Relationship between Executive Functions and Creativity in Children and Adolescents: A Systematic Review
Abstract
:1. Introduction
1.1. Executive Functions (EFs)
1.2. Creativity
1.3. Executive Functions (EFs), Creativity and Intelligence
1.4. The Current Review
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Search Strategy
2.2. Selection Criteria
2.3. Data Extraction
2.4. Study Selection
3. Results
3.1. Main Methodological Characteristics
3.2. Relationship between EFs and Creativity
3.3. Intelligence as a Mediating Variable
4. Discussion
4.1. Relationship between EFs and Creativity
4.2. Intelligence as a Mediating Variable
5. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Benedek, M.; Jauk, E.; Sommer, M.; Arendasy, M.; Neubauer, A.C. Intelligence, creativity, and cognitive control: The common and differential involvement of executive functions in intelligence and creativity. Intelligence 2014, 46, 73–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Beaty, R.E.; Silvia, P.J.; Nusbaum, E.C.; Jauk, E.; Benedek, M. The roles of associative and executive processes in creative cognition. Mem. Cogn. 2014, 42, 1186–1197. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bernabeu-Brotons, E.; De la Peña, C. Creativity in higher education: An exploratory study with Executive Functions and Academic Achievement. Profesorado 2021, 25, 313–330. [Google Scholar]
- Lloyd-Cox, J.; Christensen, A.P.; Silvia, P.J.; Beaty, R.E. Seeing outside the box: Salient associations disrupt visual idea generation. Psychol. Aesthet. Creat. Arts. 2020, 15, 575–583. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bott, N.; Quintin, E.M.; Saggar, M.; Kienitz, E.; Royalty, A.; Hong, D.W.C.; Liu, N.; Chien, Y.; Hawthrone, G.; Reiss, A.L. Creativity training enhances goal-directed attention and information processing. Think. Ski. Creat. 2014, 13, 120–128. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Forthmann, B.; Jendryczko, D.; Scharfen, J.; Kleinkorres, R.; Benedek, M.; Holling, H. Creative ideation, broad retrieval ability, and processing speed: A confirmatory study of nested cognitive abilities. Intelligence 2019, 75, 59–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Runco, M.A. Meta-creativity: Being creative about creativity. Creat. Res. J. 2015, 27, 295–298. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nusbaum, E.C.; Silvia, P.J. Are intelligence and creativity really so different?: Fluid intelligence, executive processes, and strategy use in divergent thinking. Intelligence 2011, 39, 36–45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Krumm, G.; Filippetti, V.A.; Kimel, E. Executive functions in School-aged Children with high and low Creativity. Psicogente 2020, 23, 54–72. [Google Scholar]
- Baggetta, P.; Alexander, P.A. Conceptualization and operationalization of executive function. Mind Brain Educ. 2016, 10, 10–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Miyake, A.; Friedman, N.P.; Emerson, M.J.; Witzki, A.H.; Howerter, A.; Wager, T.D. The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cogn. Psychol. 2000, 41, 49–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Diamond, A. Executive functions. Ann. Rev. Psychol. 2013, 64, 135–168. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Miyake, A.; Friedman, N.P. The nature and organization of individual differences in executive functions: Four general conclusions. Curr. Dir. Psychol. 2012, 21, 8–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Lonergan, A.; Doyle, C.; Cassidy, C.; MacSweeney Mahon, S.; Roche, R.A.; Boran, L.; Bramham, J. A meta-analysis of executive functioning in dyslexia with consideration of the impact of comorbid ADHD. J. Cogn. Psychol. 2019, 31, 725–749. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Filippetti, V.; Richaud, M.C. A structural equation modeling of executive functions, IQ and mathematical skills in primary students: Differential effects on number production, mental calculus and arithmetical problems. Child Neuropsychol. 2017, 23, 864–888. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gajda, A.; Karwowski, M.; Beghetto, R.A. Creativity and academic achievement: A meta-analysis. J. Educ. Psychol. 2017, 109, 269–299. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Guilford, J.P. The Nature of Human Intelligence, 1st ed.; McGraw-Hill: New York, NY, USA, 1967. [Google Scholar]
- Nami, Y.; Marsooli, H.; Ashouri, M. The relationship between creativity and academic achievement. Procedia-Soc. Behav. Sci. 2014, 114, 36–39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Rastelli, C.; Greco, A.; Finocchiaro, C. Revealing the role of divergent thinking and fluid intelligence in children’s semantic memory organization. J. Intell. 2020, 8, 43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Csikszentmihalyi, M. 16 Implications of a systems perspective for the study of creativity. In Handbook of Creativity; Sternberg, R., Ed.; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 1999; pp. 313–335. [Google Scholar]
- Handayani, S.A.; Rahayu, Y.S.; Agustini, R. Students’ creative thinking skills in biology learning: Fluency, flexibility, originality, and elaboration. J. Phys. Conf. Ser. 2021, 1747, 012040. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rhodes, M. An analysis of creativity. Phi Delta Kappan 1961, 42, 305–310. [Google Scholar]
- Treffinger, D.J.; Young, G.C.; Selby, E.C.; Shepardson, C. Assessing Creativity: A Guide for Educators. In National Research Center on the Gifted and Talented; University of Connecticut: Storrs, CT, USA, 2002. [Google Scholar]
- Rhoades, B.L.; Greenberg, M.T.; Lanza, S.T.; Blair, C. Demographic and familial predictors of early executive function development: Contribution of a person-centered perspective. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2012, 108, 638–662. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Dajani, D.R.; Uddin, L.Q. Demystifying cognitive flexibility: Implications for clinical and developmental neuroscience. Trends Neurosci. 2015, 38, 571–578. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Pan, X.; Yu, H. Different effects of cognitive shifting and intelligence on creativity. J. Creat. Behav. 2018, 52, 212–225. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Radel, R.; Davranche, K.; Fournier, M.; Dietrich, A. The role of (dis) inhibition in creativity: Decreased inhibition improves idea generation. Cognition 2015, 134, 110–120. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Sharma, S.; Babu, N. Interplay between creativity, executive function and working memory in middle-aged and older adults. Creat. Res. J. 2017, 29, 71–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- De Dreu CK, W.; Nijstad, B.A.; Baas, M.; Wolsink, I.; Roskes, M. Working memory benefits creative insight, musical improvisation, and original ideation through maintained task-focused attention. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 2012, 38, 656–669. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Benedek, M.; Könen, T.; Neubauer, A. Associative abilities underlying creativity. Psychol. Aesthet. Creat. Arts 2012, 6, 273–281. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zabelina, D.L.; Robinson, M.D.; Council, J.R.; Bresin, K. Patterning and nonpatterning in creative cognition: Insights from performance in a random number generation task. Psychol. Aesthet. Creat. Arts 2012, 6, 137–145. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Frith, E.; Elbich, D.B.; Christensen, A.P.; Rosenberg, M.D.; Chen, Q.; Kane, M.J.; Silvia, P.J.; Seli, P.; Beaty, R.E. Intelligence and creativity share a common cognitive and neural basis. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 2021, 150, 609–632. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Karwowski, M.; Dul, J.; Gralewski, J.; Jauk, E.; Jankowska, D.M.; Gajda, A.; Chruszczewski, M.H.; Benedek, M. Is creativity without intelligence possible? A necessary condition analysis. Intelligence 2016, 57, 105–117. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Silvia, P.J. Creativity and intelligence revisited: A latent variable analysis of Wallach and Kogan. Creat. Res. J. 2008, 20, 34–39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Garcia-Molina, A.; Tirapu-Ustarroz, J.; Luna-Lario, P.; Ibáñez, J.; Duque, P. Are intelligence and executive functions the same thing? Rev. Neurol. 2010, 50, 738–746. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
- Ardila, A. Is intelligence equivalent to executive functions? Psicothema 2018, 30, 159–164. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
- Frith, E.; Kane, M.J.; Welhaf, M.S.; Christensen, A.P.; Silvia, P.J.; Beaty, R.E. Keeping creativity under control: Contributions of attention control and fluid intelligence to divergent thinking. Creat. Res. J. 2021, 33, 138–157. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Frischkorn, G.T.; Schubert, A.L.; Hagemann, D. Processing speed, working memory, and executive functions: Independent or inter-related predictors of general intelligence. Intelligence 2019, 75, 95–110. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Diamond, A. Executive functions: Insights into ways to help more children thrive. Zero Three 2014, 35, 9–17. [Google Scholar]
- Cho, H.; Pemberton, C.L.; Ray, B. An exploration of the existence, value and importance of creativity education. Curr. Issues Educ. 2017, 20, 1–20. [Google Scholar]
- Gusenbauer, M.; Haddaway, N.R. Which academic search systems are suitable for systematic reviews or meta-analyses? Evaluating retrieval qualities of Google Scholar, PubMed, and 26 other resources. Res. Synth. Methods 2020, 11, 181–217. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Kuckertz, A.; Block, J. Reviewing systematic literature reviews: Ten key questions and criteria for reviewers. MRQ 2021, 71, 519–524. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Page, M.J.; McKenzie, J.E.; Bossuyt, P.M.; Boutron, I.; Hoffmann, T.C.; Mulrow, C.D.; Shamseer, L.; Tetzlaff, J.M.; Akl, E.A.; Brennan, S.E.; et al. The PRISMA 2020 statement: An updated guideline for reporting systematic reviews. Syst. Rev. 2021, 10, 105906. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Filippetti, V.; Krumm, G. A hierarchical model of cognitive flexibility in children: Extending the relationship between flexibility, creativity and academic achievement. Child Neuropsychol. 2020, 26, 770–800. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bai, H.; Leeman, P.; Moerbeek, M.; Kroesbergen, E.H.; Mulder, H. Serial order effect in divergent thinking in five-to-six-year-olds: Individual differences as related to executive functions. J. Intell. 2021, 9, 20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- De Chantal, P.L.; Markovit. The capacity to generate alternative ideas is more important than inhibition for logical reasoning in preschool-age children. Mem. Cogn. 2017, 45, 208–220. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Krumm, G.; Filippetti, V.; Gutierrez, M. The contribution of executive functions to creativity in children: What is the role of crystallized and fluid intelligence? Think. Ski. Creat. 2018, 29, 185–195. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Sánchez-Macías, I.; Rodríguez-Media, J.; Aparicio-Herguedas, J.L. Assessment on creativity and executive functions: Proposal for future school. REIFOP 2021, 24, 35–50. [Google Scholar]
- Stolte, M.; García, T.; Van Luit, J.E.H.; Oranje, B.; Kroesbergen, E.H. The contribution of executive functions in predicting mathematical creativity in typical elementary school classes: A twofold role for updating. J. Intell. 2020, 8, 26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Stolte, M.; Kroesbergen, E.H.; Van Luir, J.E.H. Inhibition, friend or foe? Cognitive inhibition as a moderator between mathematical ability and mathematical creativity in primary school students. Pers. Individ. Differ. 2019, 142, 196–201. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vaisarova, J.; Carlson, S.M. When a spoon is not a spoon: Examining the role of executive functions in young children’s divergent thinking. Trends Neurosci. Educ. 2021, 25, 100161. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Van Dijk, M.; Blom, E.; Kroesbergen, E.H.; Leseman, P.M. The influence of situational cues on children’s creativity in an Alternative Uses Task and the moderating effect of selective attention. J. Intell. 2020, 8, 37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zhao, X.; Zhang, W.; Tong, D.; Maes, J.H.R. Creative thinking and executive functions: Associations and training effects in adolescents. Psychol. Aesthet. Creat. Arts 2021, 17, 79–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chevalier, N.; Sheffield, T.D.; Nelson, J.M.; Clark, C.A.; Wiebe, S.A.; Espy, K.A. Underpinnings of the costs of flexibility in preschool children: The roles of inhibition and working memory. Dev. Neuropsychol. 2012, 37, 99–118. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Torrance, P.E. Torrance Test of Creative Thinking: Norms-Technical Manual Research Edition-Verbal Tests, Forms A and B-Figural Tests, Forms A and B; Personnel Press: Princeton, NJ, USA, 1966. [Google Scholar]
- Corbalán, F.J.; Martínez, F.; Donolo, D.; Tejerina, M.; Limiñana, R.M. CREA Inteligencia Creativa. Una Medida Cognitiva de la Creatividad; TEA Ediciones: Madrid, Spain, 2003. [Google Scholar]
- Mednick, S.A.; Mednick, M. Remote Associates Test: Examiner’s Manual; Houghton Mifflin: Boston, MA, USA, 1971. [Google Scholar]
- Wallach, M.A.; Kogan, N. Modes of Thinking in Young Children; Holt, Rinehart and Winston: Boston, MA, USA, 1965. [Google Scholar]
- Mottweiler, C.M.; Taylor, M. Elaborated role play and creativity in preschool age children. Psychol. Aesthet. Creat. Arts 2014, 8, 228–277. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Roskos-Ewoldsen, B.; Black, S.R.; McCown, S.M. Age-related changes in creative thinking. J. Creat. Behav. 2008, 42, 33–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Carlson, S.M.; Zelazo, P.D. Minnesota Executive Function Scale: Test Manual; Reflection Sciences: Saint Paul, MI, USA, 2014. [Google Scholar]
- Heaton, R.K.; Chelune, G.J.; Talley, J.L.; Kay, G.G.; Curtiss, G. Wisconsin Card Sorting Test Manual: Revised and Expanded; Psychological Assessment Resources: Lutz, FL, USA, 1993. [Google Scholar]
- Regard, M.; Strauss, E.; Knapp, P. Children’s production on verbal and non-verbal fluency tasks. Percept. Mot. Ski. 1982, 55, 839–844. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wechsler, D. Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Fourth Edition; The Psychological Corporation: San Antonio, TX, USA, 2003. [Google Scholar]
- Ardila, A.; Pineda, D.; Rosselli, M. Correlation between intelligence test scores and executive function measures. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 2000, 15, 31–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Golden, C.J. The Stroop Color and Word Test: A Manual for Clinical and Experimental Uses; Stoelting: Chicago, IL, USA, 1978. [Google Scholar]
- Dos Santos, E.F.; Benneworth, P. Makerspace for skills development in the industry 4.0 era. BJOPM 2019, 16, 303–315. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
First Author (Year) | Sample | Measures | Outcomes | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Location | Size/Design | Age Range | |||
Filippetti et al. (2020) [44] | Argentina | Study 1: 112 children (M = 91.75; SD = 10.56) Study 2: 177 children (M = 9.94; SD = 1.24) Descriptive design and Structural equation model (SEM) | Study 1: 8–12 years Study 2: 8–13 years | Study 1: Intelligence: K-BIT; Flexibility: (1) Wisconsin Card Sorting Test; (2) Trail Making Test; (3) Five Point Test; (4) Semantic Verbal Fluency; (5) Phonological Verbal Fluency; Working Memory: Digits, Letters and numbers (WISC-IV). Inhibition: (1) Stroop color and Word Test; (2) NEPSY, Knock and Tap; (3) D2, Attention test (Spanish adaptation) Reading and writing: (1) Reading comprehension test (ENI); (2) PROESC writing test. Study 2: Creativity: (1) Figural Torrance Test (TTCT); (2) CREA; Flexibility: Same of numbers (1) and (3) in study 1; Working memory: Same of study 1; Inhibition: Same of number (1) in study 1; Intelligence: Same of study 1. | Study 1: Working Memory and Inhibition together contribute and support flexibility. This contribution depends on the tasks used and Inhibition contribution may depend on age. Study 2: There are consistent relationship between flexibility and creativity. Being creative requires a flexible thinking but depends on the tasks used and the type of flexibility measured (spontaneous or reactive). |
Bai et al. (2021) [45] | Netherlands | 102 children (M = 5.93; SD = 0.27) Descriptive design and Multilevel regressions (longitudinal) | 5.45–6.53 years | Divergent Thinking: (1) Alternative Uses Task; Inhibition: (1) Programmed in E-Prime Go/NoGO task; (2) Animal Stroop task; Shifting: (1) Dimensional Change Car Sort (DCCS); (2) Animal shifting task; Memory: (1) Word Recall Backwards; Selective Attention: (1) Visual search task programmed in E-Prime (ad-hoc). | Executive functions such as attention and specific processes influence and are related to the creation of original ideas. The influence of inhibition and working memory cannot be confirmed, not predict/moderate originality. |
De Chantal et al. (2017) [46] | Canada | Study 1: 32 children (M = 53.3 months) Study 2: 32 children (M = 47.06 months) Correlational design and hierarchical regression | Study 1: 41–64 months Study 2: 32–61 months | Creativity: (1) Generation task. Three different problems (ad-hoc); Inhibition: (1) DCCS task; Reasoning: (1) Logical reasoning task with a sets of problems (ad-hoc). | Inhibition plays an important role in children’s reasoning. The idea generation task is very similar to the creativity tests, so it can be said that creativity and some executive functions share mental processes. |
Krumm et al. (2020) [9] | Argentina | 200 children (M = 10.01; SD = 1.24) Correlational design | 8–13 years | Creativity: (1) Figural Torrance Test (TTCT) (2) CREA; Intelligence: (1) K-BIT; Working Memory: (1) Digits, letters and numbers of WISC-IV; Inhibiton: (1) Stroop color-word test; Shifting: (1) Wisconsin Card Sorting Test Computer version; Verbal Fluency: (1) Semantic and phonological verbal test (fruits and animals); Nonverbal Fluency (shifting): (1) Five-Point Test; Planning: (1) Porteus Maze Test. | There are significant differences in the EF: (1) Working Memory would be involved in the search for creative ideas. (2) Inhibition (measured with Stroop) is positively related to creativity because it eliminates the interference of dominant responses. (3) Cognitive Flexibility is associated with fluency and creative flexibility and not with originality. |
Krumm et al. (2018) [47] | Argentina | 209 children aged (M = 9.96; SD = 1.23) Correlational and SEM | 8–13 years | Creativity: (1) Figural Torrance Test (TTCT) (2) CREA; Intelligence: (1) K-BIT; Working Memory: (1) Digits, letters and numbers of WISC IV; Inhibiton: (1) Stroop color-word test; Shifting: (1) Wisconsin Card Sorting Test Computer version; Verbal Fluency: (1) Semantic and phonological verbal test (fruits and animals); Nonverbal Fluency (shifting): (1) Five-Point Test. | Positive correlation between creativity, flexibility, inhibition and intelligence. All executive functions correlate with creativity, but only inhibition and flexibility predicted creativity with intelligence as the mediating variable. |
Sánchez-Macías et al. (2021) [48] | Spain | 96 students (M = 14,5; SD = 0.85) Correlational design | 14–17 years | Creativity: (1) Torrance Test of Creative Thinking (TTCT); Working Memory: (1) WISC-IV (Spanish adaptation; Planning: Hanoi Tower; Inhibition: (1) Stroop Test; (2) Go/noGO; Shifting: (1) Wisconsin Card Sorting Test; Decision making: Iowa Gambling task. | Positive correlation between creativity and flexibility and negative correlation between creativity and verbal inhibition. The correlations between creativity and the rest of the executive functions are not significant. |
Stolte et al. (2020) [49] | Netherlands | 278 children (M = 9.71; SD = 0.93) Correlational design and SEM | 8–13 years | Inhibition: (1) Fish Game; Shifting: (1) Second block of the Fish Game; Working Memory: (1) Monkey Game to verbal updating; (2) Lion Game to visuo-spatial updating; Mathematical Creativity (MC): (1) MC Test Dutch translation; (2) Cito test; General creativity: (1) Test for Creativity Thinking Drawing production. | Positive correlation between creativity (general and specific) and working memory, but the role of inhibition and flexibility in creativity and mathematics cannot be generalized. |
Stolte et al. (2019) [50] | Netherlands | Fluency: 80 participants (M = 9.95; SD = 0.84) Flexibility: 82 participants (M = 9.93; SD = 0.82) Originality: 81 participants (M = 9.96; SD = 0.82) Correlational design and hierarchical multiple regression | 8–12 years | Inhibition: (1) Fish game; Mathematical ability: (1) Cito test; Mathematical Creativity (MC): (1) MC Test Dutch translation. | Results showed that mathematical creativity significantly correlated with components such as flexibility, fluency and originality. In the same way, that flexibility and originality significantly correlates with inhibition and this in turn produces a stronger relationship between mathematical ability and mathematical creativity of students. |
Vaisarova et al. (2021) [51] | USA | Experiment 1. 103 children 53 4-year-old (M = 48.5 months)/50 6-year-old (M = 72.4 months) Experiment 2. To avoid age bias in Experiment 1, 78 5-year-old children with typical development from a university database were recruited (M = 66.7 months) Comparative and Experimental design | 4–6 years | Experiment 1. Executive Functions: (1) Minnesota EF scale; Effortful Control: (1) Children’s Behavior Questionnaire (parents completed); Creativity: (1) Adaptation of Montweiler and Taylor’s; Divergent Thinking: (1) AUT for preschoolers (Adaptation of Wallach and Kogan’s); Intelligence: (1) Stanford-Binet Nonverbal Fluid Reasoning and Verbal Knowledge routing subtest. Experiment 2. Excutive Functions: (1) Same of experiment 1; (2) Flexible Item Selection Task (FIST) (3) Backward Digit Span; Effortful Control: (1) Same of experiment 1; Intelligence: Same of experiment 1; Conformity: (1) Adaptation of the Asch Social conformity paradigm; Divergent Thinking: Same of experiment 1. | Both studies showed negative relationships between EF and creativity. This suggests that cognitive skills do not enhance divergent and creative thinking and may take a backseat. |
Van Dijk et al. (2020) [52] | Netherlands | 70 (M = 11.07; SD = 0.69; 36 low stimulus; 34 high stimulus) Correlational design | 9–12 years | Creativity: (1) Alternative Uses Task (visual) Computer task; (2) Semantic categories of the Torrance Test (TTCT); Selective Attention: (1) Subtest Sky Search of the Test of Everyday Attention For Children (TEA-Ch). | Overall positive effect of selective attention on creativity measures, especially originality. Attention causes distractors to be ignored, ignoring new ideas, but it helps to focus on details, which is why it scores more in originality. |
Zhao et al. (2021) [53] | China | Study 1. 73 students (M = 13.8; SD = 0.6) Study 2. 68 students (M = 13.2; SD = 0.4) Comparative and Experimental design | Study 1. 13–15 years Study 2. 12–14 years | Study 1. Working memory: 4 tasks: 3 tasks to WM updating and 1 task WM span; Inhibition: (1) stroop task with Chinese character; (2) Flanker task; (3) GO/noGO; Switching: A digital test with a series of digits; Creativity: (1) Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking; (2) RAT (Chinese version. Study 2. Pre- and Post-training Tests. Working memory task, as described for Study 1, was used to assess near-transfer effects of the WM updating training; Training Tasks. Three adaptive running WM updating (a letter, animal, and visuospatial). | Creativity is related to working memory updating and not to inhibition, processing speed, working memory maintenance, or flexibility. This may be due to taking into account convergent thinking that other research does not value. |
Chevalier et al. (2012) [54] | USA | 250 preschool children (1) M = 3.71: age range = 3.67–3.83; (2) M = 4.45; age range = 4.42–4.5; (3) mean age 5.19; age range = 5.08–5.25 Correlational design | 3 years 9 months, 4 years 6 months, and 5 years 3 months | Flexibility: (1) Shape School; Inhibition: (1) Go/No-Go task; Working memory. (1) The Nebraska Barnyard (adapted from the Noisy Book task). | Preschool-age children use working memory and inhibition to be flexible and creative. Students with greater inhibition are less flexible and concise, but this may be because at an early age it is more difficult to manage their cognitive abilities well. |
Variables | Frequencies | Percentage |
---|---|---|
Year | ||
2012 | 1 | 8.3% |
2017 | 1 | 8.3% |
2018 | 1 | 8.3% |
2019 | 1 | 8.3% |
2020 | 4 | 33.3% |
2021 | 4 | 33.3% |
Journal | ||
Child Neuropsychology | 1 | 8.3% |
Developmental psychology | 1 | 8.3% |
Journal of intelligence | 3 | 25.0% |
Memory and Cognition | 1 | 8.3% |
Personality and Individual Differences | 1 | 8.3% |
Psicogente | 1 | 8.3% |
Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity and Arts | 1 | 8.3% |
Revista de Formación del Profesorado | 1 | 8.3% |
Thinking Skills and Creativity | 1 | 8.3% |
Trends in neuroscience and education | 1 | 8.3% |
Language | ||
English | 10 | 83.3% |
Spanish | 2 | 16.7% |
Variables | Frequencies | Percentage |
---|---|---|
Sample age | ||
Early childhood (0–6 years) | 3 | 25.0% |
Childhood (7–12 years) | 7 | 53.8% |
Adolescence (13–18 years) | 2 | 23.1% |
Country | ||
Argentina | 3 | 25.0% |
Canada | 1 | 8.3% |
China | 1 | 8.3% |
Netherlands | 4 | 33.3% |
Spain | 1 | 8.3% |
USA | 2 | 16.7% |
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2023 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Pasarín-Lavín, T.; Abín, A.; García, T.; Rodríguez, C. Relationship between Executive Functions and Creativity in Children and Adolescents: A Systematic Review. Children 2023, 10, 1002. https://doi.org/10.3390/children10061002
Pasarín-Lavín T, Abín A, García T, Rodríguez C. Relationship between Executive Functions and Creativity in Children and Adolescents: A Systematic Review. Children. 2023; 10(6):1002. https://doi.org/10.3390/children10061002
Chicago/Turabian StylePasarín-Lavín, Tania, Amanda Abín, Trinidad García, and Celestino Rodríguez. 2023. "Relationship between Executive Functions and Creativity in Children and Adolescents: A Systematic Review" Children 10, no. 6: 1002. https://doi.org/10.3390/children10061002
APA StylePasarín-Lavín, T., Abín, A., García, T., & Rodríguez, C. (2023). Relationship between Executive Functions and Creativity in Children and Adolescents: A Systematic Review. Children, 10(6), 1002. https://doi.org/10.3390/children10061002