Next Article in Journal
How Customers Evaluate Genitalia versus Torso Sex Toys on Amazon.com: A Content Analysis of Product Reviews
Next Article in Special Issue
Impact of Confinement on Coping Strategies and Psychosocial Factors among University Students
Previous Article in Journal
Differences in Health-Related Quality of Life in Older People with and without Sarcopenia Covered by Long-Term Care Insurance
Previous Article in Special Issue
Mindfulness and Balanced Time Perspective: Predictive Model of Psychological Well-Being and Gender Differences in College Students
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Dark Triad Psychopathy Outperforms Self-Control in Predicting Antisocial Outcomes: A Structural Equation Modeling Approach

1
Faculdade de Psicologia (The Center for Research in Neuropsychology and Cognitive and Behavioral Intervention (CINEICC), Psychological Assessment and Psychometrics Laboratory), University of Coimbra, 3000-115 Coimbra, Portugal
2
Psychology Research Centre (CIP), Universidade do Algarve, 8005-139 Faro, Portugal
3
Social Psychology Ph.D. Program, University of Nevada, Reno, NV 89557, USA
4
Department of Sociology, Iowa State University, Ames, IA 50011, USA
5
William James Centre for Research, ISPA-Instituto Universitário, 1149-041 Lisbon, Portugal
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Eur. J. Investig. Health Psychol. Educ. 2022, 12(6), 549-562; https://doi.org/10.3390/ejihpe12060041
Submission received: 13 April 2022 / Revised: 20 May 2022 / Accepted: 23 May 2022 / Published: 27 May 2022
(This article belongs to the Collection Variables Related to Well-Being in Adolescence)

Abstract

:
Dark Triad traits and self-control are considered viable causal precursors to antisocial and criminal outcomes in youth. The purpose of the present study is to concurrently compare how Dark Triad traits and self-control differ in terms of predicting self-reported juvenile delinquency, CD symptoms, proactive overt aggression, and crime seriousness. The sample consisted of 567 (M = 15.91 years, SD = 0.99 years, age range = 14–18 years) Southern European youth from Portugal. Structural-equation-modelling procedures revealed that the psychopathy factor of Dark Triad traits presented the strongest significant hypothetical causal associations with the antisocial/criminal outcomes, followed by self-control. Machiavellianism and narcissism presented the lowest causal associations. Our findings indicate that psychopathy, as operationalized in the Dark Triad, concurrently surpasses self-control and the remaining factors of the Dark Triad in terms of predicting antisocial/criminal outcomes in youth. This suggests that behavioral disinhibition, or a core incapacity to regulate one’s conduct, is central for understanding delinquency and externalizing psychopathology. Comparatively, the interpersonal component of dark personality features, such as Machiavellianism and narcissism, are secondary for understanding crime.

1. Introduction

Delinquent behaviors often manifest prior to adulthood. These behaviors are generally limited to adolescence, but a minority of delinquent youth persist in antisocial or criminal behaviors throughout the life span [1,2,3]. Thus, many researchers aim to understand the social factors and personality traits that differentially predict persistent engagement in antisocial behavior. Gottfredson and Hirschi’s [4] general theory of crime proposes that a lack of self-control predisposes people towards antisocial and criminal behaviors. Other perspectives move beyond self-control disposition and articulate that specific personality features increase involvement in conduct problems. For example, personality features such as psychopathy, Machiavellianism, and narcissism have also been associated with antisocial behaviors, to varying degrees [5,6,7]. In particular, psychopathy is one of the best clinical predictors of violent-crime recidivism [8,9]. Although low self-control and Dark Triad [7] features overlap, they are distinct individual difference markers [10,11] that independently predict antisocial or criminal outcomes [12]. In this study, we investigate the relative strength of the causal associations between self-control, Dark Triad traits, and assorted antisocial outcomes.
In their general theory of crime, Gottfredson and Hirschi [4] argue that self-control can explain all delinquent and criminal behavior, and that all other associations with such behavior are spurious and are just other outcomes of low self-control. This theory postulates that self-control reflects a hedonic orientation to maximize pleasure and avoid pain. Popular theories of self-control suggest that there are four primary domains that control thoughts, emotions, impulses, and performance [13,14]. High self-control can be perceived of as the ability to adapt and fit the self with the environment, and to refrain from behaving in socially undesirable ways [15]. Therefore, low self-control, particularly in the performance domain, should be associated with engagement in socially undesirable behaviors, which would include criminal activity.
Studies on the relationship between self-control and crime have indeed found consistent significant associations [16,17,18,19]. The meta-analysis by Pratt and Cullen [17] suggests effect sizes of 0.26–0.28 in low self-control predicting crime. However, this effect size decreased when studies were longitudinal, which suggests that self-control might not be as effective in predicting persistent crime. This suggests that the link between self-control and general antisocial behavior could be attributable to conceptually similar but distinct constructs. For instance, Friehe and Schildberg-Hörisch [20] found evidence to suggest that the link between self-control and crime is due primarily to increased risk taking, rather than to engagement in antisocial behavior. However, longitudinal research on adolescents suggests that low self-control is associated with higher levels of aggressive and delinquent behavior [21]. Thus, other constructs appear to coexist with self-control in the etiology of conduct problems.

1.1. Interrelations between Self-Control and the Dark Triad

Despite Gottfredson and Hirschi’s [4] claim that other associations with crime beyond self-control are spurious, other researchers have found evidence to challenge their general theory. In particular, the Dark Triad of personality has been long associated with antisocial behavior and criminal activity [6,7]. The Dark Triad, which consists of traits that share a common core of callous manipulation, is comprised of Machiavellianism, narcissism, and psychopathy [7]. Each of these traits have been individually associated with criminal outcomes. Narcissism is characterized by increased grandiosity, a sense of entitlement, and a sensitivity to ego threat [22]. Machiavellianism is characterized by a cynical worldview, a long-term focus, and strategic flexibility [23]. Psychopathy is characterized by high dysfunctional impulsivity, behavioral disinhibition, and aggression [10,24,25,26]. With respect to criminal behavior, narcissism and Machiavellianism have been studied most often for instances of white-collar and financial crime [27,28], whereas psychopathy is highly predictive of generalized criminal behavior [29,30,31], and especially of violent crime [8,32,33,34]. All three are related to distinct types of aggression and antisocial behavior in adolescents [35], and to bullying behaviors in adults [5].
In adolescents, all three Dark Triad traits are differentially associated with antisocial behaviors. Whereas Machiavellianism was strongly associated with emotional dysregulation, it was not uniquely predictive of delinquency [35]. In the same study, both psychopathy and narcissism predicted overt aggression and delinquency. In a study on cyber-aggression in adolescents, Pabian and colleagues [36] found that only psychopathy was significantly predictive of cyber-aggression. Other research also finds support for Dark Triad traits, and especially psychopathy, in predicting delinquent behaviors and adolescent aggression [37,38,39]. Therefore, these traits present an alternate perspective on the causal precursors to antisocial and criminal behavior.
Both the Dark Triad traits and self-control are viable causal precursors to antisocial and criminal outcomes in adolescents. However, this may be due, in part, to overlap between the constructs. Psychopathy, in particular, is consistently related to low self-control and high levels of dysfunctional impulsivity [10,11]. When correlated with the Big Five traits of personality, both sets of constructs have similar associations with low conscientiousness, high extraversion, and low agreeableness [40,41].
Some research on self-control, the Dark Triad, and crime suggests that they are additively predictive. Narcissism and low self-control were both independently and interactively predictive of violence in an adult sample [42], as were psychopathy and low self-control in adolescents [43]. In a study on both substance use and criminal offending, the Dark Triad and self-control were both additively predictive of offending, but only self-control independently predicted substance use [44]. DeLisi and colleagues [45] found that, in a head-to-head test, low self-control was associated with more forms of delinquency than psychopathy and was a stronger independent predictor of chronic self-control. However, Wright and colleagues [12] found that the Dark Triad outperformed self-control in predicting violent delinquency, and that the variables significantly interacted. Thus, there is mixed evidence as to the nature of the relationship between self-control, the Dark Triad, and criminal outcomes.

1.2. Current Aim

Some theories of crime suggest that self-control is the only causal precursor to criminal behavior [4]. However, personality research suggests that the Dark Triad of personality might be an equally strong, if not stronger, predictor of antisocial and criminal outcomes. The present study aimed at comparing how Dark Triad traits and self-control concurrently predict self-reported delinquency, conduct-disorder symptoms, proactive overt aggression, and crime seriousness among Southern European male and female youth. On the basis of previous research, we hypothesized that both self-control and the measures of the Dark Triad, and particularly psychopathy, would be independent predictors of these outcomes, although we did not make specific hypotheses about the relative strength of these predictors.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants

The community sample consisted of 567 youth (M = 15.91 years, SD = 0.99 years, age range = 14–18 years), and was subcomposed of females (n = 256, M = 15.80 years, SD = 1.02, range = 14–18) and males (n = 311, M = 15.99 years, SD = 0.96, range = 14–18). When comparing females and males, no significant differences were detected in terms of age (F = 3.38, p = 0.06), socioeconomic status (U = 38,318.5, p = 0.41), or education (F = 0.63, p = 0.42). The majority of the participants were Portuguese nationals (88.4%) with approximately nine years of education, on average (M = 8.95, SD = 0.94).

2.2. Measures

Sociodemographic questionnaire. This questionnaire was elaborated with the aim of describing the participants’ characteristics, including nationality, sex, age, education, and socioeconomic status (SES) (estimated by considering the parents’ formal education and job, a method that provides three ordinal levels: high, medium, and low).

2.3. Predictors

Dirty Dozen (DD) [10]. This is a brief 12-item tridimensional measure of the Dark Triad construct of personality composed of Machiavellianism (e.g., “I have used deceit or lied to get my way”; “I have used flattery to get my way”), psychopathy (e.g., “I tend to be unconcerned with the morality of my actions”; “I tend to be callous or insensitive”), and narcissism (e.g., “I tend to want others to admire me”; “I tend to want others to pay attention to me”) factors. Items are rated on an ordinal Likert scale (ranging from Strongly Disagree to Strongly Agree). The score of each of the factors can be obtained by adding the respective items, and a total score can also be obtained. Higher scores indicate higher levels of Dark Triad traits (i.e., Machiavellianism, psychopathy, and narcissism). The version of the DD validated in Portugal among the youth population was used with a 5-point ordinal scale [46]. The internal consistency for the current study was Narcissism α = 0.88, Psychopathy α = 0.93, Machiavellianism α = 0.86, and DD total α = 0.93.
Brief Self-Control Scale (BSCS) [14]. This is a brief 13-item self-report unidimensional measure of self-control. The BSCS includes items such as “I refuse things that are bad for me”, “I am able to work effectively toward long-term goals”, and “Sometimes I can’t stop myself from doing something”. Items are rated on a 5-point Likert scale (ranging from 1 = Not at all like me to 5 = Very much like me). The total score of the BSCS can be obtained by adding the items. The appropriate items were reverse scored so that higher scores reflect lower levels of self-control. The version of the BSCS validated in Portugal among the youth population was used in the current study [47]. The internal consistency for the current study, estimated by Cronbach’s alpha (α), was BSCS α = 0.93.

2.4. Outcomes

Add Health Self-Report Delinquency (AHSRD) [48]. This is a 17-item self-report measure of juvenile delinquency, originally developed for the National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent Health (Add Health in its abbreviated form). The AHSRD taps criminal behaviors that occur during the last year before the assessment (e.g., “Take something from a store without paying for it”; “Steal something worth less than €50). Items are rated on a 4-point Likert scale (ranging from 0 = None to 3 = Five or more times). The total delinquency score can be obtained by adding the items. Higher scores indicate higher levels of self-reported criminality. The AHSRD was validated in Portugal among the youth population [48]. The internal consistency for the current study was AHSRD α = 0.93.
Conduct Disorder Screener (CDS) [49]. This is a brief 6-item self-report screener created to identify adolescents with CD. The items (e.g., “I got into fights; I skipped school”; “I got into trouble for lying or stealing”) are intended to be representative of CD diagnosis according to the APA’s (1994) DSM-IV, and they are rated on a 4-point ordinal Likert scale (ranging from 1 = Rarely or none of the time to 4 = Most or all of the time). Higher scores indicate higher levels of CD symptoms. The version of the CDS validated in Portugal among the youth population was used [50]. The internal consistency for the current study was CDS α = 0.84.
Peer Conflict Scale-20 (PCS-20) [51]. This is brief 20-item self-report four-dimensional measure that taps the different forms and functions of aggression. The PCS-20 has five proactive overt (PO) items (e.g., “I start fights to get what I want”), five proactive relational (PR) items (e.g., “I gossip about others to become popular”), five reactive overt (RO) items (e.g., “When someone hurts me, I end up getting into a fight”), and five reactive relational (RR) items (e.g., “If others make me mad, I tell their secrets”). Items are rated on a 4-point Likert scale (ranging from 0 = Not at all true to 3 = Definitely true). The score of each factor can be obtained by adding the respective items, and a total score can also be obtained by adding all the items. Higher scores indicate higher levels of aggression. The PCS-20 was validated in Portugal among the youth forensic population [52]. The internal consistency for the current study was PO α = 0.90, PR α = 0.82, RO α = 0.92, RR α = 0.84, and PCS-20 total α = 0.94.
Crime Seriousness. This variable was measured using a Portuguese version of the Delinquency Seriousness Classification Index (DSCI), originally developed by Loeber and colleagues [53]. The DSCI employs a four-level progressive ordinal sequence, with higher scores indicating higher seriousness levels of crimes committed by youth, such as violent-felony offenses.

2.5. Procedures

The Ministry of Education (ME) of Portugal was contacted in order to obtain authorization to assess the female and male participants of the present study. These participants were attending randomly selected schools in the southern regions of Portugal, including the capital Lisbon and the city of Faro in the Algarve region. Written parental authorization was previously obtained, and then the potential participants were themselves informed about the aims of our investigation and were asked to collaborate voluntarily and anonymously. Because of various motives, some youth were excluded (e.g., those who could not read Portuguese, those who were reluctant to participate). We also inspected each questionnaire visually and excluded incomplete questionnaires and suspiciously completed questionnaires (e.g., items systematically responded to in the same direction without paying attention to positive or negative wording). The rate of participation was 89%. No form of compensation was given, including monetary compensation. The measures and sociodemographic questionnaire included in the present study were administered in small groups of participants.

2.6. Data Analysis

EQS 6.4 [54] structural-equation-modeling software was used to estimate models (namely, to analyze the hypothetical causal associations of the predictors with the outcomes). We tested four models. In Model 1, Dark Triad traits and self-control causally predicted self-reported delinquency. In Model 2, Dark Triad traits and self-control causally predicted CD symptoms. In Model 3, Dark Triad traits and self-control causally predicted proactive overt aggression. In Model 4, Dark Triad traits and self-control causally predicted crime seriousness. Four items per measure with the highest loadings were selected to build the latent measurement models, and no modification indices were used to improve these models. Maximum Likelihood Robust (MLR) methods with covariance matrices were used; these methods work well when distributions are not severely non-normal (absolute skewness and kurtosis values below 3 and 10, respectively) [55]. The following goodness-of-fit indices served to evaluate the different models: Satorra–Bentler chi-square/degrees of freedom (SBχ2/df), Comparative Fit Index (CFI), Root Mean Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA), and Incremental Fit Index (IFI). The following criteria were considered for an adequate fit: SBχ2/df < 5, CFI and IFI > 0.90, RMSEA < 0.08; and for a good fit: SBχ2/df < 2, CFI and IFI > 0.95, RMSEA < 0.06 [55,56].
IBM SPSS Statistics v27 [57] software was used to examine descriptive statistics, Pearson correlations, group differences, and the reliability of the measures (i.e., Cronbach’s alpha). Pearson correlations were considered high if above 0.50, low if below 0.20, and moderate if in between. To compare the female and male groups, ANOVAs and Mann–Whitney’s U test were used. Alpha coefficient was considered adequate if above 0.70, and good if above 0.80 [55].

3. Results

Table 1 presents the Pearson correlation matrix of the measures that were used in the present study. The correlations between the measures were high (i.e., above 0.50) and statistically significant, except for the correlation between self-control and narcissism, which was moderate.
Figure 1 displays the first model. This model presented an adequate fit: SBχ2/df = 4.05, CFI = 0.93, IFI = 0.93, RMSEA = 0.07 [0.06–0.08]. Psychopathy and self-control positively and significantly predicted the self-reported delinquency outcome. However, the Machiavellianism and narcissism prediction over the self-reported delinquency did not reach statistical significance. Psychopathy presented the highest regression coefficient (β = 0.54, p < 0.001), followed by self-control (β = 0.16, p < 0.05).
Figure 2 shows the second model. This model presented an adequate fit: SBχ2/df = 3.88, CFI = 0.93, IFI = 0.93, RMSEA = 0.07 [0.06–0.08]. Machiavellianism, psychopathy, and self-control positively and significantly predicted the CD-symptoms outcome, but not narcissism. Psychopathy presented the highest regression coefficient (β = 0.53, p < 0.001), followed by self-control (β = 0.21, p < 0.05), and then Machiavellianism (β = 0.15, p < 0.05).
Figure 3 presents the third model. This model presented an adequate fit: SBχ2/df = 4.26, CFI = 0.92, IFI = 0.92, RMSEA = 0.07 [0.07–0.08]. Psychopathy and self-control positively and significantly predicted the proactive-overt-aggression outcome, but Machiavellianism and narcissism did not. Psychopathy presented the highest regression coefficient (β = 0.52, p < 0.001), followed by self-control (β = 0.15, p < 0.05).
Finally, Figure 4 shows the fourth model. This model presented an adequate fit: SBχ2/df = 4.82, CFI = 0.93, IFI = 0.93, RMSEA = 0.08 [0.08–0.09]. The four predictors positively and significantly predicted the crime seriousness. Psychopathy presented the highest regression coefficient (β = 0.40, p < 0.001), followed by self-control (β = 0.19, p < 0.01), then Machiavellianism (β = 0.15, p < 0.05), and lastly narcissism (β = 0.13, p < 0.05). Additionally, it is important to mention that the effects that are reported in the four models take into consideration the fact that the four predictors (i.e., Machiavellianism, psychopathy, narcissism, and self-control) are positively and significantly associated (p ≤ 0.001).

4. Discussion

The present study investigates the relative strength of the causal associations between the Dark Triad (i.e., psychopathy, Machiavellianism, and narcissism), low self-control, and antisocial/criminal outcomes. Dark Triad traits and low self-control are relevant constructs to the study of antisociality and criminality that present distinct individual difference markers [7,10]. During the last decade, several relevant studies have demonstrated the significant influence of both psychopathy and self-control as consistent and robust predictors of psychopathology and antisocial conduct [16,18,19,30,32,58,59]; however, studies that concurrently examine Dark Triad traits and low self-control among youth are much scarcer.
More specifically, the present study compared how the Dark Triad and self-control were associated with self-reported juvenile delinquency, CD symptoms, proactive overt aggression, and crime seriousness among Southern European youth. Our findings demonstrate that Dark Triad psychopathy presented the strongest significant causal associations with the several antisocial/criminal outcomes among the four structural equation models that were examined, followed by self-control. The Machiavellianism factor and the narcissism factor of the Dark Triad always presented the lowest causal associations, and they failed to reach statistical significance in most of the models examined. Our findings have several theoretical implications.
First, the findings of the present study are consistent with Wright and colleagues’ [12] investigation that found that the Dark Triad outperforms self-control in predicting violent delinquency. The findings also conflict with other studies that show that low self-control is associated with more forms of delinquency than psychopathy, and that only self-control independently predicts substance use, compared to the Dark Triad features [44,45]. Prior research used a regression-based framework that lacks the complexity and sequencing of variables that structural equation modeling offers. It is important to mention that the present study adds to the literature by using structural equation models to examine the data that offer more meaningful results in terms of validity, reliability, and the complex patterns of relationship analysis [60,61].
Second, the findings bear on important theoretical debates about general theories and the relative empirical strength of individual-level factors and their associations with crime. General theorists that advocate for self-control [4] or psychopathy [24,25,29] are supported by copious research findings that show that these individual-level factors are integral to conduct problems. Although these constructs have coexisted for decades, they have only relatively recently started to be integrated into criminological studies. The balance of which individual-level factor is “better” is uncertain, with some studies showing that self-control and psychopathy are coequal predictors of crime, while other studies favor one construct over the other [12,44,62,63,64,65]. We join that dialogue by showing that both psychopathy and self-control are significantly associated with various forms of externalizing conduct; however, psychopathy is the strongest predictor.
Third, in the event that psychopathy is the indispensable individual-level predictor of crime and analogous outcomes, this also suggests that the Dark Triad model is redundant, in that narcissism and a calculating, manipulative interpersonal style that is similar to Machiavellianism is already part of the disorder. Conceptually, it does not make much sense to “add on” additional dark personality features to a psychopathy condition that is replete with dark personality features. We concur with prior researchers who similarly express concern that the Dark Triad features are not sufficiently distinct [58,66], even with the more recent Dark Tetrad, which adds a sadism facet [67], and the Dark Core, which expands it even further [68].
Fourth, behavioral disinhibition, or a core incapacity to regulate one’s conduct, is central for understanding delinquency and other externalizing psychopathologies. This suggests that the presentation style in the interpersonal component of dark personality features, such as narcissism and Machiavellianism, are secondary or superfluous for understanding crime. We suspect that the egocentric features that are inherent to low self-control [4] and psychopathy pertain more to a basic drive to satisfy one’s wants and desires at the expense of or indifference to others. This egocentric drive or self-centeredness may not involve a cajoling, calculating, ingratiating style, which would support the weaker effects for narcissism and Machiavellianism. Indeed, among youth who are perpetrating the most serious forms of delinquency, and whose conduct rises to the level of clinical behavioral disorder, narcissism and calculated manipulation pale to brute antisocial force.
The limitations of the current investigation can inform future research and can serve to contextualize the findings. The study has a correlational/nonexperimental basis, and we did not experimentally investigate the real causal relationships between the variables. Moreover, it is a cross-sectional study, and, because of that, it is not possible to ascertain the temporal ordering of the effects, or to examine the long-term associations between the study variables. This is an important and challenging area of research because both self-control [69,70,71] and psychopathy [72,73,74] are moderately to highly heritable and thus have a substantial genetic etiological basis. This means that it is difficult to establish which behavioral tendencies emerge first, which influence the others, and which relate to conduct problems. We suspect that psychopathy is a more acute psychological condition relative to self-control, which is more normative. Thus, other investigators can model whether self-control deficits increase psychopathic functioning, whether these effects are reciprocal across developmental periods, and whether these constructs continue to influence the others vis-à-vis conduct problems over time.
The current findings also likely reflect shared method variance, as all outcomes are self-reported. Although meta-analytic research supports the use of self-reports for the Dark Triad [58], external data sources are always useful to compare to how participants view themselves. Additionally, our investigation was conducted among a school sample, which does not allow for the generalization of our findings to other types of samples, such as justice-involved or at-risk-for-delinquency youth. The severity and mix of self-control deficits and psychopathic features are much greater among justice-involved youth [45,75]; thus, future studies should be conducted among forensic samples (e.g., detained youth offenders, youth in confinement facilities, clinic-referred children and adolescents with significant behavioral disorders) to potentially replicate the current work. Finally, it is important to recognize that narcissism is a multifaceted construct that can have positive associations with productive prosocial conduct [76,77,78], which may explain some of the weaker effects in the current models.

5. Conclusions

One of the important aims of criminology is to influence the capacity to deter future delinquent and antisocial behavior on the basis of scientific findings that can guide the design of preventive and early intervention strategies among youth. We hope the present study contributes to the incentivization of further research on the self-control and the Dark Triad construct among Southern European youth to deter the development of conduct problems and other related psychosocial problems.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, P.P. and M.D.; methodology, P.P.; formal analysis, P.P. and J.M.; investigation, P.P.; data curation, P.P.; writing—original draft preparation, P.P., Shelby Curtis, M.D., and C.N.; writing—review and editing, P.P., S.C., M.D. and C.N.; visualization, P.P.; funding acquisition, C.N. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This work was funded by national funds through FCT—Fundação para a Ciência e a Tecnologia—as part the project CIP: Refª UID/PSI/04345/2020.

Institutional Review Board Statement

All of the procedures that were performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Declaration of Helsinki and its later amendments or ethical standards. The research protocol was approved by the Ethics Committee of the Direção-Geral de Educação (Code: 0618500001).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed written consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

Data are available upon reasonable request.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Moffitt, T.E. Adolescence-limited and life-course-persistent antisocial behavior: A developmental taxonomy. Psychol. Rev. 1993, 100, 674–701. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Moffitt, T.E.; Caspi, A. Childhood predictors differentiate life-course persistent and adolescence-limited antisocial pathways among males and females. Dev. Psychopathol. 2001, 13, 355–375. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  3. Vaughn, M.G.; Salas-Wright, C.P.; DeLisi, M.; Maynard, B.R. Violence and externalizing behavior among youth in the United States: Is there a severe 5%? Youth Violence Juv. Justice 2014, 12, 3–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Gottfredson, M.; Hirschi, T. A General Theory of Crime; Stanford University Press: Palo Alto, CA, USA, 1990. [Google Scholar]
  5. Baughman, H.M.; Dearing, S.; Giammarco, E.; Vernon, P.A. Relationships between bullying behaviours and the Dark Triad: A study with adults. Pers. Individ. Differ. 2012, 52, 571–575. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Furnham, A.; Richards, S.C.; Paulhus, D.L. The Dark Triad of personality: A 10 year review. Soc. Pers. Psychol. Compass 2013, 7, 199–216. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Paulhus, D.L.; Williams, K.M. The dark triad of personality: Narcissism, Machiavellianism, and psychopathy. J. Res. Pers. 2002, 36, 556–563. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Harris, G.T.; Rice, M.E.; Cormier, C.A. Psychopathy and violent recidivism. Law Hum. Behav. 1991, 15, 625–637. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Hemphill, J.F.; Templeman, R.; Wong, S.; Hare, R.D. Psychopathy and crime: Recidivism and criminal careers. In Psychopathy: Theory, Research and Implications for Society; Springer: Dordrecht, The Netherlands, 1998; pp. 375–399. [Google Scholar]
  10. Jonason, P.K.; Tost, J. I just cannot control myself: The Dark Triad and self-control. Pers. Individ. Differ. 2010, 49, 611–615. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Jones, D.N.; Paulhus, D.L. The role of impulsivity in the Dark Triad of personality. Pers. Individ. Differ. 2011, 51, 679–682. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Wright, J.P.; Morgan, M.A.; Almeida, P.R.; Almosaed, N.F.; Moghrabi, S.S.; Bashatah, F.S. Malevolent forces: Self-control, the Dark Triad, and crime. Youth Violence Juv. Justice 2017, 15, 191–215. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Baumeister, R.F.; Heatherton, T.F.; Tice, D.M. Losing Control: How and Why People Fail at Self-Regulation; Academic Press, Inc.: San Diego, CA, USA, 1994. [Google Scholar]
  14. Tangney, J.P.; Baumeister, R.F.; Boone, A.L. High self-control predicts good adjustment, less pathology, better grades, and interpersonal success. J. Pers. 2004, 72, 271–324. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Rothbaum, F.; Weisz, J.R.; Snyder, S.S. Changing the world and changing the self: A two-process model of perceived control. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1982, 4, 5–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. De Ridder, D.T.; Lensvelt-Mulders, G.; Finkenauer, C.; Stok, F.M.; Baumeister, R.F. Taking stock of self-control: A meta-analysis of how trait self-control relates to a wide range of behaviors. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 2012, 16, 76–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  17. Pratt, T.C.; Cullen, F.T. The empirical status of Gottfredson and Hirschi’s general theory of crime: A meta-analysis. Criminology 2000, 38, 931–964. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Tehrani, H.D.; Yamini, S. Parenting practices, self-control and anti-social behaviors: Meta-analytic structural equation modeling. J. Crim. Justice 2020, 68, 101687. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Vazsonyi, A.T.; Mikuška, J.; Kelley, E.L. It’s time: A meta-analysis on the self-control-deviance link. J. Crim. Justice 2017, 48, 48–63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Friehe, T.; Schildberg-Hörisch, H. Self-control and crime revisited: Disentangling the effect of self-control on risk taking and antisocial behavior. Int. Rev. Law Econ. 2017, 49, 23–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  21. De Kemp, R.A.; Vermulst, A.A.; Finkenauer, C.; Scholte, R.H.; Overbeek, G.; Rommes, E.W.; Engels, R.C. Self-control and early adolescent antisocial behavior: A longitudinal analysis. J. Early Adolesc. 2009, 29, 497–517. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Jones, D.N.; Paulhus, D.L. Differentiating the Dark Triad within the interpersonal circumplex. In Handbook of Interpersonal Psychology: Theory, Research, Assessment, and Therapeutic Interventions; Horowitz, L.M., Strack, S., Eds.; Wiley & Sons: New York, NY, USA, 2011; pp. 249–269. [Google Scholar]
  23. Bereczkei, T. The manipulative skill: Cognitive devices and their neural correlates underlying Machiavellian’s decision making. Brain Cogn. 2015, 99, 24–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Hare, R.D. Without Conscience: The Disturbing World of the Psychopaths among Us; The Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 1999. [Google Scholar]
  25. Hare, R.D.; Neumann, C.S. Psychopathy as a clinical and empirical construct. Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 2008, 4, 217–246. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  26. Pailing, A.; Boon, J.; Egan, V. Personality, the Dark Triad and violence. Pers. Individ. Differ. 2014, 67, 81–86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Blickle, G.; Schlegel, A.; Fassbender, P.; Klein, U. Some personality correlates of business white-collar crime. Appl. Psychol. 2006, 55, 220–233. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Murphy, P.R. Attitude, Machiavellianism and the rationalization of misreporting. Account. Organ. Soc. 2012, 37, 242–259. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. DeLisi, M. Psychopathy as Unified Theory of Crime; Palgrave Macmillan: New York, NY, USA, 2016. [Google Scholar]
  30. Geerlings, Y.; Asscher, J.J.; Stams, G.J.J.; Assink, M. The association between psychopathy and delinquency in juveniles: A three-level meta-analysis. Aggress. Violent Behav. 2020, 50, 101342. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Vaughn, M.G.; DeLisi, M. Were Wolfgang’s chronic offenders psychopaths? On the convergent validity between psychopathy and career criminality. J. Crim. Justice 2008, 36, 33–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Fox, B.; DeLisi, M. Psychopathic killers: A meta-analytic review of the psychopathy-homicide nexus. Aggress. Violent Behav. 2019, 44, 67–79. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Hawes, S.W.; Boccaccini, M.T.; Murrie, D.C. Psychopathy and the combination of psychopathy and sexual deviance as predictors of sexual recidivism: Meta-analytic findings using the Psychopathy Checklist—Revised. Psychol. Assess. 2013, 25, 233–243. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Krstic, S.; Neumann, C.S.; Roy, S.; Robertson, C.A.; Knight, R.A.; Hare, R.D. Using latent variable- and person-centered approaches to examine the role of psychopathic traits in sex offenders. Pers. Disord. 2018, 9, 207–216. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Lau, K.S.; Marsee, M.A. Exploring narcissism, psychopathy, and Machiavellianism in youth: Examination of associations with antisocial behavior and aggression. J. Child Fam. Stud. 2013, 22, 355–367. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  36. Pabian, S.; de Backer, C.J.; Vandebosch, H. Dark Triad personality traits and adolescent cyber-aggression. Pers. Individ. Differ. 2015, 75, 41–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Chabrol, H.; van Leeuwen, N.; Rodgers, R.; Séjourné, N. Contributions of psychopathic, narcissistic, Machiavellian, and sadistic personality traits to juvenile delinquency. Pers. Individ. Differ. 2009, 47, 734–739. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Klimstra, T.A.; Sijtsema, J.J.; Henrichs, J.; Cima, M. The dark triad of personality in adolescence: Psychometric properties of a concise measure and associations with adolescent adjustment from a multi-informant perspective. J. Res. Pers. 2014, 53, 84–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Muris, P.; Meesters, C.; Timmermans, A. Some youths have a gloomy side: Correlates of the dark triad personality traits in non-clinical adolescents. Child Psychiatry Hum. Dev. 2013, 44, 658–665. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  40. Jakobwitz, S.; Egan, V. The dark triad and normal personality traits. Pers. Individ. Differ. 2006, 40, 331–339. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Romero, E.; Gómez-Fraguela, A.; Luengo, A.N.; Sobral, J. The self-control construct in the general theory of crime: An investigation in terms of personality psychology. Psychol. Crime Law 2003, 9, 61–86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Larson, M.; Vaughn, M.G.; Salas-Wright, C.P.; DeLisi, M. Narcissism, low self-control, and violence among a nationally representative sample. Crim. Justice Behav. 2015, 42, 644–661. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Flexon, J.L.; Meldrum, R.C. Adolescent psychopathic traits and violent delinquency: Additive and nonadditive effects with key criminological variables. Youth Violence Juv. Justice 2013, 11, 349–369. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Flexon, J.L.; Meldrum, R.C.; Young, J.T.N.; Lehmann, P.S. Low self-control and the dark triad: Disentangling the predictive power of personality traits on young adult substance use, offending, and victimization. J. Crim. Justice 2016, 46, 159–169. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. DeLisi, M.; Tostlebe, J.; Burgason, K.; Heirigs, M.; Vaughn, M. Self-control versus psychopathy: A head-to-head test of general theories of antisociality. Youth Violence Juv. Justice 2018, 16, 53–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Pechorro, P.; Jonason, P.; Raposo, V.; Maroco, J. Dirty Dozen: A concise measure of Dark Triad traits among at-risk youths. Curr. Psychol. 2021, 40, 3522–3531. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Pechorro, P.; DeLisi, M.; Gonçalves, R.; Quintas, J.; Palma, V. The Brief Self-Control Scale and its refined version among incarcerated and community youths: Psychometrics and measurement invariance. Deviant Behav. 2019, 42, 425–442. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Pechorro, P.; Moreira, K.; Basto-Pereira, M.; Oliveira, J.P.; Ray, J. The Self-Report Delinquency scale from the National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent to Adult Health among at-risk for delinquency youths. Violence Vict. 2019, 34, 120–135. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  49. Lewinsohn, P.M.; Rohde, P.; Farrington, D.P. The OADP-CDS: A brief screener for adolescent conduct disorder. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 2000, 39, 888–895. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  50. Palma, V.; Pechorro, P.; Jesus, S.; Nunes, C. Propriedades psicométricas do Conduct Disorder Screener entre jovens portugueses em contexto forense e escolar. Psicol. Clin. 2022; submitted. [Google Scholar]
  51. Marsee, M.A.; Barry, C.T.; Childs, K.K.; Frick, P.J.; Kimonis, E.R.; Muñoz, L.C.; Lau, K.S. Assessing the forms and functions of aggression using self-report: Factor structure and invariance of the Peer Conflict Scale in youths. Psychol. Assess. 2011, 23, 792–804. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  52. Pechorro, P.; Russell, J.; Ayala-Nunes, L.; Gonçalves, R.A.; Nunes, C. The Brief Peer Conflict Scale: Psychometric properties among a sample of incarcerated male juvenile offenders. Int. J. Offender Ther. Comp. Criminol. 2018, 62, 2414–2429. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Loeber, R.; Farrington, D.P.; Stouthamer-Loeber, M.; van Kammen, W.B. Antisocial Behavior and Mental Health Problems: Explanatory Factors in Childhood and Adolescence; Psychology Press: New York, NY, USA, 1998. [Google Scholar]
  54. Bentler, P.M.; Wu, E.J. EQS 6.4 for Windows; Multivariate Software Inc: Encino, CA, USA, 2018. [Google Scholar]
  55. Blunch, N.J. Introduction to Structural Equation Modelling Using IBM SPSS Statistics and EQS; Sage: London, UK, 2016. [Google Scholar]
  56. West, S.G.; Taylor, A.B.; Wu, W. Model fit and model selection in structural equation modeling. In Handbook of Structural Equation Modeling; Hoyle, R.H., Ed.; The Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 2012; pp. 209–231. [Google Scholar]
  57. IBM Corp. IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows (Version 27); IBM Corp.: Chicago, IL, USA, 2020. [Google Scholar]
  58. Muris, P.; Merckelbach, H.; Otgaar, H.; Meijer, E. The malevolent side of human nature: A meta-analysis and critical review of the literature on the dark triad (narcissism, Machiavellianism, and psychopathy). Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 2017, 12, 183–204. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Salinas, K.Z.; Venta, A. Testing the Role of Emotion Dysregulation as a Predictor of Juvenile Recidivism. Eur. J. Investig. Health Psychol. Educ. 2021, 11, 83–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Kline, R.B. Principles and Practice of Structural Equation Modeling, 4th ed.; The Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 2016. [Google Scholar]
  61. Maroco, J. Análise de Equações Estruturais, 3rd ed.; ReportNumber Ltd.: Pero Pinheiro, Portugal, 2021. [Google Scholar]
  62. Armstrong, T.A.; Boisvert, D.; Wells, J.; Lewis, R.H.; Cooke, E.; Woeckner, M. Assessing potential overlap between self-control and psychopathy: A consideration of the Grasmick self-control scale and the Levenson self-report psychopathy scale. J. Crim. Justice 2020, 70, 101725. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Connolly, E.J.; Al-Ghamdi, M.S.; Kobeisy, A.N.; Alqurashi, F.; Schwartz, J.A.; Beaver, K.M. Identifying latent classes of antisocial behavior among youth from Saudi Arabia: An assessment of the co-occurrence between aggression, psychopathy, low self-control, and delinquent behavior. Youth Violence Juv. Justice 2017, 15, 219–239. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  64. Meldrum, R.C.; Lehmann, P.S.; Flexon, J.L. Who would ‘purge?’ Low self-control, psychopathy, and offending in the absence of legal controls. Crime Delinq. 2021, 67, 1582–1613. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Wiebe, R.P. Reconciling psychopathy and low self-control. Justice Q. 2003, 20, 297–336. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Miller, J.D.; Hyatt, C.S.; Maples-Keller, J.L.; Carter, N.T.; Lynam, D.R. Psychopathy and Machiavellianism: A distinction without a difference? J. Per. 2017, 85, 439–453. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  67. Paulhus, D.; Buckels, E.; Trapnell, P.; Jones, D. Screening for Dark Personalities: The Short Dark Tetrad (SD4). Eur. J. Psychol. Assess. 2021, 37, 208–222. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Moshagen, M.; Hilbig, B.E.; Zettler, I. The dark core of personality. Psychol. Rev. 2018, 125, 656–688. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  69. Willems, Y.E.; Dolan, C.V.; van Beijsterveldt, C.E.; de Zeeuw, E.L.; Boomsma, D.I.; Bartels, M.; Finkenauer, C. Genetic and environmental influences on self-control: Assessing self-control with the ASEBA self-control scale. Behav. Genet. 2018, 48, 135–146. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  70. Willems, Y.E.; Boesen, N.; Li, J.; Finkenauer, C.; Bartels, M. The heritability of self-control: A meta-analysis. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2019, 100, 324–334. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Wright, J.P.; Beaver, K.M. Do parents matter in creating self-control in their children? A genetically informed test of Gottfredson and Hirschi’s theory of low self-control. Criminology 2005, 43, 1169–1202. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Larsson, H.; Andershed, H.; Lichtenstein, P. A genetic factor explains most of the variation in the psychopathic personality. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 2006, 115, 221–230. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  73. Thomson, N.D. Understanding Psychopathy: The Biopsychosocial Perspective; Routledge: New York, NY, USA, 2019. [Google Scholar]
  74. Viding, E.; Blair, R.J.R.; Moffitt, T.E.; Plomin, R. Evidence for substantial genetic risk for psychopathy in 7-year-olds. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 2005, 46, 592–597. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  75. Baglivio, M.T.; Wolff, K.T.; DeLisi, M.; Jackowski, K. The role of adverse childhood experiences (ACEs) and psychopathic features on juvenile offending criminal careers to age 18. Youth Violence Juv. Justice 2020, 18, 337–364. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  76. Dębska, M.; Dębski, P.; Polechoński, J.; Rozpara, M.; Tomik, R. The Dark Triad of Personality in the Context of Health Behaviors: Ally or Enemy? Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18, 4113. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  77. Papageorgiou, K.A.; Gianniou, F.-M.; Wilson, P.; Moneta, G.B.; Bilello, D.; Clough, P.J. The bright side of dark: Exploring the positive effect of narcissism on perceived stress through mental toughness. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2019, 139, 116–124. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  78. Di Pierro, R.; Mattavelli, S.; Gallucci, M. Narcissistic Traits and Explicit Self-Esteem: The Moderating Role of Implicit Self-View. Front. Psychol. 2016, 7, 1815. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Figure 1. Dark Triad traits and low self-control predicting delinquency. Note. Machiavelli: Machiavellianism; BSCS: Brief Self-Control Scale: AHSRD: Add Health Self-Report Delinquency; * p ≤ 0.05; *** p ≤ 0.001.
Figure 1. Dark Triad traits and low self-control predicting delinquency. Note. Machiavelli: Machiavellianism; BSCS: Brief Self-Control Scale: AHSRD: Add Health Self-Report Delinquency; * p ≤ 0.05; *** p ≤ 0.001.
Ejihpe 12 00041 g001
Figure 2. Dark Triad traits and low self-control predicting CD symptoms. Note. Machiavelli: Machiavellianism; BSCS: Brief Self-Control Scale; CDS: Conduct Disorder Screener; * p ≤ 0.05; ** p ≤ 0.01; *** p ≤ 0.001.
Figure 2. Dark Triad traits and low self-control predicting CD symptoms. Note. Machiavelli: Machiavellianism; BSCS: Brief Self-Control Scale; CDS: Conduct Disorder Screener; * p ≤ 0.05; ** p ≤ 0.01; *** p ≤ 0.001.
Ejihpe 12 00041 g002
Figure 3. Dark Triad traits and low self-control predicting proactive overt aggression. Note. Machiavelli: Machiavellianism; BSCS: Brief Self-Control Scale; PCS20 PO: Brief Peer Conflict Scale 20 Proactive Overt Aggression; * p ≤ 0.05; *** p ≤ 0.001.
Figure 3. Dark Triad traits and low self-control predicting proactive overt aggression. Note. Machiavelli: Machiavellianism; BSCS: Brief Self-Control Scale; PCS20 PO: Brief Peer Conflict Scale 20 Proactive Overt Aggression; * p ≤ 0.05; *** p ≤ 0.001.
Ejihpe 12 00041 g003
Figure 4. Dark Triad traits and low self-control predicting crime seriousness. Note. Machiavelli: Machiavellianism; BSCS: Brief Self-Control Scale; DSCI: Delinquency Seriousness Classification Index; * p ≤ 0.05; ** p ≤ 0.01; *** p ≤ 0.001.
Figure 4. Dark Triad traits and low self-control predicting crime seriousness. Note. Machiavelli: Machiavellianism; BSCS: Brief Self-Control Scale; DSCI: Delinquency Seriousness Classification Index; * p ≤ 0.05; ** p ≤ 0.01; *** p ≤ 0.001.
Ejihpe 12 00041 g004
Table 1. Pearson correlation matrix.
Table 1. Pearson correlation matrix.
12345678
1 Machiavellianism-
2 Psychopathy0.71 ***-
3 Narcissism0.60 ***0.54 ***-
4 BSCS0.68 ***0.78 ***0.45 ***-
5 AHSRD0.61 ***0.77 ***0.50 ***0.68 ***-
6 CDS0.64 ***0.77 ***0.52 ***0.70 ***0.82 ***-
7 PCS-20 PO0.60 ***0.71 ***0.51 ***0.65 ***0.83 ***0.74 ***-
8 DSCI0.64 ***0.72 ***0.53 ***0.68 ***0.82 ***0.83 ***0.73 ***-
Note. BSCS: Brief Self-Control Scale; AHSRD: Add Health Self-Report Delinquency; CDS: Conduct Disorder Screener; PCS-20 PO: Brief Peer Conflict Scale Proactive Overt Aggression; DSCI: Delinquency Seriousness Classification Index; *** p < 0.001.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Pechorro, P.; Curtis, S.; DeLisi, M.; Maroco, J.; Nunes, C. Dark Triad Psychopathy Outperforms Self-Control in Predicting Antisocial Outcomes: A Structural Equation Modeling Approach. Eur. J. Investig. Health Psychol. Educ. 2022, 12, 549-562. https://doi.org/10.3390/ejihpe12060041

AMA Style

Pechorro P, Curtis S, DeLisi M, Maroco J, Nunes C. Dark Triad Psychopathy Outperforms Self-Control in Predicting Antisocial Outcomes: A Structural Equation Modeling Approach. European Journal of Investigation in Health, Psychology and Education. 2022; 12(6):549-562. https://doi.org/10.3390/ejihpe12060041

Chicago/Turabian Style

Pechorro, Pedro, Shelby Curtis, Matt DeLisi, João Maroco, and Cristina Nunes. 2022. "Dark Triad Psychopathy Outperforms Self-Control in Predicting Antisocial Outcomes: A Structural Equation Modeling Approach" European Journal of Investigation in Health, Psychology and Education 12, no. 6: 549-562. https://doi.org/10.3390/ejihpe12060041

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop