Cognitive Empathy and the Dark Triad: A Literature Review
Abstract
:1. Introduction
1.1. The Dark Triad
1.2. Cognitive Empathy
1.3. Theoretical Assumptions of the Relationship between the Constructs Examined
1.4. Aim of the Review
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Research Strategy
Methods, Procedures, Synthesis and Screening Process
2.2. Description of Included Studies
2.2.1. Country of Publication of the Included Studies
2.2.2. Years of Publication of the Included Studies
2.2.3. Mean Ages of the Samples in the Included Studies
2.2.4. Gender Distributions of the Samples in the Included Studies
2.2.5. Instruments Used in the Included Studies to Measure the Two Constructs
Dark Triad
Cognitive Empathy
Uni-Dimensionality and Multidimensionality of the Instruments by Which Studies Have Measured the Dark Triad and Cognitive Empathy
3. Results
3.1. Main Results
3.1.1. Narcissism and Cognitive Empathy
3.1.2. Machiavellianism and Cognitive Empathy
3.1.3. Psychopathy and Cognitive Empathy
3.1.4. Dark Triad in General and Cognitive Empathy
3.2. Study Limitations and Risks of Biases
4. Discussion and Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Conflicts of Interest
Appendix A
Reference | Sample Size | Female Percentage in the Sample (Gender Distribution) | Mean Age/Age Distribution | Country | Dark Triad Measure | Cognitive Empathy Measure |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Wai & Tiliopoulos [45] | 139 | 76.26% | M = 19.9, SD = 4.3 Range: // (Not given information) | Australia | EQ (Empathy quotient) [72] | |
Jonason & Kroll [46] | 516 | 64.9% | M = 23.99, SD = 3.82 Range: 17–48 | Germany | DDTD [62] | IRI [28,71] |
Jonason & Krause [48] | 322 | 75.16% | M (Mean) = 24.24, SD (Standard deviation) = 7.33 Range: 17–56 | Australia | DTDD (Dark Triad Dirty Dozen) [95] | BES (Basic Empathy Scale) [73] |
Turner et al. [50] | 1035 | 66.09% | M = 19.66, SD = 3.36 Range: 18–53 | USA | ||
Erickson & Sagarin. [60] | 532 | 53.20% | M1 = 40.43, SD1 = 12.62; M2 = 27.33, SD2 = 9.68 Range1: 18–75; Range2: 18–72 | USA | DTDD [62] | IRI [28] |
Vachon & Lynam [74] | 369 | 44% | M, SD: // Range: // | USA | ||
Pajevic et al. [78] | 576 | 56.60% | M = 32.91, SD = 10.94 Range: 18–68 | Serbia | ||
Fish [80] | 136 | 83.09% | M = 26.51, SD = 12.32 “Range: 18–78 | Australia | IRI [28] | |
Kaufman et al. [81] | 670 | 52.5% | M = 36.07, SD = 11.82 Range: 19–74 | USA | SD3 [59] | CASES (Cognitive, Affective, and Somatic Empathy Scales) [77] |
Schimmenti et al. [82] | 799 | 55% | M = 35.78, SD = 10.96 Range: 18–64 | Italy | DTDD [62] | EQ [110] |
Puthillam et al. [83] | 212 | 70.75% | M = 21.70, SD = 3.41 Range: 18–33 | USA | SD3 [61] | BES [73] |
Gojković et al. [84] | 263 | 73.11% | M = 18.3; SD= 1.65 Range: // | Serbia | SD3 [61] Serbian adaptation [95] | ACME (Affective and Cognitive Measure of Empathy) [74] |
Wertag & Hanzec [85] | 115 | 70.43% | M = 31.30, SD = 7.49 Range: 18–54 | Croatia | SD3 (Short Dark Triad) [61] | |
Bloxsom et al. [86] | 262 | 100% | M = 26.65, SD = 11.65 Range: 18–71 | United Kingdom | SD3 [61] | QCAE (Questionnaire of Cognitive and Affective Empathy) [75] |
Doyle [87] | 267 | 79.4% | M = 20.59, SD = 5.40 Range: 18–52 | Canada | SD3 [61] | EQ [72] |
Kajonius & Björkman [88] | 278 | 63% | M = 29.0, SD = 11.0 Range: 16–69 | Sweden | SD3 [61] | IRI [28] |
Quan et al. [89] | 698 | 65.33% | M = 24.16, SD = 2.1 Range: // | China | SD3 [61]; Chinese version: [89]) | IRI ([28] Chinese version; [111]) |
Wertag et al. [90] | 144 | 57% | M = 22.18, SD = 2.26 Range: // | Croatia | SD3 [61] | ACME [74] |
Kowalski et al. [91] | 568 | 59.86% | M = 23.57, SD = 2.55 Range: 18–30 | Italy | DTDD [62,82] | IRI [28] |
Zirenko et al. [92] | 690 (Russian sample: 308, Azerbaijani sample: 352) | Percentage of females present in the Russian sample: 80%, Percentage of females present in the Azerbaijani sample: 74% | Russian sample: M = 32.3, SD = 11.71; Azerbaijani sample: M = 30.5 SD = 10.40 Range Russian sample: 18–80; Range Azerbaijani sample: 17–74 | Russia | DTDD [62] | QCAE [75] |
Justice, [93] | 291 | 52.9% | M: 19 Range: 18–29 | USA | DTDD [62] | IRI [28] |
Tobin [94] | 153 | 85.62% | M, SD: // Range: // | USA | SD3 [61] | BES-A (Basic Empathy Scale in Adults) [112] |
Dinić et al. [95] | 443 | 49.9% | M = 28.13, SD = 6.66 Range: 19–49 | Serbia | DTDD [62] SD3 [61] | ACME [74] |
Appendix B
Reference | Statistical Main Results | Main Results | Study Limitations | Risks of Biases |
---|---|---|---|---|
Wai & Tiliopoulos [45] | Narc. × CE = 0.18 * Primary Psych. (Primary Psychopathy) × CE = −0.16 Secondary Psych. (Secondary Psychopathy) × CE = −0.10 Mach. × CE = −0.08 * p < 0.05, two tailed. ** p < 0.01; two tailed. | All of the dark triad personalities showed a negative relationship with global empathy. In particular, they showed significant deficits in affective empathy, while with cognitive empathy there were weak positive correlations. Individuals with high levels of the dark triad appear to exhibit an empathic profile that allows them to retain the ability to read and evaluate others’ emotions, and subsequently use this sensitive information to formulate strategies by which they can acquire what they want, while their lack of affective empathy may lead them to overlook or ignore the potential harm that may be inflicted on others in the process. Narcissism was found to be positively correlated with cognitive empathy. |
|
|
Jonason & Kroll [46] | PT × Psych. = −0.19 ** PT × Mach. = −0.05 PT × Narc. = −0.04 Notes. * p < 0.05. ** p < 0.01. | Psychopathy was negatively correlated with Perspective Taking (i.e., a measure of cognitive empathy). This study recognizes the distinction between the ‘‘darker’’ and ‘‘lighter’’ traits of the Dark Triad. Although both traits (Machiavellianism and psychopathy) may be part of an exploitative social strategy (Jonason & Webster, 2012; Mealey, 1995), this study suggests the way in which each trait might take a different approach when attempting to take advantage of others. Depending on different goals, each trait may steer toward qualitatively different forms of empathy. |
|
|
Jonason & Krause [48] | Narc. (Narcissism) × CE (Cognitive Empathy) = −0.14 * Psych. (Psychopathy) × CE = −0.23 ** Mach. (Machiavellianism) × CE = −0.19 ** DT (Dark Triad) × CE = −0.22 ** |
All of the traits of the Dark Triad are associated with lower levels of cognitive empathy. Psychopathy and Machiavellianism correlate with low levels of affective empathy in contrast to narcissism. This suggests that each of the traits is accompanied by unique emotional deficiencies, but psychopathy facilitates the greatest number of emotional deficiencies. |
|
|
Turner et al. [50] | CE (EQ) × Narc. (SD3) = 0.21 CE (EQ) × Narc. (NPI) = 0.18 CE (EQ) × Narc. (GNS) = 0.20 CE (EQ) × Mach. (SD3) = 0.09 CE (EQ) × Mach. (MACH-IV) = −0.09 CE (EQ) × Mach. (MPS) = 0.09 CE (EQ) × Psych. (SD3) = −0.06 CE (EQ) × Psych. (LSRP) = −0.14 CE (EQ) × Psych. (SRP) = 0.06 CE (BES) × Narc. (SD3) = 0.00 CE (BES) × Narc. (NPI) = −0.08 CE (BES) × Narc. (GNS) = 0.00 CE (BES) × Mach. (SD3) = −0.07 CE (BES) × Mach. (MACH-IV) = −0.21 CE (BES) × Mach. (MPS) = −0.12 CE (BES) × Psych. (SD3) = −0.24 CE (BES) × Psych. (LSRP) = −0.33 CE (BES) × Psych. (SRP) = −0.20 CE (HIFDS) × Narc. (SD3) = 0.12 CE (HIFDS) × Narc. (NPI) = 0.09 CE (HIFDS) × Narc. (GNS) = 0.10 CE (HIFDS) × Mach. (SD3) = −0.01 CE (HIFDS) × Mach. (MACH-IV) = −0.17 CE (HIFDS) × Mach. (MPS) = −0.01 CE (HIFDS) × Psych. (SD3) = −0.13 CE (HIFDS) × Psych. (LSRP) = −0.19 CE (HIFDS) × Psych. (SRP) = −0.11 | Unlike affective empathy, which was negatively correlated with all three Dark Triad traits, cognitive empathy was not correlated with psychopathy while it was positively correlated with narcissism and Machiavellianism (both of which have above-average levels of cognitive empathy). The links between empathy and the Dark Triad were negative for affective empathy but positive for cognitive empathy except for psychopathy. In addition, the results suggest that people with higher levels of narcissism and Machiavellianism have the strongest cognitive empathy skills. |
|
|
Erickson & Sagarin. [60] | CE × Mach. = −0.11 * CE × Psych. = −0.32 *** CE × Narc. = −0.03 PT × Mach. = −0.24 *** PT × Psych. = −0.38 *** PT × Narc. = −0.21 *** | The objective of hypothesis H2 was to test correlations between everyday sadism and cognitive empathy, affective empathy, Dark Triad traits, and personality traits in a sample practicing BDSM when the CAST physics instructions explicitly indicated consent or nonconsent. Correlations for cognitive empathy were significant for the nonconsent condition but not significant (although in the same direction) in the consent condition. |
|
|
Vachon & Lynam. [74] | SRP Psych. Tot. × CE IRI = −0.24 SRP Psych. Total × CE BES = −0.29 SRP Psych. Tot. × CE ACME = −0.11 DD Mach. × CE IRI = −0.23 DD Mach. × CE BES = −0.19 DD Mach. × CE ACME = −0.02 DD Narc. × CE IRI = −0.08 DD Narc. × CE BES = 0.05 DD Narc. × CE ACME = 0.11 | Low empathy in the children’s literature is often associated with lack of emotionality (e.g., callous, unemotional traits), so it is possible that high empathy is an expression of emotionality. High affective empathy was associated with emotional stability rather than emotionality—since empathy is a desirable trait, correlations between high empathy scores and undesirable traits (e.g., aggression, psychopathology, etc.) may be explained by social desirability effects. However, the scores of all three ACME scores were not correlated with social desirability. |
|
|
Pajevic et al. [78] | CE × Narc. = 0.17 ** CE × Mach. = −0.16 ** CE × Psych. = −0.14 ** ** p < 0.01; two-tailed. | Although narcissism showed higher self-reported cognitive empathy, this was not confirmed by performance in the emotion recognition task, which showed a non-significant relationship. This suggests that narcissism is associated with greater confidence in one’s ability to infer emotions in others, but superior performance in the actual task was not found. However, since narcissism emerged as a negative predictor of affective empathy and was not related to emotion recognition disorder, it could be argued that narcissism is associated with an empathic profile that allows one to understand how others feel without experiencing emotional contagion, which could be advantageous in conducting an exploitative and manipulative interpersonal style. |
|
|
Fish 2018 [80] | Mach. × PT = −0.19 * Psych. × PT= −0.35 *** Grandiose Narc. (Grandiose Narcissism) × PT = −0.06 Vulnerable Narc. (Vulnerable Narcissism) × PT = −0.17 * Note. * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001. | Gender and sensitivity to the expressive behavior of others have emerged as positive predictors of emotional regard. In contrast to existing research reporting that females possess higher levels of empathy than males, the data reported by this study suggest that being male contributed to a higher consideration of users’ feelings. In addition, it is surprising that perspective taking and empathic control did not contribute significantly to the pattern, given that they are related to the basic construct of empathy. |
|
|
Kaufman et al. [81] | DT × CE = −0.14 ** DT (Partial) × CE = 0.12 ** | In the regression, affective empathy was found to be a strong independent negative predictor of the Dark Triad, while cognitive empathy was a slight but significant positive predictor and independent predictor of the Dark Triad. |
|
|
Schimmenti et al. [82] | DTDD Tot. (Total) × EQ CE = −0.02 DTDD Mach. × EQ CE = −0.07 DTDD Psych. × EQ CE = −0.23 ** DTDD Narc. × EQ CE = 0.20 * * p < 0.05; ** p < 0.01 (two tails) | Dark Triad traits were significantly associated with reduced theory of mind, alexithymic traits and low empathy. |
|
|
Puthillam et al. [83] | CE × Mach. (SD3) = −0.15 CE × Narc. (SD3) = −0.03 CE × Psych. (SD3) = −0.38 *** CE × DT (SD3) = −0.24 *** *p ≤ 0.05, ** p ≤ 0.01, *** p ≤ 0.001 | The Dark Triad negatively predicts cognitive empathy, especially with regard to psychopathy. Machiavellianism was a significant negative predictor only of affective empathy, while narcissism predicted neither. Moreover, Machiavellian individuals might use cognitive empathy to charm and manipulate others ([43]), while a low level of affective empathy might facilitate this exploitation. |
|
|
Gojković et al. [84] | CE × Mach. = 0.13 * (p = 0.040) CE × Narc. = 0.24 *** CE × Psych. = −0.05 CE × DT Tot.= 0.44 *** Notes: * p <0.05; **p <0.01; *** p <0.001; | The close connections of Machiavellianism and cognitive empathy with the SD3N-admiration axis speak to the manipulative and duplicative quality of this cluster of four traits. There is a direct link between the SD3N-admiration nodes and cognitive empathy, but with none of the indices of affective empathy. In narcissists, the presence of cognitive empathy is primarily indicative of an instrumentally refined ability to read the emotional states of others. Thus, both narcissism and Machiavellianism mask their fundamentally aversive character, as the absence of an affective response unequivocally resonates the antagonistic nature of SD3N. In their work, Vachon and Lynam [79] report that cognitive empathy is poorly associated with externalizing psychopathology. |
|
|
Wertag & Hanzec [85] | EQ28 × Mach. = −0.155 CE × Mach. = −0.030 PT30 × Mach. = −0.372 ** EQ28 × Narc. = 0.000 CE × Narc. = 0.049 PT × Narc. = −0.216 * EQ28 × Psych. = −0.410 ** CE × Psych. = −0.285 ** PT × Psych. = −0.477 ** * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01 | The correlations between EQ−28 and the Dark Triad are low/moderate and negative in nature. |
|
|
Bloxsom et al. [86] | CE × Mach. = −0.161 ** CE × Narc. = 0.058 CE × Psych. = −0.310 *** Note: * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001. | Cognitive empathy is negatively correlated with Machiavellianism and psychopathy, while affective empathy has been associated negatively with all DT traits. There is an assumption that participants self-report truthfully; however, dark personalities are prone to dishonesty and manipulation; in the case of grandiose narcissism, one’s abilities are exaggerated, which may have led to the positive relationship with cognitive empathy. Indeed, the SD3 measures grandiose narcissism (rather than vulnerable narcissism), and, as a result, participants with higher levels of this trait may have overestimated their ability to understand others or their relational security. |
|
|
Doyle [87] | CE × Mach. = −0.09 CE × Narc. = 0.15 * CE × Psych. = −0.21 ** Note: p < 0.10. * p < 0.05. ** p < 0.01. *** p < 0.001. | Machiavellianism has not been significantly associated with cognitive empathy. It is possible that individuals with higher Machiavellianism scores simply possess “normal” abilities in these domains. Narcissism, on the other hand, was found to be positively associated with cognitive empathy. The ability to assume another person’s perspective would facilitate attempts to manipulate and deceive others ([45,48];). However, it is possible that individuals with higher narcissism tend to “over-report” their cognitive empathic abilities. Because the EQ is a self-report measure, individuals with higher narcissism scores may have reported an overly favorable view of their empathic abilities. Psychopathy was negatively associated with cognitive empathy. This finding suggests that individuals with high psychopathy scores are unable to lie effectively [48,95]. It could be argued that psychopaths may have earned a reputation as liars and manipulators because of the frequency with which they enact these behaviors. However, there is only mixed evidence to support the idea that psychopathy is associated with success in these endeavors [96,97,98]. |
|
|
Kajonius & Björkman. [88] | CE (IRI PT) × Mach. = −0.23 CE (IRI PT) × Narc. = −0.18 CE (IRI PT) × Psych. = −0.33 Note. r > 0.10 was significant at p < 0.05 and r > 0.16 at p < 0.01. | The results showed that it is more the lack of empathic disposition rather than inability that characterizes dark personalities. First, the Dark Triad had a very strong relationship with trait-based empathy. Second, the Dark Triad had a weak (almost nonexistent) relationship with ability-based empathy. Third, cognitive ability explained most of the ability-based empathy. This study, by testing negative relationships with the IRI scales, confirms the general notion that the Dark Triad and dispositions to empathy are negatively correlated [40]. |
|
|
Quan et al. [89] | CE (IRI PT) × Mach. = −0.036 CE (IRI PT) × Narc. = −0.012 CE (IRI PT) × Psych. = −0.090 * Note: * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01 | The Machiavellianism score is significantly negatively correlated with the Empathic Concern and Perspective Taking scores. The narcissism score is not significantly correlated with the empathic subscales. |
|
|
Wertag et al. [90] | CE × Mach. = −0.08 CE × Narc. = 0.28 ** CE × Psych. = −0.07 Notes: * p < 0.05. ** p < 0.01. | Regarding the relationships of the dark traits with empathy, narcissism was found to be positively associated with cognitive empathy, while the other dark traits were associated with lack of affective empathy, consistent with the notion of narcissism as a “brighter” trait. |
|
|
Kowalski et al. [91] | IRI PT × DTDD Psych. = −0.35 * IRI PT × DTDD Mach. = −0.34 * IRI PT × DDDT Narc. = −0.31 * Note. Bonferroni correction applied to correlations (significant at p < 0.004). p < 0.05. p < 0.001. | None of the dark tetrad traits were found to significantly correlate with the “negative affect”. |
|
|
Zirenko et al. [92] | Mach. × PT = 0.09 Psych. × PT = 0.07 Narc. × PT = 0.09 Mach. × CE = −0.06 Psych. × CE = −0.12 Narc. × CE = 0.03 Note. * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001 | In the Azerbaijani sample, ITE showed positive associations with online simulation and cognitive empathy, and negative associations with emotion contagion. Risk propensity was positively associated with Machiavellianism and Perspective Taking in both samples, particularly with proximal responsiveness and cognitive empathy in Azerbaijan, with affective empathy and narcissism in Russia, and negatively with rationality and Emotion Contagion in both samples. In Russia and Azerbaijan, similar relationships were observed between empathy and risk propensity, rationality, and DT traits. Previous cross-cultural studies have shown that DT traits and emotional intelligence are not correlated, therefore, the observed relationship between empathy and DT traits is probably not mediated by emotional intelligence, which was not measured in this study. |
|
|
Justice, [93] | Mach. × PT = −0.08 Psych. × PT = −0.20 * Narc. × PT = −0.17 ** p < 0.01 | As expected, many of the Dark Tetrad traits correlated negatively with empathy. Machiavellianism correlated negatively with Empathic Concern and the Empathy subscale of the I7 scale. Psychopathy also correlated negatively with these two scales (Empathic Concern and I7 Empathy subscale), as well as correlated negatively with Perspective Taking. Narcissism correlates positively with some of the empathy scales; the only negative correlation found was between narcissism and Perspective Taking. As expected, psychopathy was significantly predictive of low empathic concern and predictive of low empathy on the I7. Narcissism resulted in positive predictions on all but one subscale (Perspective Taking). Machiavellianism showed no indication of being predictive of decreased empathy. |
|
|
Tobin, [94] | Mach. × CE = 0.056 Narc. × CE = 0.123 Psych. × CE = 0.204 * * Correlation is significant at the 0.05 level. | It is hypothesized that cognitive empathy and empathic ability are predictive of infrahumanization and that the Dark Triad traits are not predictive of it. No significant regression equations were found. |
|
|
Dinić et al. [95] | CE × DTDD Mach. = −0.00 CE × DTDD Psych. = −0.09 CE × DTDD Narc. = 0.06 CE × SD3 Mach. = 0.02 CE × SD3 Psych. = −0.15 ** CE × SD3 Narc. = 0.04 * p < 0.05. | The dark traits of each scale, especially psychopathy, are not related to affective empathy, but when it comes to cognitive empathy a correlation is found. |
|
|
References
- Jonason, P.K.; Lyons, M.; Bethell, E.J.; Ross, R. Different routes to limited empathy in the sexes: Examining the links between the Dark Triad and empathy. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2013, 54, 572–576. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McHoskey, J.W.; Worzel, W.; Szyarto, C. Machiavellianism and psychopathy. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1998, 74, 192–210. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Muris, P.; Merckelbach, H.; Otgaar, H.; Meijer, E. The Malevolent Side of Human Nature. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 2017, 12, 183–204. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Fluyau, D.; Mitra, P.; Jain, A.; Kailasam, V.K.; Pierre, C.G. Selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors in the treatment of depression, anxiety, and post-traumatic stress disorder in substance use disorders: A Bayesian meta-analysis. Eur. J. Clin. Pharmacol. 2022, 78, 931–942. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Miller, J.D.; Hoffman, B.J.; Gaughan, E.T.; Gentile, B.; Maples, J.; Keith Campbell, W. Grandiose and Vulnerable Narcissism: A Nomological Network Analysis. J. Personal. 2011, 79, 1013–1042. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Patrick, C.J.; Fowles, D.C.; Krueger, R.F. Triarchic conceptualization of psychopathy: Developmental origins of disinhibition, boldness, and meanness. Dev. Psychopathol. 2009, 21, 913–938. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Lynam, D.R.; Widiger, T.A. Using a General Model of Personality to Identify the Basic Elements of Psychopathy. J. Personal. Disord. 2007, 21, 160–178. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Woodworth, M.; Porter, S. In cold blood: Characteristics of criminal homicides as a function of psychopathy. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 2002, 111, 436–445. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Blackburn, R. An Empirical Classification of Psychopathic Personality. Br. J. Psychiatry 1975, 127, 456–460. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fowles, D.C. The Three Arousal Model: Implications of Gray’s Two-Factor Learning Theory for Heart Rate, Electrodermal Activity, and Psychopathy. Psychophysiology 1980, 17, 87–104. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lykken, D.T. The Antisocial Personalities; Psychology Press: New York, NY, USA, 1995; ISBN 978-0-203-76355-1. [Google Scholar]
- Porter, S. Without conscience or without active conscience? The etiology of psychopathy revisited. Aggress. Violent Behav. 1996, 1, 179–189. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jones, D.N.; Paulhus, D.L. Machiavellianism. In Handbook of Individual Differences in Social Behavior; The Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 2009; pp. 93–108. ISBN 978-1-59385-647-2. [Google Scholar]
- Petrides, K.V.; Vernon, P.A.; Schermer, J.A.; Veselka, L. Trait Emotional Intelligence and the Dark Triad Traits of Personality. Twin Res. Hum. Genet. 2011, 14, 35–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bereczkei, T. The manipulative skill: Cognitive devices and their neural correlates underlying Machiavellian’s decision making. Brain Cogn. 2015, 99, 24–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Walker, S.A.; Double, K.S.; Birney, D.P. The complicated relationship between the dark triad and emotional intelligence: A systematic review. Emot. Rev. 2021, 13, 257–274. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nagler, U.K.J.; Reiter, K.J.; Furtner, M.R.; Rauthmann, J.F. Is there a “dark intelligence”? Emotional intelligence is used by dark personalities to emotionally manipulate others. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2014, 65, 47–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Davis, M.H. Empathy: A Social Psychological Approach; Routledge: London, UK, 2018; ISBN 978-0-429-97352-9. [Google Scholar]
- Singer, T. The neuronal basis and ontogeny of empathy and mind reading: Review of literature and implications for future research. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2006, 30, 855–863. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Singer, T.; Lamm, C. The Social Neuroscience of Empathy. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 2009, 1156, 81–96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Titchener, E.B. The Experimental Psychology of Thought. In Lectures on the Experimental Psychology of the Thought-Processes; MacMillan Co.: New York, NY, USA, 1909; pp. 157–194. [Google Scholar]
- Köhler, W. Ein altes Scheinproblem. Naturwissenschaften 1929, 17, 395–401. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mead, G.H.; Morris, C.W. Mind, Self, and Society From the Standpoint of a Social Behaviorist. Philosophy 1935, 10, 493–495. [Google Scholar]
- Piaget, J. The Moral Judgment Of The Child; Routledge: London, UK, 2013; ISBN 978-1-315-00968-1. [Google Scholar]
- Stotland, E. Exploratory Investigations of Empathy11The preparation of this article and all of the initially reported studies were supported by a grant from the National Science Foundation. In Advances in Experimental Social Psychology; Berkowitz, L., Ed.; Academic Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1969; Volume 4, pp. 271–314. [Google Scholar]
- Wispé, L. The distinction between sympathy and empathy: To call forth a concept, a word is needed. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1986, 50, 314–321. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rogers, C.R. Empathic: An Unappreciated Way of Being. Couns. Psychol. 1975, 5, 2–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Davis, M.H. Measuring individual differences in empathy: Evidence for a multidimensional approach. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1983, 44, 113–126. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Feshbach, N.D. Empathy in Children: Some Theoretical and Empirical Considerations. Couns. Psychol. 1975, 5, 25–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hoffman, M.L. Interaction of affect and cognition in empathy. In Emotions, Cognition, and Behavior; Cambridge University Press: New York, NY, USA, 1985; pp. 103–131. ISBN 978-0-521-25601-8. [Google Scholar]
- Hakansson, J.; Montgomery, H. The role of action in empathy from the perspective of the empathizer and the target. Curr. Res. Soc. Psychol. 2002, 8, 50–62. [Google Scholar]
- Decety, J.; Jackson, P.L. The Functional Architecture of Human Empathy. Behav. Cogn. Neurosci. Rev. 2004, 3, 406–412. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Preston, S.D.; de Waal, F.B.M. The communication of emotions and the possibility of empathy in animals. In Altruism & Altruistic Love: Science, Philosophy, & Religion in Dialogue; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 2002; pp. 284–308. ISBN 978-0-19-514358-4. [Google Scholar]
- Albiero, P.; Ingoglia, S.; Lo Coco, A. Contributo all’adattamento italiano dell’Interpersonal Reactivity Index di Davis. Test. Psicometria Metodol. 2006, 13, 107–125. [Google Scholar]
- Eisenberg, N.; Miller, P.A. The relation of empathy to prosocial and related behaviors. Psychol. Bull. 1987, 101, 91–119. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Penner, L.A.; Dovidio, J.F.; Piliavin, J.A.; Schroeder, D.A. Prosocial Behavior: Multilevel Perspectives. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2005, 56, 365–392. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Borke, H. Interpersonal perception of young children: Egocentrism or empathy? Dev. Psychol. 1971, 5, 263–269. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hogan, R. Development of an empathy scale. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 1969, 33, 307–316. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dvash, J.; Shamay-Tsoory, S.G. Theory of Mind and Empathy as Multidimensional Constructs: Neurological Foundations. Top. Lang. Disord. 2014, 34, 282. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McIlwain, D. Bypassing empathy: A Machiavellian theory of mind and sneaky power. In Individual Differences in Theory of Mind: Implications for Typical and Atypical Development; Macquarie monographs in cognitive science; Psychology Press: New York, NY, USA, 2003; pp. 39–66. ISBN 978-1-84169-093-3. [Google Scholar]
- Miao, C.; Humphrey, R.H.; Qian, S.; Pollack, J.M. The relationship between emotional intelligence and the dark triad personality traits: A meta-analytic review. J. Res. Personal. 2019, 78, 189–197. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Petrides, K.V.; Frederickson, N.; Furnham, A. The role of trait emotional intelligence in academic performance and deviant behavior at school. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2004, 36, 277–293. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fix, R.L.; Fix, S.T. Trait psychopathy, emotional intelligence, and criminal thinking: Predicting illegal behavior among college students. Int. J. Law Psychiatry 2015, 42–43, 183–188. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kilduff, M.; Chiaburu, D.S.; Menges, J.I. Strategic use of emotional intelligence in organizational settings: Exploring the dark side. Res. Organ. Behav. 2010, 30, 129–152. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wai, M.; Tiliopoulos, N. The affective and cognitive empathic nature of the dark triad of personality. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2012, 52, 794–799. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jonason, P.K.; Kroll, C.H. A multidimensional view of the relationship between empathy and the dark triad. J. Individ. Differ. 2015, 36, 150–156. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Raskin, R.; Terry, H. A principal-components analysis of the Narcissistic Personality Inventory and further evidence of its construct validity. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1988, 54, 890–902. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jonason, P.K.; Krause, L. The emotional deficits associated with the Dark Triad traits: Cognitive empathy, affective empathy, and alexithymia. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2013, 55, 532–537. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jonason, P.K.; Webster, G.D. A protean approach to social influence: Dark Triad personalities and social influence tactics. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2012, 52, 521–526. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Turner, I.N.; Foster, J.D.; Webster, G.D. The Dark Triad’s inverse relations with cognitive and emotional empathy: High-powered tests with multiple measures. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2019, 139, 1–6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Paulhus, D.L.; Williams, K.M. The Dark Triad of personality: Narcissism, Machiavellianism, and psychopathy. J. Res. Personal. 2002, 36, 556–563. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Heym, N.; Firth, J.; Kibowski, F.; Sumich, A.; Egan, V.; Bloxsom, C.A.J. Empathy at the Heart of Darkness: Empathy Deficits That Bind the Dark Triad and Those That Mediate Indirect Relational Aggression. Front. Psychiatry 2019, 10, 95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Smith, A. Cognitive Empathy and Emotional Empathy in Human Behavior and Evolution. Psychol. Rec. 2006, 56, 3–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- van Langen, M.A.M.; Wissink, I.B.; van Vugt, E.S.; Van der Stouwe, T.; Stams, G.J.J.M. The relation between empathy and offending: A meta-analysis. Aggress. Violent Behav. 2014, 19, 179–189. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bischof-Köhler, D. The development of empathy in infants. In Infant Development: Perspectives from German-Speaking Countries; Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc: Hillsdale, NJ, USA, 1991; pp. 245–273. ISBN 978-0-8058-0666-3. [Google Scholar]
- Byrne, R.W.; Whiten, A. Toward the next generation in data quality: A new survey of primate tactical deception. Behav. Brain Sci. 1988, 11, 267–273. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Crook, J.H. The Evolution of Human Consciousness; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 1980. [Google Scholar]
- Lowe, L. Angela Davis: Reflections on Race, Class, and Gender in the USA. In The Politics of Culture in the Shadow of Capital; Duke University Press: New York, NY, USA, 1997. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- van Zonneveld, L.; Platje, E.; de Sonneville, L.; van Goozen, S.; Swaab, H. Affective empathy, cognitive empathy and social attention in children at high risk of criminal behaviour. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 2017, 58, 913–921. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Erickson, J.M.; Sagarin, B.J. The prosocial sadist? A comparison of BDSM sadism and everyday sadism. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2021, 176, 110723. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jones, D.N.; Paulhus, D.L. Introducing the short Dark Triad (SD3): A brief measure of dark personality traits. Assessment 2014, 21, 28–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jonason, P.K.; Webster, G.D. The dirty dozen: A concise measure of the dark triad. Psychol. Assess. 2010, 22, 420–432. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Christie, R. Chapter I—Why Machiavelli? In Studies in Machiavellianism; Christie, R., Geis, F.L., Eds.; Academic Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1970; pp. 1–9. ISBN 978-0-12-174450-2. [Google Scholar]
- Raskin, R.N.; Hall, C.S. A Narcissistic Personality Inventory. Psychol. Rep. 1979, 45, 590. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Paulhus, D.L.; Hemphill, J.; Hare, R. Manual for the Self-Report Psychopathy Scale (SRP-III) Multi-Health Systems; MHS: Toronto, ON, Canada, 2009. [Google Scholar]
- Levenson, M.R.; Kiehl, K.A.; Fitzpatrick, C.M. Assessing psychopathic attributes in a noninstitutionalized population. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1995, 68, 151–158. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Foster, J.D.; McCain, J.L.; Hibberts, M.F.; Brunell, A.B.; Burke Johnson, R. The Grandiose Narcissism Scale: A Global and Facet-Level Measure of Grandiose Narcissism. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2015, 73, 12–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dahling, J.J.; Whitaker, B.G.; Levy, P.E. The Development and Validation of a New Machiavellianism Scale. J. Manag. 2009, 35, 219–257. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Paulhus, D.L.; Neumann, C.S.; Hare, R.D.; Williams, K.M.; Hemphill, J.F. Self-Report Psychopathy Scale 4th Edition (SRP 4) Manual; MHS, Multi-Health Systems Incorporated: Toronto, ON, Canada, 2017. [Google Scholar]
- Pincus, A.L.; Ansell, E.B.; Pimentel, C.A.; Cain, N.M.; Wright, A.G.C.; Levy, K.N. Initial construction and validation of the Pathological Narcissism Inventory. Psychol. Assess. 2009, 21, 365–379. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Davis, M.H. Interpersonal Reactivity Index. Available online: https://psycnet.apa.org/doiLanding?doi=10.1037%2Ft01093-000 (accessed on 16 July 2023).
- Baron-Cohen, S.; Wheelwright, S. The Empathy Quotient: An Investigation of Adults with Asperger Syndrome or High Functioning Autism, and Normal Sex Differences. J. Autism. Dev. Disord. 2004, 34, 163–175. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Jolliffe, D.; Farrington, D.P. Development and validation of the Basic Empathy Scale. J. Adolesc. 2006, 29, 589–611. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Vachon, D.D.; Lynam, D.R. Fixing the Problem With Empathy: Development and Validation of the Affective and Cognitive Measure of Empathy. Assessment 2016, 23, 135–149. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Reniers, R.L.E.P.; Corcoran, R.; Drake, R.; Shryane, N.M.; Völlm, B.A. The QCAE: A Questionnaire of Cognitive and Affective Empathy. J. Personal. Assess. 2011, 93, 84–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bonino, S.; Coco, A.L.; Tani, F. Empatia. I Processi di Condivisione Delle Emozioni; Giunti Psychometrics: Firenze, Italy, 1998; ISBN 978-88-09-21286-2. [Google Scholar]
- Raine, A.; Chen, F.R. The Cognitive, Affective, and Somatic Empathy Scales (CASES) for Children. J. Clin. Child Adolesc. Psychol. 2018, 47, 24–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pajevic, M.; Vukosavljevic-Gvozden, T.; Stevanovic, N.; Neumann, C.S. The relationship between the Dark Tetrad and a two-dimensional view of empathy. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2018, 123, 125–130. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gignac, G.E.; Szodorai, E.T. Effect size guidelines for individual differences researchers. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2016, 102, 74–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fish, L.R. Exploring the Motivations Underlying the Use of Paralinguistic Digital Affordances on Facebook. Ph.D. Thesis, University Of Tasmania, Hobart, Australia, 2018. [Google Scholar]
- Kaufman, S.B.; Yaden, D.B.; Hyde, E.; Tsukayama, E. The Light vs. Dark Triad of Personality: Contrasting Two Very Different Profiles of Human Nature. Front. Psychol. 2019, 10, 467. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schimmenti, A.; Jonason, P.; Passanisi, A.; La Marca, L.; Dio, N.; Gervasi, A. Exploring the Dark Side of Personality: Emotional Awareness, Empathy, and the Dark Triad Traits in an Italian Sample. Curr. Psychol. 2019, 38, 100–109. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Puthillam, A.; Karandikar, S.; Kapoor, H. I see how you feel: How the dark triad recognizes emotions. Curr. Psychol. 2021, 40, 3966–3973. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gojković, V.; Dostanić, J.S.; Đurić, V. Structure of darkness: The Dark Triad, the ‘Dark’ Empathy and the ‘Dark’ Narcissism. Primenj. Psihol. 2022, 15, 237–268. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wertag, A.; Hanzec, I. Factor structure and psychometric properties of the Croatian short version of The Empathy Quotient. Suvremena Psihol. 2016, 19, 101–109. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bloxsom, C.A.J.; Firth, J.; Kibowski, F.; Egan, V.; Sumich, A.L.; Heym, N. Dark shadow of the self: How the dark triad and empathy impact parental and intimate adult attachment relationships in women. Forensic Sci. Int. Mind Law 2021, 2, 100045. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Doyle, L. Anti-Social Cognition: Exploring the Relationships Between the Dark Triad, Empathy, and Theory of Mind. Master’s Thesis, Trent University, Peterborough, ON, Canada, 2020. [Google Scholar]
- Kajonius, P.J.; Björkman, T. Individuals with dark traits have the ability but not the disposition to empathize. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2020, 155, 109716. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Quan, R.; Guo, W.; He, L.; Pan, R.; Wang, Q.; Zhang, R.; Fan, Y.; Zhang, M.; Gong, P. The BDNF Val66Met modulates the Dark Triad: Empathic concern and aggression as mediators. Aust. J. Psychol. 2021, 73, 338–347. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wertag, A.; Ribar, M.; Sučić, I. Empathy of the devil: Sex differences in the Dark Tetrad and empathy. Empir. Stud. Psychol. 2021, 42–45. [Google Scholar]
- Kowalski, C.M.; Di Pierro, R.; Plouffe, R.A.; Rogoza, R.; Saklofske, D.H. Enthusiastic Acts of Evil: The Assessment of Sadistic Personality in Polish and Italian Populations. J. Personal. Assess. 2020, 102, 770–780. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Zirenko, M.; Kornilova, T.; Qiuqi, Z.; Izmailova, A. Personality regulation of decisions on physical distancing: Cross-cultural comparison (Russia, Azerbaijan, China). Personal. Individ. Differ. 2021, 170, 110418. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Justice, A. The Relationship of Empathy and Impulsivity to The Dark Tetrad of Personality. Master’s Thesis, Abilene Christian University, Abilene, TX, USA, 2016. [Google Scholar]
- Tobin, C. Measurements of Emotional Attributional Biases: A Confirmatory Study of Infrahumanization. Master’s Thesis, Montclair State University, Montclair, NJ, USA, 2017. [Google Scholar]
- Dinić, B.M.; Petrović, B.; Jonason, P.K. Serbian adaptations of the Dark Triad Dirty Dozen (DTDD) and Short Dark Triad (SD3). Personal. Individ. Differ. 2018, 134, 321–328. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Henrich, J.; Heine, S.J.; Norenzayan, A. The weirdest people in the world? Behav. Brain Sci. 2010, 33, 61–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Maples, J.L.; Lamkin, J.; Miller, J.D. A test of two brief measures of the dark triad: The dirty dozen and short dark triad. Psychol. Assess. 2014, 26, 326–331. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Luchner, A.; Tantleff-Dunn, S. Dysfunctional Empathy in Vulnerable Narcissism. N. Am. J. Psychol. 2016, 18, 597–610. [Google Scholar]
- Talwar, V. Development of Lying and Cognitive Abilities. In The Oxford Handbook of Lying; Meibauer, J., Ed.; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 2018; ISBN 978-0-19-873657-8. [Google Scholar]
- Book, A.S.; Holden, R.R.; Starzyk, K.B.; Wasylkiw, L.; Edwards, M.J. Psychopathic traits and experimentally induced deception in self-report assessment. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2006, 41, 601–608. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hare, R.D.; Forth, A.E.; Hart, S.D. The Psychopath as Prototype for Pathological Lying and Deception. In Credibility Assessment; Yuille, J.C., Ed.; Nato Science; Springer: Dordrecht, The Netherlands, 1989; pp. 25–49. ISBN 978-94-015-7856-1. [Google Scholar]
- Lee, Z.; Klaver, J.R.; Hart, S.D. Psychopathy and verbal indicators of deception in offenders. Psychol. Crime Law 2008, 14, 73–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Yu, C.-L.; Chou, T.-L. A dual route model of empathy: A neurobiological prospective. Front. Psychol. 2018, 9, 2212. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bubandt, N.; Willerslev, R. The Dark Side of Empathy: Mimesis, Deception, and the Magic of Alterity. Comp. Stud. Soc. Hist. 2015, 57, 5–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bošnjaković, J.; Radionov, T. Empathy: Concepts, Theories and Neuroscientific Basis. Alcohol. Psychiatry Res. J. Psychiatr. Res. Addict. 2018, 54, 123–150. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Young, A. Empathic Cruelty and the Origins of the Social Brain. In Critical Neuroscience; John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 2011; pp. 159–176. ISBN 978-1-4443-4335-9. [Google Scholar]
- Hanich, J.; Wagner, V.; Shah, M.; Jacobsen, T.; Menninghaus, W. Why we like to watch sad films. The pleasure of being moved in aesthetic experiences. Psychol. Aesthet. Creat. Arts 2014, 8, 130–143. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Breithaupt, F. The bad things we do because of empathy. Interdiscip. Sci. Rev. 2018, 43, 166–174. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Massa, A.A.; Eckhardt, C.I. Self-Report Psychopathy Scale (SRP). In Encyclopedia of Personality and Individual Differences; Zeigler-Hill, V., Shackelford, T.K., Eds.; Springer International Publishing: Cham, Switzerland, 2017; pp. 1–4. ISBN 978-3-319-28099-8. [Google Scholar]
- Lawrence, E.J.; Shaw, P.; Baker, D.; Baron-Cohen, S.; David, A.S. Measuring empathy: Reliability and validity of the Empathy Quotient. Psychol. Med. 2004, 34, 911–920. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chang, C.-W.; Chen, H.; Strong, C.; Lin, Y.-C.; Tsai, M.-C.; Lin, C.-Y.; Pakpour, A.; Griffiths, M. Reciprocal Relationships between Problematic Internet Use and Psychological Distress: A Nine-Month Longitudinal Study. 2021. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Carré, A.; Stefaniak, N.; D’Ambrosio, F.; Bensalah, L.; Besche-Richard, C. The Basic Empathy Scale in Adults (BES-A): Factor structure of a revised form. Psychol. Assess. 2013, 25, 679–691. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Source | Sample Size | Narcissism r Value | Machiavellianism r Value | Psychopathy r Value |
---|---|---|---|---|
Wai & Tiliopoulos [45] | 139 | 0.18 | −0.08 | −0.16 |
Jonason & Kroll [46] | 516 | −0.04 | −0.05 | −0.19 |
Jonason & Krause [48] | 322 | −0.14 | −0.19 | −0.23 |
Turner et al. [50] | 1035 | 0.21 | 0.09 | −0.06 |
Erickson & Sagarin [60] | 532 | −0.03 | −0.11 | −0.32 |
Vachon & Lynam [74] | 369 | −0.08 | −0.23 | −0.24 |
Pajevic et al. [78] | 576 | 0.17 | −0.16 | −0.14 |
Fish [80] | 136 | −0.06 | −0.19 | −0.35 |
Schimmenti et al. [82] | 799 | 0.20 | −0.07 | −0.23 |
Puthillamet al. [83] | 212 | −0.03 | −0.15 | −0.38 |
Gojković et al. [84] | 263 | 0.24 | 0.13 | −0.05 |
Wertag & Hanzec [85] | 115 | 0.04 | −0.03 | −0.28 |
Bloxsom et al. [86] | 262 | 0.05 | −0.16 | −0.31 |
Doyle [87] | 267 | 0.15 | −0.09 | −0.21 |
Kajonius & Björkman [88] | 278 | −0.18 | −0.23 | −0.33 |
Quan et al. [89] | 698 | −0.01 | −0.03 | −0.09 |
Wertag et al. [90] | 144 | 0.28 | −0.08 | −0.07 |
Kowalski et al. [91] | 568 | −0.31 | −0.34 | −0.35 |
Zirenko et al. [92] | 690 | 0.03 | −0.06 | −0.12 |
Justice [93] | 291 | −0.17 | −0.08 | −0.20 |
Tobin [94] | 153 | 0.12 | 0.05 | 0.20 |
Dinić et al. [95] | 443 | 0.06 | −0.00 | −0.09 |
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2023 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Duradoni, M.; Gursesli, M.C.; Fiorenza, M.; Donati, A.; Guazzini, A. Cognitive Empathy and the Dark Triad: A Literature Review. Eur. J. Investig. Health Psychol. Educ. 2023, 13, 2642-2680. https://doi.org/10.3390/ejihpe13110184
Duradoni M, Gursesli MC, Fiorenza M, Donati A, Guazzini A. Cognitive Empathy and the Dark Triad: A Literature Review. European Journal of Investigation in Health, Psychology and Education. 2023; 13(11):2642-2680. https://doi.org/10.3390/ejihpe13110184
Chicago/Turabian StyleDuradoni, Mirko, Mustafa Can Gursesli, Maria Fiorenza, Alessia Donati, and Andrea Guazzini. 2023. "Cognitive Empathy and the Dark Triad: A Literature Review" European Journal of Investigation in Health, Psychology and Education 13, no. 11: 2642-2680. https://doi.org/10.3390/ejihpe13110184