitigates negative perceptions of objective inequalities and asymmetries. Empirical evidence suggests that system justification may also be associated with higher perceived quality of life. The present study translated and validated the SJS, providing complex and detailed information on the psychometric properties of the scale. In addition, the scale’s internal consistency, unidimensionality, and construct validity were examined. The conclusions presented are based on the results of exploratory factor analysis, internal consistency assessment, analysis of variance, correlation analysis, and confirmatory factor analysis. Results were obtained from face-to-face survey data collected from a sample of 1419 individuals representing the Czech population aged 18–79 years. Since the SJS showed high internal consistency, adequately discriminated levels of system justification, and had robust psychometric properties, it could be recommended for further use.

1. Introduction

System justification theory has received considerable attention from researchers in a number of disciplines, including sociology, political science, and psychology. It has been studied extensively over the past three decades in a variety of cultural and political contexts, where it has been used, among other things, to explain the functioning of society and to predict collective behavior [1,2,3,4,5,6]. Moreover, research on the mechanisms and contexts of system justification has become increasingly important in recent times, when many societies have had to adapt to a changing external environment. An appropriate response in the form of societal change can be effectively moderated by the needs, interests, and actions of affected actors, stakeholders, and populations. This is why questions of system justification are so important; indeed, Vesper et al. [7] considered system justification to be the most influential theory in contemporary social psychological research.
System justification posits that people tend to justify the existing social, economic, and political order [8]. As a social science construct, system justification is therefore based on the assumed need of individuals to maintain perceived justice and legitimacy [9,10], developing an idea of social order and the mechanisms of its maintenance. Thus, members of a given community tend to defend, rationalize, and legitimize the existing social system, including its norms, rules, and practices. Most often, they do this through prejudices, stereotypes, and ideologies [11,12]. Within this framework, established social relations are considered useful and appropriate, and individual actors therefore seek to legitimize and rationalize them. An important element of system justification theory is that all population groups are motivated to engage in system justification in a cross-sectional manner, i.e., not only those who benefit from the existing social order (for whom the defense of the given order can be assumed), but also those who are disadvantaged in some way within the given social structure. In their case, system justification may represent an ex post rationalization that mitigates negative perceptions of objective inequalities and asymmetries [13,14]. System justification theory posits that the seeds of system justification attitudes are common to all individuals, but that specific actors differ in the extent to which they accentuate this attitude [15].
Empirical evidence suggests that system justification as a psychological construct is related to political conservatism [16,17,18], particularly in terms of rejection of social change. However, system justification has also been found to be related to social dominance [19,20] or patriotism [21]. In addition, some authors have pointed out that system justification may be negatively correlated with willingness to participate in protests aimed at challenging the existing social order and structure [22,23,24]. Perceived justice and legitimacy of the system have been found to reinforce stabilizing social mechanisms and inhibit social change. Individuals who support the existing system and emphasize its legitimacy internalize social inequalities and denigrate or reject alternative worldviews [25,26]. In addition, higher levels of system justification may be associated with higher perceived quality of life [27,28,29].
Research on the causes, context, and consequences of system justification is linked to the development and refinement of relevant research methods and tools. Because system justification is often masked by stereotypes [30] and ideologies [15], it (system justification) cannot be studied directly. Because system justification is not directly reflected in a particular pattern of behavior, it cannot be observed. Therefore, research on this construct requires the use of an appropriate research instrument in the form of a specific scale that is both sufficiently sensitive, reliable, and valid. Given the crucial role of perceptions of social order and its evaluation, the System Justification Scale (hereafter referred to as the SJS) was developed by Jost and Banaji [8] as a useful tool for measuring issues related to inequalities and the legitimacy of the status quo.
The SJS is a self-report research tool designed to assess the level of perceived justice, fairness, and importance of a stable social order. The developed scale can be used to determine perceptions of country conditions, assessments of the need for social change, and perceived congruence between the actual situation and individual norms or values [8]. The proposed scale recognizes that the level of system justification may vary depending on specific situational factors [31,32]. Therefore, a careful analysis of system justification in different social and cultural contexts is necessary.
Although the SJS has been used in many countries around the world during its 30 years of existence, to the best of our knowledge, it has not been validated in the context of Czech society. Therefore, the aim of this study was to fill the existing knowledge gap and to verify the applicability of the scale in the Czech society. The accomplishment of this research task involved a number of activities, including translation of the SJS into the Czech language, primary data collection, and special analyses focused primarily on the psychometric properties of the scale. In addition, the study used some novel indicators for examining construct validity that, to our knowledge, have not been used before (compliance with legal norms, acceptance of the rule of law, or civic engagement). However, the research objective reflects the specific focus of this study, which directs its main attention to the measurement instrument itself. Thus, it is primarily concerned with examining the appropriateness of the individual items of the scale, evaluating the structure of the scale, its unidimensionality, and, last but not least, a thorough analysis of the psychometric properties of the scale. These objectives are expressed in instrumental form by the specific hypotheses that this study seeks to answer. The following hypotheses were tested:
(a)
System justification, as examined by the SJS, is a unidimensional construct.
(b)
All SJS items are highly correlated with each other.
(c)
The psychometric properties of the SJS indicate a good fit of the tested construct to the empirical data.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants and Procedures

Given the aim of the research, it was desirable to obtain findings on the SJS from a representative sample of the country’s general population aged 18–79 years. For this reason, an address-based sampling technique was used to select houses and apartments in predefined primary territorial units. The corresponding list of houses and dwellings was compiled during the census, so these data represent the best available sampling frame in terms of accuracy and completeness. In each household, specific respondents were identified using the Kish table [33]. In this way, a total of 2826 respondents were asked to participate during November 2021, and a total of 1438 face-to-face interviews were conducted; the response rate was thus 50.9%. Given that some interviews were incomplete, the final sample used in the analysis includes 1419 cases, which can be considered sufficient for the purposes of scale validation. The specific structure of the sample in terms of gender, age and size of place of residence is presented in Table 1.

2.2. Research Ethics

Informed consent was always obtained from participants prior to each interview. To ensure confidentiality of answers, all responses were anonymized and are presented only in summary form to avoid direct or indirect identification of specific individuals. In the case of participants aged 18–21, informed consent was obtained not only from the respondents interviewed, but also from at least one parent or guardian who was present throughout the interview.

2.3. Translation of the Instrument

An important step in the development of the research instrument was the translation of the SJS into the Czech language. This process was carried out carefully due to its critical importance, following the recommendations of Sousa and Rojjanasrirat [34] and Yu, Lee, and Woo [35]. Thus, two simultaneous translations were performed, with the original English version of the SJS being translated into Czech by two independent translators. The two translations were then compared and the differences identified were subsequently resolved resulting in a consolidated form of translation of the scale. A third translator then performed a back-translation from Czech to English to confirm the equivalence of the two language versions. In addition to the wording of the individual statements, the wording of the alternatives, i.e., the way in which the degree of agreement with each item was expressed, was also taken from the original scale.
The translated scale was then pilot-tested on a sample of 23 respondents recruited from the target population. Cognitive interviews in the form of think-alouds [36] were used to test the clarity of the items and their difficulty from the respondents’ perspective. The aim of these cognitive interviews was to identify any problematic, misleading or otherwise inappropriate wording. Based on the interviews conducted, one item that was perceived by respondents to be a double-barreled question was modified. Specifically, the item “Everyone has a fair chance at wealth and happiness.” Was modified to retain only “wealth” while omitting “happiness”. The reason for this was that respondents did not perceive “wealth” and “happiness” to be related, but rather pointed out their differences. In addition, only minor wording changes were made in the English translations of three other items. No other methodological problems or significant deficiencies in the clarity or accuracy of the scale items were found. The research instrument was subsequently supplemented with additional socio-demographic and merit questions.

2.4. Measures

In addition to the SJS, five independent variables that are substantially related to the construct of system justification were used to test the construct validity of the scale. They are explained in more detail along with the introduction of the SJS itself.

2.4.1. System Justification Scale (SJS)

As mentioned above, Jost and Banaji [8] developed the SJS to measure respondents’ attitudes and perceptions regarding social order. The SJS consists of a total of eight items relating to, among other things, perceived fairness, assessment of the functioning of the existing social system, quality of life, and opportunities to access wealth. The individual items were rated on a four-point Likert-type scale ranging from 1 = strongly disagree to 4 = strongly agree, in accordance with the findings and recommendations of Roccato et al. [37]. Thus, the total score on the scale ranges from 8 to 32, with higher scores indicating higher levels of system justification. The developed scale has been repeatedly used in many studies (e.g., [1,2,3,37,38]) to assess its reliability and validity.

2.4.2. Control Indicators

Given that system justification is intended to reflect the legitimacy of the existing social system and the acceptance of the status quo, even though it may not be advantageous for a given individual at a given time [15], the level of agreement with the statement “Even bad laws should be obeyed.” was measured. The initial assumption was that the degree of agreement with a given statement would be positively correlated with system justification. Next, perceived fairness was examined by measuring agreement with the statement “Ordinary people do not get a fair share of the national wealth.” In this case, a negative association was hypothesized if people with higher SJS scores were expected to agree with the statement to a lesser extent than people with lower SJS scores. The legitimacy of social order (or a rule-of-law principle) was also tested by the statement “There are different rules for rich people and different rules for poor people.” Again, a negative association was expected, with people with higher SJS scores less likely to agree with the statement than people with lower SJS scores. An important perspective that might have differentiated SJS well is the willingness to strike. Vesper et al. [7] consider this indicator to be a key element in examining the construct validity of the SJS. Given that the indicators presented above could lead to the misleading conclusion that system justifiers, i.e., individuals with high levels of system justification, are rather passive, the statement: “It is important for me to participate in preserving the environment in which I live.” was also included in the control indicators. All of these control indicators are presented in Table 2.

2.5. Data Analysis

All statistical analyses were performed with IBM SPSS ver. 27 except for confirmatory factor analysis, for which AMOS 24 was used.
In order to provide comprehensive information about the sample and the responde;nts involved in the research, a series of descriptive statistics were calculated: for each item of the scale, the distribution of responses was examined and the results obtained were summarized using basic statistics in the form of mean (M), standard deviation (SD), skewness, and kurtosis. The internal consistency of the scale was assessed using Cronbach’s alpha [39,40,41].
To assess the construct validity of the translated SJS, appropriate methodological recommendations and procedures were consistently followed [42]. Exploratory factor analysis (EFA) based on principal component analysis [43] was used to assess the dimensionality of the scale. However, EFA provided important evidence not only in terms of the number of dimensions (corresponding to the number of factors) and the total amount of variance explained, but also in terms of the construct validity of the scale. Therefore, factor loadings (FL) and communality data (h2) were used to assess concurrent validity. Factor loadings, which indicate the degree to which a variable was associated with a factor, indicated the strength of the relationship between the factor and the variable. For convergent validity, factor loadings of individual scale items should be high. In addition, communalities, which document the extent to which the total variability of a variable is explained by the factors in the analysis, indicate how the variable is explained by the identified factors. Therefore, a higher communality value indicates a higher convergent validity. Confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) was then performed using maximum likelihood estimation methods. In order to obtain a more accurate assessment of the model’s fit to the data, a number of indices were generated, namely the root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA), as well as the standardized root mean square residual (SRMR), the comparative fit index (CFI), the Tucker–Lewis index (TLI), and the GFI. In the context of conducting exploratory and confirmatory factor analysis, it should be noted that the total sample was randomly divided into two equivalent halves using a randomization procedure, as recommended by other researchers (e.g., [44]), which ensured the equivalence of the two subsamples. Subsequently, an exploratory factor analysis was conducted on the first subsample (n = 709), while the second subsample (n = 710) was used for confirmatory factor analysis. The above procedure has been successfully applied in other scale validation studies (e.g., [45]). In conducting the factor analysis, missing values were handled using the listwise method; it should be emphasized that the analysis of other variables (particularly the control indicators) may be based on a different number of valid cases.
Due to the nature of the data, tests of correlations between scale items were performed using Pearson’s correlation coefficient. An analysis of variance (ANOVA) was performed to determine if there were statistically significant differences between the groups based on the SJS scale and the control indicators, or Kendall’s tau_b values were calculated with respect to the types of variables given.

3. Results

3.1. Univariate Statistics and Internal Consistency

Table 3 provides information on the mean scores of the individual items that make up the SJS; the table also includes standard deviations. The mean scores range from 2.22 to 2.78, and the standard deviations range from 0.775 to 0.851, indicating that no score is significantly different from the others. The floor effect was 1.8% and the ceiling effect was 0.0%; the values obtained do not exceed the threshold recommended by Cain et al. [46], which is 50%. The overall mean SJS was 18.13 and the standard deviation was 4.815.
As further shown in Table 3, the SJS in this study reached a skewness of −0.142 and a kurtosis of −0.740; both values fall within the recommended range of −1.5 to +1.5 [47], and thus the scale can be considered normally distributed. The actual distribution of the scale scores is shown in Figure 1.
The unidimensionality of the scale was assessed using the results of exploratory factor analysis. The value of the Kaiser–Meyer–Olkin (KMO) test of sampling adequacy was 0.901, which can be interpreted as showing that the analyzed data were suitable for exploratory factor analysis. In addition, Bartlett’s test of sphericity revealed a statistically significant result with χ2 = 2570.627 (df = 28, p < 0.001). A factor with an eigenvalue >1 was extracted using principal component analysis, which explained 54.5% of the total variance. Table 3, with the factor loadings and communalities, shows that the absolute values of the factor scores ranged from 0.601 to 0.875. According to Pett et al. [48], higher absolute values indicate a greater contribution of the item to the explanation of the factor, while factor loadings greater than 0.8 indicate that the item contributes significantly to the explanation of the factor. Thus, based on the evidence presented, it can be summarized that the items comprising the SJS were interrelated through a common factor [49] and the results obtained support the hypothesis of the unidimensionality of the SJS.
The above statement is further supported by the results of the internal consistency assessment of the SJS. In this regard, Cronbach’s alpha coefficient was calculated and reached a high value of 0.884, which indicated the high internal consistency of the tested scale. This is further evidenced by the high item–total correlation (ITC), where, according to Taherdoost [50], a value of the coefficient higher than 0.4 can be considered high enough. Table 3 shows that in this study, the correlations between the items and the total scale value ranged from 0.553 to 0.815, which exceeds the recommended threshold and thus supports the conclusion that the tested scale is internally consistent. Thus, the above results provided credible and sufficient evidence that the scale items reflect the intended theoretical construct with a high degree of probability.

3.2. Psychometric Properties of the SJS

The construct validity of the scale and its psychometric properties were assessed using confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) with maximum likelihood estimation. A separate subsample independent of the data used for exploratory factor analysis was used for this analysis. The results for this separate subsample (n = 710) showed a significant chi-square value of 30.227 (df = 14, p < 0.05), which is common for large samples of several hundred cases [51]. A graphical representation of the corresponding SJS model is shown in Figure 2 that introduces the standardized coefficients (factor loadings) ranging from −0.65 for the item “Czech society needs to be radically restructured” to 0.92 for the item “In general, you find society to be fair”. Moreover, the communalities are presented as well.
As the analysis progressed, the original model was adjusted to account for errors representing unobserved variables that affect variance not captured by the latent construct. Table 4 shows the results of both the original and modified models, as well as the specific absolute and incremental indices that were calculated. The results indicate a good fit and show that the proposed model fits the empirical data well.
It can be seen that the main goodness-of-fit indices provided convincing support for a good fit: the root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) reached 0.041, which is below the recommended threshold. The standardized root mean square residuals (SRMR) also reached an excellent value of 0.0206. In addition, the goodness-of-fit index (GFI), comparative fit index (CFI), Tucker–Lewis index (TLI), and normalized fit index (NFI) also provided strong enough arguments to confirm the hypothesis of excellent fit. Their values are shown in Table 4.

3.3. Construct Convergent and Divergent Validity

Construct validity is crucial in the process of validating a research instrument as it informs how accurately a given scale measures the intended construct [52]. Construct validity examines the extent to which scale items are correlated with each other, as a high correlation between them indicates that they measure the same construct in a consistent manner [53]. A combination of the following tests and analyses was used to test the construct convergent validity of the Czech version of the SJS:
(a)
Exploratory factor analysis; in addition to the previously mentioned results regarding factor loadings (FL) and communalities (h2) in Table 3, low or no cross-validation in the EFA indicated high construct convergent validity. The latter is high because there is only one factor.
(b)
The average variance extracted (AVE), which represented the average amount of variance captured by the indicators associated with the latent variable, reached 0.54. The value obtained indicated that, on average, 54% of the variance of the items was explained by the latent variable; the result also indicated a high (acceptable) convergent validity given that the items were interrelated and measured the same underlying construct.
(c)
Composite reliability (CR), which tested for correlations between indicators of the latent variable. The CR value in this case was 0.76, which exceeded the recommended value of 0.7. Thus, this result also indicated that the items were correlated with each other, which supported the hypothesis of the satisfactory convergent validity of the SJS and suggested that the scale was a reliable instrument in this case.
(d)
The correlation matrix presented in Table 5 shows statistically significant associations between all items, suggesting that all items may be measuring the same construct. Thus, the correlation matrix provides another strong argument to support the construct validity assumption of the scale.
  • (e) Table 6 shows that respondents who agreed with the statement about the need to comply with the laws, even if they are bad, had higher scores on the SJS. Furthermore, the expected negative association between the SJS and perceived fairness is evident in the empirical data, with respondents who agreed with the statement that ordinary people do not get a fair share of the national wealth scoring lower on the SJS. Similarly, those who agreed with the statement that different rules apply to rich and poor people had lower SJS scores. Consistent with the expectation embedded in social justification theory, the significantly lower willingness to strike of people with high SJS scores is evident, confirming their resistance to activities aimed at delegitimizing the existing order.
An indicator of divergent validity is the willingness of people with high SJS scores to participate in activities aimed at preserving their environment. The obtained results are consistent with the hypotheses and allow us to conclude that the SJS in the Czech environment measures the intended theoretical construct.

4. Discussion

The main aim of this study was to evaluate the psychometric properties of the Czech version of the SJS. To this end, a combination of several analytical procedures was used, including an assessment of the scale’s internal consistency, exploratory factor analysis, and confirmatory factor analysis. Overall, the Czech version of the SJS was found to have excellent psychometric properties and to be a reliable and valid instrument for measuring system justification. Specifically, the results indicated that the SJS performed very well in the Czech context: the scale showed acceptable skewness and kurtosis [47] and the inter-item correlation values exceeded the recommended threshold of 0.4, which met the criteria for acceptability [49]. The SJS also demonstrated a unidimensional structure, high internal consistency, and satisfactory construct validity. The standardized root mean square residuals (SRMR) reached 0.0206, which was within the acceptable range [54]; the root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) reached 0.041, which also exceeded the recommended threshold of 0.080 [55]; and the comparable fit index (CFI) and the Tucker–Lewis index (TLI) also exceeded the recommended threshold of 0.95, indicating a high level of model fit to the empirical data [54].
In addition to the exploratory factor analysis results and the value of the correlations, convergent validity was also demonstrated by the average variance extracted (AVE) and composite reliability (CR), which exceeded the recommended thresholds of 0.5 and 0.7, respectively [56,57]. In addition, the scale showed the expected (and justified) associations with each of the control indicators included in the study. The results showed, among other things, that there is a statistically significant association between the SJS and agreement with the statement that even bad laws should be complied with. This statement is one of the key elements of the convergent validity of the SJS because it is consistent with the theoretical framework, which posits that people tend to defend and justify the existing order, even when laws or structures appear imperfect or unjust.
The data also showed that people with higher SJS scores downplayed inequalities in the distribution of wealth. Again, such an interpretation is consistent with system justification theory, in that people who support and justify the current system (i.e., those with higher SJS scores) crowd out issues of inequality and fairness in distribution of wealth. Thus, the statement supports the convergent validity of the SJS scale by finding a statistically significant association between responses to this statement and the level of system justification as measured by the SJS scale.
The ANOVA results showed a statistically significant difference between the different levels of agreement with the statement about different rules for rich and poor people and the SJS score. Such a result is consistent with system justification theory, which posits that people tend to justify and legitimize the existing social order, even though it may contain inequalities and differential access to rights. Thus, the third statement documented the validity of the SJS scale by empirically confirming the expectations based on system justification theory.
Furthermore, the results showed that people’s willingness to strike was related to different levels of system justification. As expected, higher SJS scores were related to lower willingness to strike. Indeed, willingness to strike implies disagreement with some aspects of the current system or working conditions, and so it is not surprising that people who justify the current system are more likely to resist activities that inherently challenge or criticize some aspect of the current system.
The lower willingness to strike, however, cannot be equated with passivity and disinterest among people with high SJS scores. On the contrary, ANOVA results show that there is a statistically significant difference between SJS scores and commitment to engage in activities aimed at preserving the current environment. It appears that respondents with higher SJS scores consider personal involvement and efforts to preserve the environment as part of a functioning system. The results resemble the statement that people tend to justify and legitimize a system that ensures its stability by encouraging citizen participation and supporting their efforts to participate.
Given the cross-sectional nature of the research, it is important to note that it was not possible to determine the direction of association between system justification and other attitudinal indicators. For example, it is not clear whether higher levels of compliance led to higher levels of system justification or, conversely, whether higher levels of system justification led to higher levels of compliance. Clearly, both of these explanations have merit, but a different research design would be needed to clearly establish causality.
Another limitation of the research conducted stems from the fact that only a limited number of control variables were observed in the present research. It would be useful to use a wide range of behavioral questions to better describe the conditions and circumstances from which individuals form their attitudes toward the social system.
Despite these limitations, the findings may have a number of practical implications that can contribute to a better understanding of human behavior, social phenomena and processes. Understanding system justification can help to better assess people’s responses to political and social change. Knowledge of the extent to which people justify or reject the current system can be useful in designing social programs, initiatives, and campaigns aimed at social change. Knowledge of system justification can help to better understand some of the cognitive biases that may influence the way people perceive information, interpret events, and formulate opinions. Moreover, system justification can provide a useful perspective on the mechanisms that contribute to the reproduction of social inequalities. Finally, understanding the mechanisms of system justification can help identify ways to raise awareness of social problems and motivate advocacy for social justice.

5. Conclusions

Understanding attitudes related to system justification is crucial for effective responses to contemporary social and economic change. The results provide interesting and actionable insights into people’s perceptions of the current social, economic, and political order, the extent to which they tend to question fundamental social values, or the extent to which they support the status quo.
The SJS is an easy-to-use research instrument that provides valuable information about system justification; based on the research conducted and the empirically validated properties of the scale, the SJS can be recommended for use in future research. The SJS has demonstrated very good psychometric properties in the Czech environment. The establishment of baseline values for the scale and its individual items can serve as a starting point for many other comparisons. In the future, it would be advisable to conduct separate research that would examine in detail the associations of system justification with specific behavioral patterns and conduct robust concurrent validation; an experiment to establish criterion validity or a comparative analysis of different populations would also be appropriate. Specific research attention could also be focused on a meta-analysis or systematic-review study comparing system justification according to SJS scores in countries where the scale has been used.
The findings presented above are useful not only for political science researchers or public policy makers, but also for other stakeholders, as they add to the body of knowledge on system justification and provide specific insights into the attitudes of the population.

Funding

This submission was funded by Operation Program Research, Development and Education, European Structural and Investment Funds, and by the Ministry of Education, Youth and Sports of the Czech Republic, grant number/registration number CZ.02.2.69/0.0/0.0/18_054/0014660 as a part of the project “Setting the conditions and the environment for international and cross-sector cooperation”.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki of 1975 (https://www.wma.net/what-we-do/medical-ethics/declaration-of-helsinki/) and follows the Ethical code of AAPOR (https://www.aapor.org/Standards-Ethics/AAPOR-Code-of-Ethics.aspx, accessed on 21 August 2023). The research design as well as the research instrument (the questionnaire) were approved in INESAN by the Research Ethics Board (IREBA/2021/1134). The institute holds the HRS4R HR Excellence in Research award (https://inesan.eu/en/hrs4r-2/) which acknowledges the high standard of ethics maintained by researchers at this institute (https://www.euraxess.cz/jobs/hrs4r). All links were accessed on 21 August 2023.

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study. This study involved data collected from anonymous respondents. All subjects gave their informed consent for inclusion before their participation in the survey.

Data Availability Statement

The data used to support the findings of this study will be available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.

Acknowledgments

The author thanks all interviewers engaged in this study and to all members of the supportive research team.

Conflicts of Interest

The author declares no conflict of interest. The funders had no role in the design of the study; in the collection, analyses, or interpretation of data; in the writing of the manuscript; or in the decision to publish the results.

References

  1. Brandt, M.J.; Kuppens, T.; Spears, R.; Andrighetto, L.; Autin, F.; Babincak, P.; Badea, C.; Bae, J.; Batruch, A.; Becker, J.C.; et al. Subjective status and perceived legitimacy across countries. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 2020, 50, 921–942. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Cichocka, A.; Jost, J.T. Stripped of illusions? Exploring system justification processes in capitalist and post-communist societies. Int. J. Psychol. 2014, 49, 6–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. van der Toorn, J.; Berkics, M.; Jost, J.T. System justification, satisfaction, and perceptions of fairness and typicality at work: A cross-system comparison involving the US and Hungary. Soc. Justice Res. 2010, 23, 189–210. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Osborne, D.; Sengupta, N.K.; Sibley, C.G. System justification theory at 25: Evaluating a paradigm shift in psychology and looking towards the future. Br. J. Soc. Psychol. 2019, 58, 340–361. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Harwood-Gross, A.; Vayngrib, M.; Halperin, E. Moral Injury as a Social Phenomenon: Looking at the Unique Relationship with System Justification. J. Aggress. Maltreatment Trauma 2023, 1–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Cargile, A.C.; Kahn, A.S. System justification in communication: A study of imagined dialogue receptivity. Commun. Res. Rep. 2021, 38, 103–111. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Vesper, D.; König, C.J.; Siegel, R.; Friese, M. Is use of the general system justification scale across countries justified? Testing its measurement equivalence. Br. J. Soc. Psychol. 2022, 61, 1032–1049. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  8. Jost, J.T.; Banaji, M.R. The role of stereotyping in system-justification and the production of false consciousness. Br. J. Soc. Psychol. 1994, 33, 1–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Jost, J.T.; van der Toorn, J. System justification theory. In Handbook of Theories of Social Psychology; van Lange, P.A.M., Kruglanski, A.W., Higgins, E.T., Eds.; Sage: London, UK, 2012; Volume 2, pp. 313–343. [Google Scholar]
  10. Silva, W.A.D.; Pereira, C.R. Do people see the way things are as they should be? Measuring the individual differences in system justification. Curr. Psychol. 2022, 42, 17805–17824. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Kay, A.C.; Gaucher, D.; Peach, J.M.; Friesen, J.; Laurin, K.; Zanna, M.P.; Spencer, S.J. Inequality, discrimination, and the power of the status quo: Direct evidence for a motivation to view what is as what should be. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2009, 97, 421–434. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Jost, J.T.; Banaji, M.R.; Nosek, B.A. A decade of system justification theory: Accumulated evidence of conscious and unconscious bolstering of the status quo. Political Psychol. 2004, 25, 881–920. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. van der Toorn, J.; Tyler, T.R.; Jost, J.T. More than fair: Outcome dependence, system justification, and the perceived legitimacy of authority. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 2011, 47, 127–138. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Card, K.G.; Hepburn, K. Social position and economic system justification in Canada: Implications for advancing health equity and social justice from an exploratory study of factors shaping economic system justification. Front. Public Health 2022, 10, 3912. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Jost, J.T. A quarter century of system justification theory: Questions, answers, criticisms, and societal applications. Br. J. Soc. Psychol. 2019, 58, 263–314. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Caricati, L. Evidence of decreased system justification among extreme conservatives in non-American samples. J. Soc. Psychol. 2019, 159, 725–745. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Jost, J.T.; Glaser, J.; Kruglanski, A.W.; Sulloway, F. Political conservatism as motivated social cognition. Psychol. Bull. 2003, 129, 339–375. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Nakagoshi, M.; Inamasu, K. The role of system justification theory in support of the government under long-term conservative party dominance in Japan. Front. Psychol. 2023, 14, 909022. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Jylhä, K.M.; Akrami, N. Social dominance orientation and climate change denial: The role of dominance and system justification. Pers. Individ. Dif. 2015, 86, 108–111. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Pratto, F.; Sidanius, J.; Stallworth, L.M.; Malle, B.F. Social dominance orientation: A personality variable predicting social and political attitudes. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1994, 67, 741–763. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Carter, T.J.; Ferguson, M.J.; Hassin, R.R. Implicit nationalism as system justification: The case of the United States of America. Soc. Cogn. 2011, 29, 341–359. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Osborne, D.; Sibley, C.G. Through rose-colored glasses: System-justifying beliefs dampen the effects of relative deprivation on well-being and political mobilization. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 2013, 39, 991–1004. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Jost, J.T.; Chaikalis-Petritsis, V.; Abrams, D.; Sidanius, J.; van der Toorn, J.; Bratt, C. Why men (and women) do and don’t rebel: Effects of system justification on willingness to protest. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 2012, 38, 707–720. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. Liekefett, L.; Becker, J.C. Low system justification is associated with support for both progressive and reactionary social change. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 2022, 52, 1015–1030. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Solak, N.; Tamir, M.; Sümer, N.; Jost, J.T.; Halperin, E. Expressive suppression as an obstacle to social change: Linking system justification, emotion regulation, and collective action. Motiv. Emot. 2021, 45, 661–682. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Jost, J.T.; Hunyady, O. Antecedents and consequences of system-justifying ideologies. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2005, 14, 260–265. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Zimmerman, J.L.; Reyna, C. The meaning and role of ideology in system justification and resistance for high-and low-status people. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2013, 105, 1–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Jost, J.T.; Pelham, B.W.; Carvallo, M.R. Non-conscious forms of system justification: Implicit and behavioral preferences for higher status groups. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 2002, 38, 586–602. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Vargas-Salfate, S.; Paez, D.; Khan, S.S.; Liu, J.H.; Gil de Zúñiga, H. System justification enhances well-being: A longitudinal analysis of the palliative function of system justification in 18 countries. Br. J. Soc. Psychol. 2018, 57, 567–590. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Durante, F.; Tablante, C.B.; Fiske, S.T. Poor but warm, rich but cold (and Competent): Social classes in the stereotype content model. J. Soc. 2017, 73, 138–157. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Jost, J.T.; Kay, A.C.; Thorisdottir, H. (Eds.) Social and Psychological Bases of Ideology and System Justification; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 2009; pp. 158–181. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Kay, A.C.; Friesen, J. On social stability and social change: Understanding when system justification does and does not occur. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2011, 20, 360–364. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Kish, L. A procedure for objective respondent selection within the household. J. Am. Stat. Assoc. 1949, 44, 380–387. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Sousa, V.D.; Rojjanasrirat, W. Translation, adaptation and validation of instruments or scales for use in cross-cultural health care research: A clear and user-friendly guideline. J. Eval. Clin. Pract. 2011, 17, 268–274. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Yu, D.S.; Lee, D.T.; Woo, J. Issues and challenges of instrument translation. West. J. Nurs. Res. 2004, 26, 307–320. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Willis, G.B. Cognitive Interviewing; SAGE Publications, Inc.: Thousand Oaks, CA, USA, 2015. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Roccato, M.; Rosato, R.; Mosso, C.O.; Russo, S. Measurement properties of the system justification scale: A Rasch analysis. TPM Test. Psychom. Methodol. Appl. Psychol. 2014, 21, 267–278. [Google Scholar]
  38. Langer, M.; Vasilopoulos, P.; McAvay, H.; Jost, J.T. System justification in France: Liberté, égalité, fraternité. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 2020, 34, 185–191. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Raykov, T. Scale reliability, Cronbach’s coefficient alpha, and violations of essential tau-equivalence with fixed congeneric components. Multivar. Behav. Res. 1997, 32, 329–353. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Revelle, W.; Zinbarg, R. Coefficients alpha, beta, omega, and the GLB: Comments on Sijtma. Psychometrika 2009, 74, 145–154. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Tavakol, M.; Dennick, R. Making sense of Cronbach’s alpha. Int. J. Med. Educ. 2011, 2, 53–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Field, A. Discovering Statistics Using SPSS, 5th ed.; Sage Publications: London, UK, 2017. [Google Scholar]
  43. Comrey, A.L.; Lee, H.B. A First Course in Factor Analysis; Psychology Press: London, UK, 2013. [Google Scholar]
  44. Byrne, B.M. Structural Equation Modeling with AMOS. In Basic Concepts, Applications, and Programming, 2nd ed.; Routledge: New York, NY, USA, 2001. [Google Scholar]
  45. Remr, J. Validation of the Health Consciousness Scale among the Czech Population. Healthcare 2023, 11, 1628. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Cain, M.K.; Zhang, Z.; Yuan, K.H. Univariate and multivariate skewness and kurtosis for measuring nonnormality: Prevalence, influence and estimation. Behav. Res. Methods 2017, 49, 1716–1735. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Byrne, B.M.; Campbell, T.L. Cross-cultural comparisons and the presumption of equivalent measurement and theoretical structure: A look beneath the surface. J. Cross-Cult. Psychol. 1999, 30, 555–574. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Pett, M.A.; Lackey, N.R.; Sullivan, J.J. Making Sense of Factor Analysis; SAGE Publications, Inc.: Thousand Oaks, CA, USA, 2003. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Hogarty, K.Y.; Hines, C.V.; Kromrey, J.D.; Ferron, J.M.; Mumford, K.R. The Quality of Factor Solutions in Exploratory Factor Analysis: The Influence of Sample Size, Communality, and Overdetermination. Educ. Psychol. Meas. 2005, 65, 202–226. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Taherdoost, H. Validity and reliability of the research instrument; How to Test the Validation of a Questionnaire/Survey in a Research. Int. J. Acad. Res. Manag. 2016, 5, 28–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Pituch, K.A.; Stevens, J.P. Applied Multivariate Statistics for the Social Sciences Analyses with SAS and IBM’s SPSS, 6th ed.; Routledge Taylor & Frances Group: New York, NY, USA, 2016. [Google Scholar]
  52. Nunnally, J.C.; Bernstein, I.H. Validity. Psychom. Theory 1994, 3, 99–132. [Google Scholar]
  53. Brown, T.A. Confirmatory Factor Analysis for Applied Research, 2nd ed.; The Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 2015. [Google Scholar]
  54. Hu, L.; Bentler, P.M. Fit indices in covariance structure modeling: Sensitivity to underparameterized model misspecification. Psychol. Methods 1998, 3, 424–453. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Furr, R.M. Scale Construction and Psychometrics for Social and Personality Psychology; Sage Publications: London, UK, 2011. [Google Scholar]
  56. Schreiber, J.B.; Nora, A.; Stage, F.K.; Barlow, E.A.; King, J. Reporting structural equation modeling and confirmatory factor analysis results: A review. J. Educ. Res. 2006, 99, 323–338. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Bardhoshi, G.; Erford, B.T. Processes and procedures for estimating score reliability and precision. Meas. Eval. Couns. Dev. 2017, 50, 256–263. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. System justification scale distribution.
Figure 1. System justification scale distribution.
Ejihpe 13 00131 g001
Figure 2. Confirmatory factor analysis (SJS) of the improved model.
Figure 2. Confirmatory factor analysis (SJS) of the improved model.
Ejihpe 13 00131 g002
Table 1. Socio-demographic details of respondents.
Table 1. Socio-demographic details of respondents.
VariableTheoretical Population *Sample
(%)
GenderMale50.0%50.2%
Female50.0%49.8%
Total100.0%100.0%
Age18–29 years17.1%16.9%
30–39 years17.6%17.5%
40–49 years21.2%21.4%
50–59 years16.2%16.3%
60–79 years27.9%27.9%
Total100.0%100.0%
Size of place of residenceLess than 10,000 inhabitants46.1%46.4%
10,000 to 19,999 inhabitants9.0%9.1%
20,000 to 49,999 inhabitants13.0%12.8%
50,000 to 99,999 inhabitants9.0%9.0%
100,000 inhabitants or more22.9%22.7%
Total100.0%100.0%
* Data about the theoretical population come from the Czech statistical office.
Table 2. Distribution of the responses to control indicators.
Table 2. Distribution of the responses to control indicators.
VariableDefinitely AgreeAgreeDisagreeDefinitely DisagreenMeanSD
1Even the bad laws must be complied with.21.0%38.9%26.3%13.8%6922.670.959
2Ordinary people do not get a fair share of the national wealth.25.0%47.3%23.0%4.7%6772.920.815
3There are different rules for rich and poor people.33.9%41.2%20.5%4.4%6873.050.847
4I would strike for better living conditions.12.7%36.3%42.0%9.0%6912.470.827
5It is important for me to participate in preserving the environment in which I live.21.6%43.8%26.7%7.9%7002.790.869
Note: n differs due to uneven number of missing cases.
Table 3. Summary statistics and item analysis of the system justification scale (SJS).
Table 3. Summary statistics and item analysis of the system justification scale (SJS).
MeanSDSkewnessKurtosisITCFLh2
1In general, you find society to be fair.2.300.795−0.059−0.6440.8150.8750.77
2In general, the Czech political system operates as it should.2.340.7850.039−0.4700.7470.8300.69
3Czech society needs to be radically restructured. (R)2.740.848−0.120−0.6770.5970.7950.63
4Czechia is the best country in the world to live in.2.250.8510.210−0.5960.5740.7580.57
5Most policies serve the greater good.2.230.8020.067−0.6370.7140.6770.46
6Everyone has a fair chance at wealth.2.220.7750.006−0.6410.5740.6740.45
7Our society is getting worse every year. (R)2.780.818−0.208−0.5020.553−0.6460.42
8Society is set up so that people usually get what they deserve.2.270.8000.052−0.5740.668−0.6010.43
The whole SJS scale18.134.815−0.142−0.740
(R) = reverse coding; n = 705.
Table 4. Absolute and incremental indices (SJS).
Table 4. Absolute and incremental indices (SJS).
IndicesCritical ValuesOriginal ModelImproved Model
RMSEA (Root Mean Square Error of Approximation)<0.0800.0890.041
SRMR (Standardized Root Mean Square Residual)<1.00000.04340.0206
GFI (Goodness-of-Fit Index)>0.90.9490.989
CFI (Comparative Fit Index)>0.90.9580.994
TLI (Tucker–Lewis Index)>0.90.9410.988
NFI (Normed Fit Index)>0.90.9510.989
Table 5. Correlation matrix (SJS).
Table 5. Correlation matrix (SJS).
Item12345678
1In general, you find society to be fair.1.000
2In general, the Czech political system operates as it should.0.722 **1.000
3Czech society needs to be radically restructured. (R)–0.504 **–0.455 **1.000
4Czechia is the best country in the world to live in.0.472 **0.404 **–0.281 **1.000
5Most policies serve the greater good.0.594 **0.554 **–0.414 **0.457 **1.000
6Everyone has a fair chance at wealth.0.463 **0.437 **–0.229 **0.401 **0.472 **1.000
7Our society is getting worse every year. (R)–0.429 **–0.376 **0.443 **–0.187 **–0.357 **–0.230 **1.000
8Society is set up so that people usually get what they deserve.0.558 **0.559 **–0.293 **0.410 **0.479 **0.465 **–0.303 **1.000
Kendall’s tau_b; ** = correlation is significant at the 0.01 level (2-tailed); (R) = reverse coding.
Table 6. System justification scale (SJS) measured by the control indicators.
Table 6. System justification scale (SJS) measured by the control indicators.
Itemsn%SJS
Mean Score
SJS
Score SD
Fdfp-Value *
Even the bad laws must be complied with.definitely agree14521.0%18.735.1207.46330.000
agree26938.9%18.614.752
disagree18226.3%18.004.093
definitely disagree9613.8%16.135.208
Ordinary people do not get a fair share of the national wealth.definitely agree16925.0%16.315.22611.96430.000
agree32047.3%18.424.648
disagree15623.0%19.124.054
definitely disagree324.7%19.475.224
There are different rules for rich and poor people.definitely agree23333.9%16.385.05918.45130.000
agree28341.2%18.694.436
disagree14120.5%19.273.923
definitely disagree304.4%19.785.971
I would strike for better living conditions.definitely agree8812.7%16.535.51313.02630.000
agree25136.3%17.834.558
disagree29042.0%18.274.472
definitely disagree629.0%21.264.925
It is important for me to participate in preserving the environment in which I live.definitely agree15121.6%18.975.3034.84930.002
agree30743.8%18.354.585
disagree18726.7%17.644.372
definitely disagree557.9%16.405.459
* ANOVA.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Remr, J. Assessing System Justification in Czech Population Using the System Justification Scale. Eur. J. Investig. Health Psychol. Educ. 2023, 13, 1801-1814. https://doi.org/10.3390/ejihpe13090131

AMA Style

Remr J. Assessing System Justification in Czech Population Using the System Justification Scale. European Journal of Investigation in Health, Psychology and Education. 2023; 13(9):1801-1814. https://doi.org/10.3390/ejihpe13090131

Chicago/Turabian Style

Remr, Jiri. 2023. "Assessing System Justification in Czech Population Using the System Justification Scale" European Journal of Investigation in Health, Psychology and Education 13, no. 9: 1801-1814. https://doi.org/10.3390/ejihpe13090131

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop