Understanding Plant Nutrition—The Genesis of Students’ Conceptions and the Implications for Teaching Photosynthesis
Abstract
:1. Introduction: Students’ Conceptions about Plant Nutrition
2. Theoretical Background
- (1)
- What conceptions do students have to conceive plant nutrition and photosynthesis? (identification of conceptions)
- (2)
- What embodied conceptions and image schemas are fundamental for these conceptions to evolve? (genesis of conceptions)
- (3)
- What influence do alternative conceptions towards the van-Helmont experiment and the learning process in general have? (potentials of conceptions)
3. Research Design and Methods
- (i)
- Interview about students’ conceptions towards plant nutrition. This part was meant to elicit and identify students’ concepts about plant nutrition (1). First, the interviewees were asked to complete a sketch regarding plant and human nutrition. In addition, questions about the absorption and production of substances as well as the function of nutrients were asked. Furthermore, the students were questioned to compare human and plant nutrition. By that, we hoped to identify embodied concepts that give hints to the genesis of the students’ conceptions of plant nutrition (2);
- (ii)
- Group discussion about the outcome of the van-Helmont experiment. In this part of the teaching experiment, the triads were exposed to the van-Helmont experiment. They were asked to describe and explain the results of the experiment. During the second part, the researcher merely guided the discussion. This part was meant to give information about the influence of different students’ conceptions on the learning efficacy in case of a cognitive conflict (3). Furthermore, the discussions provided the basis to analyze students’ learning pathways.
4. Results
4.1. Learners Understand Plant Nutrition Predominantly as Absorption of Matter
4.2. When Talking about Plant Nutrition, Learners Often Refer to Their Own Diet
4.3. Alternative Conceptions Can Be Fostered by the Van-Helmont Experiment
5. Discussion and Conclusions
5.1. Genesis of Alternative Conceptions
5.2. Using Alternative Conceptions and Image Schemas for Conceptual Change
- (I)
- Humans and plants alike need the same types of substances to stay alive. In a first step, it has to be elaborated on the usage of nutrients for energetic (cellular respiration) and constructive metabolism (growth, etc.). Nutrients are seen explicitly as high-energy substances such as carbohydrates, fats, and proteins. Questions like “What do humans and plants need in order to grow?” have to be part of a constructive teaching unit (see Figure 2). Thereby, embodied cognition about human nutrition is mapped onto plant nutrition. As a consequence, students are able to discover that plants and humans are similar by using the same low-energetic substances like water, minerals, and high-energy nutrients to stay alive (similarity: usage of nutrients). Here the comparison of plants and humans points out that both are conceptualized as objects that consist of different types of substances (Component-Integral-Object Schema). An increase in numbers of these substances automatically leads to growth (Becoming-Bigger Schema). Subsequently, they are experienced as living organisms of the same structure (dissimilation aspect). For means of reduction, vitamins and fibers needed by heterotrophic organisms were left aside.
- (II)
- After addressing dissimilation processes by showing the similarities between humans’ and plants’ usage of nutrition, the inevitable question about the acquisition of these fundamental substances and, thereby, the differences between human and plant nutrition can be derived—“Where does the organism get water, minerals and nutrients from?” Talking about human nutrition (source domain), learners’ know that they have to consume necessary substances (Container Schema, Source-Path-Goal Schema). Because the only source that consists of high-energy nutrients comes with other (living) organisms, humans are called heterotrophic—obtaining their food by consuming other organisms. As far as plant nutrition is concerned (target domain), it is essential to point out that plants need to absorb minerals, water, and carbon dioxide (Container Schema, Source-Path-Goal Schema) to produce high-energy nutrients (frame Nutrition due to Nutrient Production). These nutrients are the building units used for plant growth (Transformation Schema, Component-Integral-Object Schema). For many students it remains unclear that humans as well as plants actually consist of the same types of substances leading to the genesis of alternative conceptions such as absorption of nutrients from the soil. The learning potential can be seen in pointing out the similarities both types of organisms have regarding their kind of nutrition: Plants need the same nutrients, but produce them themselves. They both need to absorb minerals and water (frame Nutrition due to Absorption). In this way, the topic of plant nutrition can be addressed by using embodied conceptions based on human nutrition.
Author Contributions
Funding
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Driver, R. Changing conceptions. In Adolescent Development and School Science; Taylor & Francis Group: Boca Raton, FL, USA, 1989; pp. 79–99. [Google Scholar]
- Duit, R.; Treagust, D.F. Conceptual change: A powerful framework for improving science teaching and learning. IJSE 2003, 25, 671–688. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Baalmann, W.; Kattmann, U. Towards a better understanding of genetics and evolution—Research in students’ conceptions leads to a re-arrangement of teaching biology. In Proceedings from III Conference of European Researchers in Didactic of Biology (ERIDOB); García-Rodeja Gayoso, I., Díaz de Bustamante, J., Harms, U., Jiménez Aleixandre, M.P., Eds.; Univ Santiago de Compostela: La Coruña, Spain, 2001; pp. 13–25. [Google Scholar]
- Zabel, J. Stories and meaning: What students’ narratives reveal about their understanding of the Theory of Evolution. In Conference of the European Science Education Research Association (ESERA); ESERA: Leeds, UK, 2007; pp. 22–25. [Google Scholar]
- Munson, B.H. Ecological misconceptions. J. Environ. Educ. 1994, 25, 30–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Marmaroti, P.; Galanopoulou, D. Pupils’ understanding of photosynthesis: A questionnaire for the simultaneous assessment of all aspects. IJSE 2006, 28, 383–403. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Anderson, C.W.; Sheldon, T.H.; Dubay, J. The effects of instruction on college nonmajors’ conceptions of respiration and photosynthesis. J. Res. Sci. Teach. 1990, 27, 761–776. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Krall, R.M.; Lott, K.H.; Wymer, C.L. Inservice elementary and middle school teachers’ conceptions of photosynthesis and respiration. J. Sci. Teach. Educ. 2009, 20, 41–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Eisen, Y.; Stavy, R. Students’ understanding of photosynthesis. Am. Biol. Teach. 1988, 50, 208–212. [Google Scholar]
- Barker, M.A.; Carr, M.D. Photosynthesis—Can our pupils see the wood for the trees? JBE 1989, 23, 41–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bell, B. Students’ ideas about plant nutrition: What are they? JBE 1985, 19, 213–218. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cañal, P. Photosynthesis and ‘inverse respiration’ in plants: An inevitable misconception? IJSE 1999, 21, 363–371. [Google Scholar]
- Özay, E.; Öztaş, H. Secondary students’ interpretations of photosynthesis and plant nutrition. JBE 2003, 37, 68–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Waheed, T.; Lucas, A.M. Understanding interrelated topics: Photosynthesis at age photosynthesis at age14+. JBE 1992, 26, 193–199. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Phillips, D.C. Constructivism in Education: Opinions and Second Opinions on Controversial Issues; University of Chicago Press: Chicago, IL, USA, 2000. [Google Scholar]
- Gerstenmaier, J.; Mandl, M. Wissenserwerb unter konstruktivistischer Perspektive (The acquisition of knowledge from constructivist perspective). Z. Pädag. (J. Pedag.) 1995, 41, 867–888. [Google Scholar]
- Chi, M.T. Three types of conceptual change: Belief revision, mental model transformation, and categorical shift. In International Handbook of Research on Conceptual Change; Routledge: London, UK, 2008; pp. 61–82. [Google Scholar]
- Strike, K.A.; Posner, G.J.A. revisionist theory of conceptual change. In Philosophy of Science, Cognitive Psychology, and Educational Theory and Practice; Suny Press: Albany, NY, USA, 1992. [Google Scholar]
- Duit, R.; Treagust, D.F. Learning in science: From behaviourism towards social constructivism and beyond. Int. Handb. Sci. Educ. 1998, 1, 3–25. [Google Scholar]
- Gropengießer, H. What language reveals about our conceptions—A case study on seeing. Res. Sci. Educ. Past Present Future 1998, 1, 140–142. [Google Scholar]
- Kattmann, U. Learning biology by means of anthropomorphic conceptions? In Biology in Context: Learning and Teaching for 21st Century; Hammann, M., Ed.; Institute of Education, University of London: London, UK, 2007; pp. 21–26. [Google Scholar]
- Lakoff, G.; Johnson, M. Metaphors We Live By; University of Chicago Press: Chicago, IL, USA, 1980. [Google Scholar]
- Lakoff, G.; Johnson, M. Philosophy in the Flesh; Basic Books: New York, NY, USA, 1999. [Google Scholar]
- Gallese, V.; Lakoff, G. The brain’s concepts: The role of the sensory-motor system in conceptual knowledge. Cognit. Neuropsychol. 2005, 22, 455–479. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Gropengießer, H. Theorie des erfahrungsbasierten Verstehens (Theory of experimentalism). In Theorien in der Biologiedidaktischen Forschung (Theories in the Field of Biology Didactics); Krüger, D., Vogt, H., Eds.; Springer: Berlin, Germany, 2007; pp. 105–116. [Google Scholar]
- Niebert, K.; Marsch, S.; Treagust, D.F. Understanding needs embodiment: A theory-guided reanalysis of the role of metaphors and analogies in understanding science. Sci. Educ. 2012, 96, 849–877. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rohrer, T. Image schemata in the brain. In From Perception to Meaning: Image Schemas in Cognitive Linguistics; De Gruyter Mouton: Berlin, Germany, 2005; pp. 165–196. [Google Scholar]
- Johnson, M. The philosophical significance of image schemas. In From Perception to Meaning: Image Schemas in Cognitive Linguistics; De Gruyter Mouton: Berlin, Germany, 2005; pp. 15–33. [Google Scholar]
- Johnson, M.; Lakoff, G. Leben in Metaphern—Konstruktion und Gebrauch von Sprachbildern, 6th ed.; Carl-Auer: Heidelberg, Germany, 2004. [Google Scholar]
- Winston, M.E.; Chaffin, R.; Herrmann, D. A taxonomy of part-whole relations. Cognit. Sci. 1987, 11, 417–444. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Riemeier, T. Schülervorstellungen von Zellen, Teilung und Wachstum (Students’ conceptions about cells, cell division and growth). Z. Didakt. Naturwiss. 2005, 11, 52–72. [Google Scholar]
- Hewson, P.W.; Hewson, M.G.B. The role of conceptual conflict in conceptual change and the design of science instruction. Instruct. Sci. 1984, 13, 1–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Duit, R.; Gropengießer, H.; Kattmann, U.; Komorek, M.; Parchmann, I. The Model of Educational Reconstruction—A Framework for Improving Teaching and Learning Science1. In Science Education Research and Practice in Europe; Sense Publishers: Rotterdam, The Netherlands, 2012; pp. 13–37. [Google Scholar]
- Steffe, L.P.; D’Ambrosio, B.S. Using teaching experiments to understand students’ mathematics. In Improving Teaching and Learning in Science and Mathematics; Treagust, D., Duit, R., Fraser, B., Eds.; Teacher College Press: New York, NY, USA, 1996; pp. 65–76. [Google Scholar]
- Komorek, M.; Duit, R. The teaching experiment as a powerful method to develop and evaluate teaching and learning sequences. IJSE 2004, 25, 619–633. [Google Scholar]
- Mayring, P. Qualitative Content Analysis. In A Companion to Qualitative Research; Flick, U., von Kardoff, E., Steinke, I., Eds.; Sage Publications: Thousand Oaks, CA, USA, 2004; pp. 266–269. [Google Scholar]
- Gropengießer, H. Qualitative Inhaltsanalyse in der fachdidaktischen Lehr-Lernforschung (Qualitative content analysis in teaching and learning research). In Die Praxis der Qualitativen Inhaltsanalyse (Practice of Qualitative Content Analysis); Mayring, P., Gläser-Zikuda, M., Eds.; Beltz Verlag: Weinheim, Germany, 2008; pp. 172–189. [Google Scholar]
- Paul, J.; Lederman, N.G.; Groß, J. Learning experimentation through science fairs. IJSE 2016, 38, 2367–2387. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lakoff, G. Women, Fire, and Dangerous Things; University of Chicago Press: Chicago, IL, USA, 1987. [Google Scholar]
- Teixeira, F.M. What happens to the food we eat? Children’s conceptions of the structure and function of the digestive system. IJSE 2000, 22, 507–520. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Messig, D.; Zacher, T.; Groß, J. Die Fotosynthese verstehen. MNU Math. Naturwiss. Unterr. (Underst. Photosynth.) 2016, 2, 101–106. [Google Scholar]
- Groß, J. Biologie Verstehen: Wirkungen außerschulischer Lernorte (Understanding Biology: Effects of Non-Formal Education); Didaktisches Zentrum: Oldenburg, Germany, 2007. [Google Scholar]
- Nehm, R.; Kampourakis, K. Conceptual change in science and science education. In Encyclopedia of Educational Philosophy and Theory; Sage Publications: Thousand Oaks, CA, USA, 2016; pp. 1–5. [Google Scholar]
- Vosniadou, S.; Vamvakoussi, X.; Skopelitit, I. The Framework Theory Approach to the Problem of Conceptual Change. In International Handbook of Research on Conceptual Change; Vosniadou, S., Ed.; Routledge: New York, NY, USA, 2008; pp. 3–34. [Google Scholar]
Identified Conception | Number of Students | Underlying Image Schema | Anchor Example |
---|---|---|---|
plants grow due to enlargement | 12 | Becoming-Bigger Schema | “Plants grow because their roots become bigger.” |
plants grow due to cell division | 6 | Becoming-Bigger Schema | “Cell division makes a tree to build up more tissue and as a result it starts growing.” |
plants grow by absorbing water and nutrients | 11 | Container Schema Source-Path-Goal Schema | “I guess a plant mostly needs water and those nutrients from the soil as food. Then it is able to grow.” |
plants absorb carbon dioxide | 5 | Source-Path-Goal Schema | “A tree is able to breath in carbon dioxide from the air.” |
plants converse carbon dioxide into oxygen | 2 | Container Schema Transformation Schema | “Plants take in carbon dioxide and release oxygen. This is essential for humans.” |
plants produce nutrients | 3 | Transformation Schema Component-Integral-Object Schema | “Plants feed on glucose. They produce this themselves by photosynthesis.” |
plant growth is based on nutrient production | 1 | Transformation Schema Component-Integral-Object Schema | “Plants produce food on their own. They use glucose to grow.” |
© 2018 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Messig, D.; Groß, J. Understanding Plant Nutrition—The Genesis of Students’ Conceptions and the Implications for Teaching Photosynthesis. Educ. Sci. 2018, 8, 132. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci8030132
Messig D, Groß J. Understanding Plant Nutrition—The Genesis of Students’ Conceptions and the Implications for Teaching Photosynthesis. Education Sciences. 2018; 8(3):132. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci8030132
Chicago/Turabian StyleMessig, Denis, and Jorge Groß. 2018. "Understanding Plant Nutrition—The Genesis of Students’ Conceptions and the Implications for Teaching Photosynthesis" Education Sciences 8, no. 3: 132. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci8030132
APA StyleMessig, D., & Groß, J. (2018). Understanding Plant Nutrition—The Genesis of Students’ Conceptions and the Implications for Teaching Photosynthesis. Education Sciences, 8(3), 132. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci8030132