Direct and Indirect Pathways of Variation in Length of Exposure to the Majority Language, Cognitive and Language Skills in Preschoolers’ Listening Narrative Comprehension
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Multicomponent Model of Listening Narrative Comprehension
2.1. The Direct and Mediated Role of Lower-Order Cognitive Skills in Listening Narrative Comprehension
2.2. The Direct and Mediated Role of Lower-Order Linguistic Skills in Listening Narrative Comprehension
2.3. The Direct and Mediated Role of Higher-Order Cognitive Skills in Listening Narrative Comprehension
3. The Role of Linguistic Experience in Listening Narrative Comprehension
4. The Current Study
4.1. Method
4.1.1. Participants
4.1.2. Procedure
Listening Narrative Comprehension (Dependent Variable)
Lower-Order Cognitive Skills
Lower-Order Oral Language Skills
Higher-Order Cognitive Skills
Length of Exposure to Majority Language
4.2. Data Analysis Strategy
4.3. Models’ Description
5. Results
5.1. Descriptive Statistics and Preliminary Analyses
5.2. Direct and Indirect Pathways of Language Exposure to the Majority Language, Cognitive and Language Skills to Listening Narrative Comprehension
6. Discussion
6.1. The Comprehensive Multicomponent Model of Listening Narrative Comprehension in Preschool Age
6.2. The Role of Linguistic Exposure in Listening Narrative Comprehension
7. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Conflicts of Interest
Appendix A. Story and Example of Questions in Narrative Listening Comprehension Task, (TOR 3–8)
References
- Oakhill, J.; Cain, K. Children’s Comprehension Problems in Oral and Written Language: A Cognitive Perspective. Challenges in Language and Literacy; Guilford Publications: New York, NY, USA, 2007; pp. 3–40. [Google Scholar]
- Dickenson, D.K.; Snow, C.E. Interrelationships among pre-reading and oral language skills in kindergartners from two social classes. Early Child. Res. Q. 1987, 2, 1–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lepola, J.; Lynch, J.; Laakkonen, E.; Silvén, M.; Niemi, P. The role of inference making and other language skills in the development of narrative listening comprehension in 4–6-year-old children. Read. Res. Q. 2012, 47, 259–282. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Paris, A.H.; Paris, S.G. Assessing narrative comprehension in young children. Read. Res. Q. 2003, 38, 36–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Dickenson, D.K.; Tabors, P.O. Early literacy: Linkages between home, school, and literacy achievement at age five. J. Res. Child. Educ. 1991, 6, 30–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bonifacci, P.; Barbieri, M.; Tomassini, M.; Roch, M. In few words: Linguistic gap but adequate narrative structure in preschool bilingual children. J. Child Lang. 2018, 45, 120–147. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Iluz-Cohen, P.; Walters, J. Telling stories in two languages: Narratives of bilingual preschool children with typical and impaired language. Biling. Lang. Cogn. 2012, 15, 58–74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Boerma, T.; Leseman, P.; Timmermeister, M.; Wijnen, F.; Blom, E. Narrative abilities of monolingual and bilingual children with and without language impairment: Implications for clinical practice. Int. J. Lang. Commun. Disord. 2016, 51, 626–638. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Blom, E.; Boerma, T. Why do children with language impairment have difficulties with narrative macrostructure? Res. Dev. Disabil. 2016, 55, 301–311. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Justice, L.M.; Bowles, R.P.; Kaderavek, J.N.; Ukrainetz, T.A.; Eisenberg, S.L.; Gillam, R.B. The index of narrative microstructure: A clinical tool for analyzing school-age children’s narrative performances. Am. J. Speech Lang. Pathol. 2006, 15, 177–191. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Miller, J.F.; Heilmann, J.; Nockerts, A.; Iglesias, A.; Fabiano, L.; Francis, D.J. Oral language and reading in bilingual children. Learn. Disabil. Res. Pract. 2006, 21, 30–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gough, P.B.; Tunmer, W.E. Decoding, reading, and reading disability. Remedial Spec. Educ. 1986, 7, 6–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kintsch, W. Comprehension: A Paradigm for Cognition; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 1998. [Google Scholar]
- Kim, Y.S.G. Direct and mediated effects of language and cognitive skills on comprehension of oral narrative texts (listening comprehension) for children. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2016, 141, 101–120. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Florit, E.; Roch, M.; Levorato, M.C. The relationship between listening comprehension of text and sentences in preschoolers: Specific or mediated by lower and higher level components? Appl. Psycholinguist. 2013, 34, 395–415. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Florit, E.; Roch, M.; Altoè, G.; Levorato, M.C. Listening comprehension in preschoolers: The role of memory. Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 2009, 27, 935–951. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Oakhill, J.V.; Cain, K.; Bryant, P.E. The dissociation of word reading and text comprehension: Evidence from component skills. Lang. Cogn. Process. 2003, 18, 443–468. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Oakhill, J.; Hartt, J.; Samols, D. Levels of comprehension monitoring and working memory in good and poor comprehenders. Read. Writ. 2005, 18, 657–686. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Strasser, K.; del Río, F. The role of comprehension monitoring, theory of mind, and vocabulary depth in predicting story comprehension and recall of kindergarten children. Read. Res. Q. 2014, 49, 169–187. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Catts, H.W.; Fey, M.E.; Zhang, X.; Tomblin, B.J. Language basis of reading and reading disabilities: Evidence from a longitudinal investigation. Sci. Stud. Read. 1999, 3, 331–361. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kendeou, P.; Van den Broek, P.; White, M.J.; Lynch, J.S. Predicting reading comprehension in early elementary school: The independent contributions of oral language and decoding skills. J. Educ. Psychol. 2009, 101, 765–778. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Perfetti, C.A.; Landi, N.; Oakhill, J. The Acquisition of Reading Comprehension Skill. In Blackwell Handbooks of Developmental Psychology. The Science of Reading: A Handbook; Snowling, M.J., Hulme, C., Eds.; Blackwell Publishing: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 2005; pp. 227–247. [Google Scholar]
- Kim, Y.S.; Phillips, B. Cognitive correlates of listening comprehension. Read. Res. Q. 2014, 49, 269–281. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Newman, A.J.; Bavelier, D.; Corina, D.; Jezzard, P.; Neville, H.J. A critical period for right hemisphere recruitment in American Sign Language processing. Nat. Neurosci. 2002, 5, 76–80. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Takashi, O.; Matsuda, H.; Asada, T.; Aruga, M.; Hirakata, M.; Nishikawa, M.; Katoh, A.; Imabayashi, E. Functional anatomy of musical perception in musicians. Cereb. Cortex 2001, 11, 754–760. [Google Scholar]
- Jasinska, K.K.; Petitto, L.A. How age of bilingual exposure can change the neural systems for language in the developing brain: A functional near infrared spectroscopy investigation of syntactic processing in monolingual and bilingual children. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 2013, 6, 87–101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Meir, N.; Armon-Lotem, S. Independent and combined effects of socioeconomic Status (SES) and bilingualism on children’s vocabulary and verbal short-term memory. Front. Psychol. 2017, 8, 1442. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Hoff, E. The specificity of environmental influence: Socioeconomic status affects early vocabulary development via maternal speech. Child Dev. 2003, 74, 1368–1378. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Hurtado, N.; Marchman, V.A.; Fernald, A. Does input influence uptake? Links between maternal talk, processing speed and vocabulary size in Spanish-learning children. Dev. Sci. 2008, 11, F31–F39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Kovelman, I.; Baker, S.A.; Petitto, L. Age of first bilingual language exposure as a new window into bilingual reading development. Biling. Lang. Cogn. 2008, 11, 203–223. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Place, S.; Hoff, E. Properties of dual language exposure that influence 2-year-olds’ bilingual proficiency. Child Dev. 2011, 82, 1834–1849. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Song, L.; Tamis-LeMonda, C.S.; Yoshikawa, H.; Kahana-Kalman, R.; Wu, I. Language experiences and vocabulary development in Dominican and Mexican infants across the first 2 years. Dev. Psychol. 2012, 48, 1106–1123. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Florit, E.; Roch, M.; Levorato, M.C. Listening text comprehension of explicit and implicit information in preschoolers: The role of verbal and inferential skills. Discourse Process. 2011, 48, 119–138. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cain, K.; Oakhill, J.; Bryant, P. Children’s reading comprehension ability: Concurrent prediction by working memory, verbal ability, and component skills. J. Educ. Psychol. 2004, 96, 31–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Miyake, A.; Friedman, N.P. The nature and organization of individual differences in executive functions four general conclusions. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2012, 21, 8–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Baddeley, A.; Eysenck, M.W.; Anderson, M.C. Memory; Psychology Press: Hove, UK, 2009. [Google Scholar]
- Daneman, M.; Blennerhassett, A. How to assess the listening comprehension skills of prereaders. J. Educ. Psychol. 1984, 76, 1372–1381. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Diamond, A.; Lee, K. Interventions shown to aid executive function development in children 4 to 12 years old. Science 2011, 333, 959–964. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Blair, C.; Diamond, A. Biological processes in prevention and intervention: The promotion of self-regulation as a means of preventing school failure. Dev. Psychopathol. 2008, 20, 899–911.7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Duinmeijer, I.; de Jong, J.; Scheper, A. Narrative abilities, memory and attention in children with a specific language impairment. Int. J. Lang. Commun. Disord. 2012, 47, 542–555. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kendeou, P.; Bohn-Gettler, C.; White, M.J.; Van Den Broek, P. Children’s inference generation across different media. J. Res. Read. 2008, 31, 259–272. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sénéchal, M.; Ouellette, G.; Rodney, D. The misunderstood giant: On the predictive role of early vocabulary to future reading. Handbook of Early Literacy Research; Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 2006; Volume 2, pp. 173–182. [Google Scholar]
- Florit, E.; Roch, M.; Levorato, M.C. Listening text comprehension in preschoolers: A longitudinal study on the role of semantic components. Read. Writ. 2014, 27, 793–817. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ouellette, G.P. What’s meaning got to do with it: The role of vocabulary in word reading and reading comprehension. J. Educ. Psychol. 2006, 98, 554–566. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Perfetti, C. Reading ability: Lexical quality to comprehension. Sci. Stud. Read. 2007, 11, 357–383. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pizzioli, F.; Schelstraete, M.A. Real-time sentence processing in children with specific language impairment: The contribution of lexicosemantic, syntactic, and world-knowledge information. Appl. Psycholinguist. 2013, 34, 181–210. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kim, Y.S. Language and cognitive predictors of text comprehension: Evidence from multivariate analysis. Child Dev. 2015, 86, 128–144. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Young, A.; Bowers, P.G. Individual difference and text difficulty determinants of reading fluency and expressiveness. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 1995, 60, 428–454. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Parrila, R.; Kirby, J.; McQuarrie, L. Articulation rate, naming speed, verbal short-term memory, and phonological awareness: Longitudinal predictors of early reading development? Sci. Stud. Read. 2004, 8, 3–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kail, R.; Hall, L.K.; Caskey, B.J. Processing speed, exposure to print, and naming speed. App. Psycholinguist. 1999, 20, 303–314. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Spring, C.; Davis, J.M. Relations of digit naming speed with three components of reading. App. Psycholinguist. 1988, 9, 315–334. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kail, R.; Hall, L.K. Processing speed, naming speed, and reading. Dev. Psychol. 1994, 30, 949–954. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Oakhill, J. Inferential and memory skills in children’s comprehension of stories. Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 1984, 54, 31–39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tompkins, V.; Guo, Y.; Justice, L.M. Inference generation, story comprehension, and language skills in the preschool years. Read. Writ. 2013, 26, 403–429. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Oakhill, J.V.; Cain, K. The precursors of reading ability in young readers: Evidence from a four-year longitudinal study. Sci. Stud. Read. 2012, 16, 91–121. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Howlin, P.; Cohen, S.B.; Hadwin, J. Teoria della Mente e Autismo. Insegnare a Comprendere Gli Stati Psichici dell’Altro; Edizioni Erickson: Trento, Italy, 1999. [Google Scholar]
- Makdissi, H.; Boisclair, A. Interactive reading: A context for expanding the expression of causal relations in preschoolers. Writ. Lang. Lit. 2006, 9, 177–211. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Graesser, A.; Golding, J.M.; Long, D.L. Narrative representation and comprehension. In Handbook of Reading Research; Barr, R., Kamil, M.L., Mosenthal, P., Pearson, P.D., Eds.; Longman: White Plains, NY, USA, 1991; Volume II, pp. 171–205. [Google Scholar]
- Nelson-Herber, J.; Johnston, C.S. Questions and concerns about teaching narrative and expository text. In Children’s Comprehension of Text: Research Into Practice; International Reading Association: Newark, DE, USA, 1989; pp. 263–280. [Google Scholar]
- Zampini, L.; Suttora, C.; D’Odorico, L.; Zanchi, P. Sequential reasoning and listening text comprehension in preschool children. Eur. J. Dev. Psychol. 2013, 10, 563–579. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dicataldo, R.; Roch, M. Are the Effects of Variation in Quantity of Daily Bilingual Exposure and Socioeconomic Status on Language and Cognitive Abilities Independent in Preschool Children? Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 4570. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Unsworth, S. Assessing the role of current and cumulative exposure in simultaneous bilingual acquisition: The case of Dutch gender. Biling. Lang. Cogn. 2013, 16, 86–110. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Anderson, J.A.; Hawrylewicz, K.; Bialystok, E. Who is bilingual? Snapshots across the lifespan. Biling. Lang. Cogn. 2018, 23, 929–937. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pliatsikas, C.; DeLuca, V.; Voits, T. The many shades of bilingualism: Language experiences modulate adaptations in brain structure. Lang. Learn. 2020, 70.S2, 133–149. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Paradis, J.; Nicoladis, E.; Crago, M.; Genesee, F. Bilingual children’s acquisition of the past tense: A usage-based approach. J. Child Lang. 2011, 38, 554–578. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Luk, G.; Bialystok, E. Bilingualism is not a categorical variable: Interaction between language proficiency and usage. J. Cogn. Psychol. 2013, 25, 605–621. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rodina, Y. Narrative abilities of preschool bilingual Norwegian-Russian children. Int. J. Biling. 2017, 21, 617–635. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hipfner-Boucher, K.; Milburn, T.; Weitzman, E.; Greenberg, J.; Pelletier, J.; Girolametto, L. Narrative abilities in subgroups of English language learners and monolingual peers. Int. J. Biling. 2015, 19, 677–692. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fiestas, C.E.; Peña, E.D. Narrative discourse in bilingual children. Lang. Speech Hear. Serv. Sch. 2004, 35, 166–168. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pearson, B.Z. Narrative competence among monolingual and bilingual school children in Miami. In Language and Literacy in Bilingual Children; Oller, D.K., Eilers, R.E., Eds.; Multilingual Matters: Clevedon, UK, 2002; pp. 135–174. [Google Scholar]
- Hoff, E.; Core, C.; Place, S.; Rumiche, R.; Señor, M.; Parra, M. Dual language exposure and early bilingual development. J. Child Lang. 2012, 39, 1–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Pearson, B.Z. Social factors in childhood bilingualism in the United States. Appl. Psycholinguist. 2007, 28, 399–410. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Jia, G.; Chen, J.; Kim, H.; Chan, P.S.; Jeung, C. Bilingual lexical skills of school-age children with Chinese and Korean heritage languages in the United States. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 2014, 38, 350–358. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hoff, E.; Ribot, K.M. Language growth in English monolingual and Spanish-English bilingual children from 2.5 to 5 years. J. Pediatr. 2017, 190, 241–245. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Tomasello, M. Constructing a Language: A Usage-Based Theory of Language Acquisition; Harvard University Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2003. [Google Scholar]
- Uccelli, P.; Páez, M.M. Narrative and vocabulary development of bilingual children from kindergarten to first grade: Developmental changes and associations among English and Spanish skills. Lang. Speech Hear. Serv. Sch. 2007, 38, 225–236. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bialystok, E. Reshaping the mind: The benefits of bilingualism. Can. J. Exp. Psychol. 2011, 65, 229. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Carlson, S.M.; Meltzoff, A.N. Bilingual experience and executive functioning in young children. Dev. Sci. 2008, 11, 282–298. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Martin-Rhee, M.M.; Bialystok, E. The development of two types of inhibitory control in monolingual and bilingual children. Biling. Lang. Cogn. 2008, 11, 81–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Poarch, G.J.; van Hell, J.G. Executive functions and inhibitory control in multilingual children: Evidence from second-language learners, bilinguals, and trilinguals. J. Exp. Child. Psychol. 2012, 113, 535–551. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bialystok, E.; Craik, F.I.; Binns, M.A.; Ossher, L.; Freedman, M. Effects of bilingualism on the age of onset and progression of MCI and AD: Evidence from executive function tests. Neuropsychology 2014, 28, 290–304. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nicolay, A.C.; Poncelet, M. Cognitive advantage in children enrolled in a second-language immersion elementary school program for three years. Biling. Lang. Cogn. 2013, 16, 597–607. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Purić, D.; Vuksanović, J.; Chondrogianni, V. Cognitive advantages of immersion education after 1 year: Effects of amount of exposure. J. Exp. Child. Psychol. 2017, 159, 296–309. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Costa, A.; Hernández, M.; Costa-Faidella, J.; Sebastián-Gallés, N. On the bilingual advantage in conflict processing: Now you see it, now you don’t. Cognition 2009, 113, 135–149. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Levorato, M.C.; Nesi, B.; Roch, M. TOR: Test di Comprensione del Testo Orale, 3–8 Anni; Giunti OS: Florence, Italy, 2007. [Google Scholar]
- Wechsler, D. Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (WISC-IV), 4th ed.; The Psychological Corporation: San Antonio, TX, USA, 2003. [Google Scholar]
- Orsini, A.; Pezzuti, L.; Picone, L. WISC-IV: Contributo alla Taratura Italiana; Giunti OS: Florence, Italy, 2012. [Google Scholar]
- Usai, M.C.; Viterbori, P.; Gandolfi, E.; Traverso, L. FE-PS 2-6: Batteria per la Valutazione delle Funzioni Esecutive in età Prescolare; Edizioni Centro Studi Erickson: Trento, Italy, 2017. [Google Scholar]
- Usai, M.C.; Viterbori, P.; Traverso, L.; De Franchis, V. Latent structure of executive function in five-and six-year-old children: A longitudinal study. Eur. J. Dev. Psychol. 2014, 11, 447–462. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Stella, G.; Pizzoli, C.; Tressoldi, P.E. Peabody Test di Vocabolario Recettivo; Omega Edizioni: Torino, Italy, 2000. [Google Scholar]
- Rustioni, D. Prove di Valutazione della Comprensione Linguistica; Organizzazioni Speciali: Florence, Italy, 1994. [Google Scholar]
- Korkman, M.; Kirk, U.; Kemp, S. NEPSY-II Neuropsychological Assessment Battery; Pearson: Bloomington, MN, USA, 2007. [Google Scholar]
- Urgesi, C.; Campanella, F.; Fabbro, F. NEPSY-II. Contributo alla Taratura Italiana; Giunti OS: Florence, Italy, 2011. [Google Scholar]
- Gopnik, A.; Astington, J.W. Children’s understanding of representational change and its relation to the understanding of false belief and the appearance-reality distinction. Child. Dev. 1988, 59, 26–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jose, P.E. Doing Statistical Mediation and Moderation; Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 2013. [Google Scholar]
- Schermelleh-Engel, K.; Moosbrugger, H.; Müller, H. Evaluating the fit of structural equation models: Tests of significance and descriptive goodness-of-fit measures. Methods Psychol. Res. Online 2003, 8, 23–74. [Google Scholar]
- Rosseel, Y. Lavaan: An R Package for Structural Equation Modeling and More, Version 0.4-11 (BETA). Available online: http://lavaan.ugent.be (accessed on 24 December 2011).
- Cain, K.; Oakhill, J.V.; Barnes, M.A.; Bryant, P.E. Comprehension skill, inference-making ability, and their relation to knowledge. Mem. Cogn. 2001, 29, 850–859. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Silva, M.; Cain, K. The relations between lower and higher-level comprehension skills and their role in prediction of early reading comprehension. J. Educ. Psychol. 2015, 107, 321–331. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Stevens, R.J.; Meter, P.V.; Warcholak, N.D. The effects of explicitly teaching story structure to primary grade children. J. Lit. Res. 2010, 42, 159–198. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Lynch, J.S.; van den Broek, P. Understanding the glue of narrative structure: Children’s on-and off-line inferences about characters’ goals. Cogn. Dev. 2007, 22, 323–340. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Van den Broek, P.; Rapp, D.N.; Kendeou, P. Integrating memory-based and constructionist processes in accounts of reading comprehension. Discourse Process. 2005, 39, 299–316. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kail, R. Developmental change in speed of processing during childhood and adolescence. Psychol. Bull. 1991, 109, 490–501. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Casteel, M.A.; Simpson, G.B. Textual coherence and the development of inferential generation skills. J. Res. Read. 1991, 14, 116–129. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Roth, F.P.; Speece, D.L.; Cooper, D.H. A longitudinal analysis of the connection between oral language and early reading. J. Educ. Res. 2002, 95, 259–272. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Potocki, A.; Ecalle, J.; Magnan, A. Narrative comprehension skills in 5-year-old children: Correlational analysis and comprehender profiles. J. Edu. Res. 2013, 106, 14–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lynch, J.S.; Van Den Broek, P.; Kremer, K.E.; Kendeou, P.; White, M.J.; Lorch, E.P. The development of narrative comprehension and its relation to other early reading skills. Read. Psychol. 2008, 29, 327–365. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kline, R.B. Principles and Practice of Structural Equation Modeling; Guilford Publications: New York, NY, USA, 2015. [Google Scholar]
- Hogan, T.; Bridges, M.S.; Justice, L.M.; Cain, K. Increasing Higher Level Language Skills to Improve Reading Comprehension. In Focus on Exceptional Children; Love Publishing Company: Denver, CO, USA, 2011; Volume 44. [Google Scholar]
- Wise, J.C.; Sevcik, R.A.; Morris, R.D.; Lovett, M.W.; Wolf, M. The relationship among receptive and expressive vocabulary, listening comprehension, pre-reading skills, word identification skills, and reading comprehension by children with reading disabilities. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 2007, 50, 1093–1109. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Variable | Min | Max | Mean | sd | Skewness | Kurtosis |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Age (in months) | 44 | 75 | 62 | 6.8 | −0.05 | −0.78 |
Length of exposure to Italian (in months) Dependent variable | 24 | 75 | 60 | 8.8 | −1.4 | 5.2 |
TOR 3–8 standard score (M = 10; sd = 2) | 7 | 15 | 10.5 | 1.8 | −0.10 | −0.73 |
TOR 3–8 raw score (range 0–20) | 3 | 19 | 11.5 | 4.1 | −0.27 | −0.99 |
Lower-order cognitive skills | ||||||
Backward Digit span (range 0–8) | 0 | 4 | 1.45 | 1.14 | −0.17 | −1.18 |
Day & Night (range −16–16) | −11 | 14 | 1.8 | 4.4 | 0.91 | 1.9 |
DCCS (range 0–24) | 7 | 24 | 18.4 | 2.9 | −1.01 | 3.8 |
Lower-order oral language skills | ||||||
PPVT-R standard score (M = 100; sd = 15) | 53 | 118 | 82.6 | 13.4 | 0.32 | −0.34 |
PPVT-R raw score (range 0–175) | 6 | 109 | 60.3 | 23.2 | −0.27 | −0.04 |
PVCL standard score (range 0–100) | 11 | 93 | 55 | 19.2 | 0.13 | −0.73 |
Rapid -naming combined score (M = 10; sd = 3) | 4 | 16 | 9.69 | 2.8 | −0.18 | −0.56 |
Higher-order cognitive skills | ||||||
Inferential abilities (range 0–40) | 1 | 31 | 15.5 | 7.36 | −0.13 | −0.63 |
Theory of Mind (z score) | −1.7 | 1.1 | 0 | .99 | −0.30 | −0.81 |
Knowledge of story structure (range 0–36) | 2 | 30 | 14.8 | 7.06 | 0.31 | −0.90 |
1. | 2. | 3. | 4. | 5. | 6. | 7. | 8. | 9. | 10. | 11. | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1. Listening narrative comprehension (Tor 3–8) | - | 0.27 ** | 0.26 ** | 0.15 | 0.68 ** | 0.46 ** | 0.41 ** | 0.65 ** | 0.33 ** | 0.53 ** | 0.42 ** |
2. Working memory (Backward Digit span) | - | −0.19 * | 0.26 ** | 0.39 ** | 0.38 ** | 0.22 * | 0.24 * | 0.24 ** | 0.38 * | 0.36 ** | |
3. Inhibitory control (Day & Night) | - | 0.01 | −0.27 ** | −0.07 | −0.09 | −0.15 | −0.01 | −0.13 | −0.26 ** | ||
4. Attention (DCCS) | - | 0.20 * | 0.27 ** | 0.26 ** | 0.19 * | 0.11 | 0.37 ** | 0.30 ** | |||
5. Receptive Vocabulary (PPVT-R) | - | 0.50 ** | 0.48 ** | 0.71 ** | 0.45 ** | 0.45 ** | 0.53 ** | ||||
6. Syntactic knowledge (PVCL) | - | 0.26 ** | 0.40 ** | 0.39 ** | 0.36 ** | 0.25 ** | |||||
7. Rapid naming | - | 0.49 ** | 0.30 ** | 0.13 | 0.20 * | ||||||
8. Inferential Abilities | - | 0.45 ** | 0.34 ** | 0.39 ** | |||||||
9. Theory of Mind | - | 0.19 * | 0.19 * | ||||||||
10. Knowledge of Story structure | - | 0.49 ** | |||||||||
11. Length of exposure | - |
Model | χ2 (df), p | AIC | BIC | NNFI | CFI | RMSEA | SRMR | Δχ2 (Δdf) | ΔAIC |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1 | 43.66 (25), <0.01 | 6198 | 6307 | 0.89 | 0.94 | 0.082 | 0.066 | ||
2 | 27. 91 (22), 0.17 | 6188 | 6305 | 0.95 | 0.97 | 0.052 | 0.058 | M1 vs. M2 = 15.7 (3), p < 0.001 | 9.74 |
3 | 31.63 (16), <0.05 | 6204 | 6337 | 0.86 | 0.96 | 0.094 | 0.059 | M2 vs. M3 = −3.72 (6), p = 1 | −15.7 |
4 | 26.64 (13), <0.05 | 6205 | 6346 | 0.85 | 0.96 | 0.097 | 0.055 | M3 vs. M4 = 4.98 (3), p = 0.17 | −1.02 |
5 | 11.44 (10), 0.32 | 6196 | 6345 | 0.98 | 0.99 | 0.036 | 0.045 | M1 vs. M5 = 32.2 (15), p < 0.001 | 2.21 |
M2 vs. M5 = 16.4 (12), p = 0.17 | −7.53 | ||||||||
M3 vs. M5 = 20.2 (6), p < 0.001 | 8.18 | ||||||||
M4 vs. M5 = 15.1 (3), p < 0.001 | 9.19 |
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. |
© 2021 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Dicataldo, R.; Roch, M. Direct and Indirect Pathways of Variation in Length of Exposure to the Majority Language, Cognitive and Language Skills in Preschoolers’ Listening Narrative Comprehension. Children 2021, 8, 636. https://doi.org/10.3390/children8080636
Dicataldo R, Roch M. Direct and Indirect Pathways of Variation in Length of Exposure to the Majority Language, Cognitive and Language Skills in Preschoolers’ Listening Narrative Comprehension. Children. 2021; 8(8):636. https://doi.org/10.3390/children8080636
Chicago/Turabian StyleDicataldo, Raffaele, and Maja Roch. 2021. "Direct and Indirect Pathways of Variation in Length of Exposure to the Majority Language, Cognitive and Language Skills in Preschoolers’ Listening Narrative Comprehension" Children 8, no. 8: 636. https://doi.org/10.3390/children8080636
APA StyleDicataldo, R., & Roch, M. (2021). Direct and Indirect Pathways of Variation in Length of Exposure to the Majority Language, Cognitive and Language Skills in Preschoolers’ Listening Narrative Comprehension. Children, 8(8), 636. https://doi.org/10.3390/children8080636