Next Article in Journal
“It’s Like Being Back in GCSE Art”—Engaging with Music, Film-Making and Boardgames. Creative Pedagogies within Youth Work Education
Previous Article in Journal
A Blended Learning Approach for an Introductory Computer Science Course
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Progress Report in Neuroscience and Education: Experiment of Four Neuropedagogical Methods

1
Laboratory of Analytical Chemistry and Physico-Chemistry of Materials, Hassan II University of Casablanca, Casablanca 20100, Morocco
2
Multidisciplinary Laboratory in Educations Sciences and Training Engineering, Assessment in Sport Sciences and in Physical Activity Didactic, Normal Superior School (ENS), Hassan II University of Casablanca, Casablanca 50069, Morocco
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Educ. Sci. 2021, 11(8), 373; https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci11080373
Submission received: 23 June 2021 / Revised: 16 July 2021 / Accepted: 21 July 2021 / Published: 22 July 2021

Abstract

:
We endeavor through this work to demonstrate the effects of the introduction of neuroeducation data in schools and their application, via an experiment of neuropedagogical methods, to respond to the hypothesis that the contribution of neuroeducation may be beneficial for learning. During this study we designed four neuropedagogical methods and to measure their effect on the learning of students in the classroom we used an evaluation grid comprising six psychopedagogical parameters. The results show that no statistically significant variation of these psychoeducational parameters is observed between sexes. However, a variation is recorded for the four neuropedagogical methods between the groups in pre-test and post at the levels of the mean scores ranging from 5.15% to 440%.

1. Introduction

The creation of “neuroeducator” training, first proposed 30 years ago [1], was based on the belief that brain science could transform and improve teacher practice. However, the role of neuroscience in education is still under discussion [2,3]. The educational research community has not finished defining and conceptualizing a science of learning and education [4]. Bridges have been crossed to connect neuroscience and education, for example, long bridges [5], double track bridges [6], bridges over troubled waters [7], and others can be built to make connections between education and cognitive neuroscience. These bridges can be beneficial to both educators and neuroscientists, helping to ask critical questions about the learning brain and providing guidance on where to answer them (bridges over troubled waters education and cognitive neuroscience) [7].
Neuroeducation unites education and neuroscience. Neuroscience, in its broadest sense, studies the processes by which the brain learns and remembers at the molecular and cellular levels (for example, the system of neural areas and pathways underlying our ability to speak and understand language). It seems timely then, to consider how we might apply our increased understanding of brain function and development to explore educational questions [8]. Studies of brain function can contribute, alongside behavioral data, to an understanding of underlying learning processes, and may lead to improved teaching and learning [9].
Currently, one of the challenges of educational neuroscience is the improvement of scientific dialogue and creation of a more widely shared language in educational and neuroscientific circles. Hence the need for professionals who are proficient in shared communication across disciplines [10]. Although policy makers are calling for “scientifically sound practice”, there is little consensus on the conceptual and methodological underpinnings of these research efforts. Part of the challenge stems from the need to balance academic freedom with clear research standards and to establish principles that allow for the generalization and in-depth evaluation of educationally relevant research findings while maintaining the integrity of each field.
These problems are complicated when pedagogically relevant research from different academic disciplines is examined and attempts at integration are undertaken. In the context of bridging cognitive neuroscience and education, it is important to assess the differences and similarities in methodology, approach, and conceptualization of scientific research between what might be called “traditional pedagogical research” and “traditional cognitive neuroscience research” [7].
Numerous studies have questioned the ways in which neuroscience can be applied in the classroom. The entry of neuroscience into the world of education is felt to be an overbearing intrusion. The difficulty is even greater in defining a disciplinary field that still does not exist as such at the university [11]. Neuroeducation does not yet have an academic status in its own right, its title and definition are not yet unanimously accepted, which is why the marriage between cognitive neuroscience and conventional teaching still seems difficult [12]. The real challenge, then, is to translate laboratory results into strategies to improve learning when establishing links between research and practice is not always easy [13].
Recent brain imaging techniques make it possible to explore the effects of certain types of teaching on the brains of students. The example of reading by grapheme or phoneme with regard to [14] dyslexia and dyscalculia [15] clearly demonstrates this. This encounter between neuroscience and education has given rise to a new research discipline, “neuroeducation” [16,17,18,19]. Research in this area of neuroeducation is recent, but the results are of scientific value and of interest to many educators. Although the brain mechanisms involved in learning to read and compute are fairly well known, the effects on the brain of teaching practices associated with these skills remain largely unknown [20].
It therefore seems appropriate to consider how to apply an understanding of brain development and function to explore educational issues [21]. Cruickshank [1] proposed the creation of a neuroeducator profile, on the basis that understanding the brain could improve teacher practice. Neuroscience provides a biologically-based mechanistic approach that could explain why these practices work, suggest additional new approaches [22], and indicate which neural functions are typical or impaired and how they work; but this knowledge must be translated by pedagogical principles into interventions which can be evaluated for effectiveness by behavioral testing in educational settings [9]. Much water has flowed under the “bridge” since Bruer’s argument [5] that a direct brain-behavior-education link was a “bridge too far”. Research findings in mind and cognitive psychology are building that bridge [14,19,23,24].
Research in the area of neuroeducation is recent, but the results are of scientific value and of interest to many educators. Although the brain mechanisms involved in learning to read and calculate are fairly well known, the effects on the brain of the teaching practices associated with these skills remain very little known [20]. Certainly, the debate between neuroscience and education is always lively, but this relationship can be more fruitful when it favors a two-way exchange of ideas and approaches

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Characteristics of the Sample

The study involved a total of 239 students (aged 12 to 18) divided into 8 classes and 4 subjects in three secondary schools (Moulay Idriss Premier high school, Fechtali high school and Al Andalouss middle school) in Casablanca with the collaboration of 3 administrative staff and 4 secondary school teachers (1 female and 3 male). They all gave their consent to collaborate in this experiment, were between 25 and 57 years of age, and had less than 6 years of experience.
The students were reassured, informed, and prepared to behave as naturally as possible with a stranger in their classroom.
Before starting the experiment interviews were carried out. A first exploratory interview with the administrative managers of the three schools was carried out in order to prepare the field of experimentation. This allowed us to define the list of teachers who would agree to participate in the experiment. In a second step, short training sessions with the teachers were carried out around the following axes: the interest of the study and the contribution of cognitive neurosciences for pedagogy, the principles of learning based on neurosciences, and experiments carried out internationally. Then, with each of the teachers separately, we defined the approach to follow for the proposal of the theoretical sheets and the co-construction of the practical pedagogical sheets, as well as the didactic supports necessary for each lesson. Thus, the sequences that would be used as practice with the control group were defined: two teaching sessions per neuro-pedagogical method, with two pre- and post-test evaluations. This study was carried out with the approval of the educational inspectors and the provincial director, Ben M’sik Sidi Othman.

2.2. Neuropedagogical Methods

2.2.1. Overall Goal of the Methods:

The tool used in this study was a design of four neuro-pedagogical methods inspired by the work of [25,26,27], whose purpose was to test whether neuroscience research is capable of changing teaching practices to make them more effective, and whether it can help learners take full advantage of their intellectual and affective capacities.
The methods were adapted to our situation and then implemented in the experimental classes while taking into consideration the dimensions of the socio-demographic characteristics of the schools. For each method, we specified the role of the teacher and that of the student during the experiment session (in the form of a sheet).

2.2.2. Experimental Method

  • Experimental hypothesis:
  • Application material(s):
  • Sample:
    Grade Level(s):
    Total number of students (G. Exp): number of males, females
  • Description of method of experimentation: what the teacher is to do in this experiment, and what the student should do in this experiment (Table 1)
  • Student assessment: number of questions, structures, and nature of questions.

2.3. Measuring Instrument

Data were collected through guided observation using a grid to capture the behavior of learners in class during the pre-test session and during the experimental session as a test station according to the proposed instructional sheets. Evaluation grids were duly completed and reviewed with the teacher during the pre-test and post-test sessions to measure the variables as shown in Table 2.
Variables such as attention and active engagement were measured by direct observation in class during learning activities according to a Likert scale in post-test and pre-test, while the other variables, in this case the number of words memorized, feedback on errors, and grades were measured by a written test at the end of the experimental session and also in two periods post-test and pre-test. Satisfaction of the students toward the method was measured at the end of the experiment according to a Likert scale.

2.4. Data Analysis Process

The information collected was processed using SPSS (Statistical Package for the Social Sciences “25 version”). For quantitative data, we used the Wilcoxon test which is a non-parametric alternative to the t-test for paired samples (pre-test and post-test). This allowed us to test our hypothesis that neuroeducation can help learners develop a more alert brain and enable them to be more active, attentive, motivated, and better able to overcome difficulties, by checking whether the psychoeducational parameters vary significantly between the pre-test and post-test of the experiment. The significance level is set at (p < 0.05).

3. Results

As shown in Table 3 and Table 4, no statistically significant variation in psychoeducational parameters is found between the sexes; however, they vary for the six neuroeducational methods between the groups in pre-test and post-test according to the following order: highly significant *** (p < 0.001), moderately significant ** (p < 0.01), insignificant * (p < 0.05), and non-significant (p ≥ 0.05).
The six psycho-pedagogical parameters of four neuro-pedagogical methods named “vary the access to information”, “mind map”, and “memory cards” vary in a highly significant way (p < 0.001) between the beginning and the end of each of the experiments at the level of the averages of the scores, going from 28.62% to 706.25%.
For the neuro-pedagogical method called “role playing”, there were variances of 80% for the number of key words memorized, 40.38% commitment, 50% for attention, 43.09% for the return on error, 32.19%for the satisfaction of the students towards the method, and 94.12% for the results of the written test (Figure 1).
Indeed, at the level of the averages of the attention scores, for the neuro-pedagogical method “vary the access to information” there is a significant increase of 235.13% ((1.24 − 0.37)/0.37) × 100%) at the post-test compared to the pre-test corresponding to 0.37 ± 0.7 vs. 1.24 ± 0.5. There is also a significant increase of 215.55% in active engagement, 89.37% in the number of words memorized, 175% in the error feedback, 440.54% in the satisfaction with the method and 32.19% in the written test (Figure 2).
For the neuro-pedagogical method called “the expert student”, there are variations of 66.20% for the number of key words memorized, 57.01% for commitment, 19.30% for attention, 18.51% for return on error, 12.19% for satisfaction of the students towards the method, and 28.62 for the results of the written test (Figure 3).
Finally, the average scores of the “mind map” method, whose results are also highly significant, vary from 35.31% for the written test, 36.01% for the number of key words memorized, 38% for active engagement, 61.26% for the parameters of attention, 163.63% for the parameters of return on error, and 181.69% for satisfaction of the students towards the method (Figure 4).
It is easy to see from Figure 1 that for each treatment group, the relationship between post-test and pre-test is approximately linear. It is also clear that the linear relationship is similar for all three treatment groups, as illustrated by the simplified representation in Figure 5.

4. Discussion

Throughout this study, we have sought to highlight the effects of introducing neuroeducational data into the school and more specifically into classroom learning situations. The results obtained are promising.
For the neuro-pedagogical method “role playing”, a method designed to develop active engagement in students, our results show a significant increase (p < 0.008) of 40.38% in active engagement. Our hypothesis was based on our belief that role playing is action learning [28]. It is “a fictional interaction in which one should participate by inventing a scenario and investing oneself in the skin of a character other than oneself” [29].
During this role playing session, students used analogies to represent the concepts studied; thus, learning is facilitated by comparing new information with known contexts [30]. This allows the internalization of concepts, and also explains the significant increase (p < 005) of 43.09% in the number of words memorized.
Reasoning by analogy is at the heart of the generalization and abstraction processes that allow the development of concepts and creativity and is one of the best strategies for explaining complex concepts [31]. It corresponds to the natural way in which the brain prioritizes information [32], allows the mind to convert known concepts into new knowledge, and plays an important role in deducing general representations from similarities, which in turn can be applied to solving new problems.
One of the reasons analogies work so well is that they take advantage of the brain’s normal neural circuitry [33]. Despite the importance of reasoning by analogy for everyday life and in society, it remains fairly understudied [34]. The brain regions involved in analogical thinking are much less explored. Nevertheless, it has been found that certain types of analogical thinking activate the prefrontal and medial cortexes [35] and that the integrity of the left rostro-lateral prefrontal cortex is necessary in the processes related to reasoning by analogy. A study on this type of reasoning [36] concluded that the left posterolateral prefrontal cortex (with its long-range anatomical connections) is specifically critical for analogical reasoning and that a lesion of the left rostro-lateral prefrontal cortex may impair the relational integration and matching processes involved in abstract thinking. The critical bundles for analogy connect the frontal lobe to the temporal cortex (via the uncinate bundle), to the occipital cortex (via the inferior fronto-occipital bundle), and to subcortical structures [37].
Other areas are also mobilized during role play: the orbitofrontal cortex, mirror neurons, and Broca’s field in the left hemisphere, which is associated with the empathy of the leader or group members. Particularly mobilized are the prefrontal cortex and limbic system of the protagonist who makes many connections, internalizes, judges, invokes memories, experiences, and perhaps restructurings [38]. Role playing is a technique where the player feels the emotional support of the group the and the closeness they need [39].
For the neuro-pedagogical method “the expert student”, the students, firstly, must cooperatively build and produce explanatory presentations of the notions of the concept in their charge, and secondly, are placed in a competitive situation to present, in the best way they can, what they have produced in the cooperative phase. Our results show a significant improvement between the beginning and the end of the experiment (p < 0.5) of the psycho-pedagogical parameters, ranging from 19% to more than 66%.
At the neural level, cooperation provides a social incentive and is associated with an involvement of the orbitofrontal cortex, mobilization during such cooperation is considered closely related to the motivational control of target behaviors [40], and in the processing of positive feedback information [41]. This explains, in our opinion, the results recorded at the level of student engagement and their feedback on errors during this experiment; students developed an expert posture since each of them plays the role of the teacher and takes charge of the explanation of a concept for which they have become an expert, giving them a feeling of self-efficacy and acting as a central self-regulatory mechanism of human activity [42].
The active involvement of the students, with very limited teacher intervention during the experiment, improves the activity related to the cognitive task (e.g., working memory) [43]. This is perfectly consistent with our results, since the number of words memorized was significantly improved by 66.20%, and also allows the development of self-esteem and motivation which seem to modulate the neural activity related to learning. Self-determination theory [44] explains why students thrive when they are autonomously motivated, and it explains why they benefit when teachers support their autonomy. Individuals with high self-esteem have greater volumes of gray matter in the right lateral prefrontal cortex [45].
The link is more subtle between emotional intelligence and metacognition [46]. Recall that the session in this experiment is conducted in two stages, the first of coordination and a second of competition which requires additional mentalization resources and is associated with an increase in medial prefrontal activity. The two positions of cooperation and competition lead to the activation of a common frontoparietal network subordinating the executive functions, (as well as the anterior insula, involved in autonomic excitation) [47].
The neuro-pedagogical method “Vary the ways in which information is accessed” speaks to how our brain is designed to capture simultaneously, in different modes, information received from the environment that is captured by the senses and sent by the nerves to the sensory regions of the brain such as the visual cortex, the auditory cortex, and the somato-sensory cortex. This offers a complementarity in perception, and a cross-reinforcement of information by multiplying access by exploiting the three indices: semantic, sensory, and emotional. It is by multiplying the access routes to stored knowledge that we facilitate its retrieval; learning occurs when two neurons communicate with each other, that is to say, when one neuron sends a message to another [48]. This information can then be used by the brain as feedback to adjust its neuronal connections. This action is important not only because it is essential for survival, but also because it allows the collection of new data about the environment and thus amplifies the effects of perception [49].
The results obtained from our experiment are in line with scientific findings on the differentiation of information access voices and their impact on students’ attention [50,51,52]. In fact, during this experiment, the attention of the students increased significantly (p < 0.001) by 253% and on the memorization, since the variation of the ways of accessing information allows the consolidation of knowledge, when an idea is evoked or a strategy is used, it is a set of neuron-neural networks that are activated together. Masson [13] thus responds to Hebb’s law “neurons that activate together connect together”, and in order to reinforce this connection to facilitate memory retrieval stimuli should be varied. The richer and more varied the stimulation of the senses, the more the connections are reinforced, if more than one sense is stimulated during learning, each recorded sensation then functions as a different input capable of leading to the totality of the information on a subject.
Our results also showed a highly significant (p < 0.001) improvement in memory between the pre-test and the post-test with a percentage of 175%; the richer and more varied the stimulation of the senses, the stronger the connections and the stronger the links between perceptual memory (senses) and episodic memory and the easier the retrieval of information [53].
The limitations of this technique are related to the lack of adequate material that allows this practice to achieve expected results, as well as to the effort that many teachers should make to better master its use. We propose that in the absence of didactic material, the teacher can be helped by software or open-source courseware and digital simulation resources that allow them to represent unobservable phenomena and to simulate physical interactions. The three-dimensional visualization illustrates in a simplified way a complex structure that can be manipulated in real time.
For the neuro-pedagogical method “Mind map”, studies conducted in different sectors such as design [54], medical sciences [55], linguistics [56,57], or economics and management [58,59] show that mind maps promote the structured representation of problems, memorization and creativity. The mind map is a graphic and playful way to represent ideas and to stimulate creativity [60,61]. The results obtained with this practice are in line with the vision of the above-mentioned authors. All the expectations were fulfilled, and the results show a highly significant increase (p < 0.001) of the psycho-pedagogical parameters, especially the results of writing and the number of memorized words, whose improvement reached 163.64% and 181.69%, respectively. Concept mapping can effectively function as an effective learning activity when it involves retrieval practice [62] because the use of mind mapping involves the linking of a word to prior knowledge which can allow for a double coding of information in the verbal and visual components of long-term memory, thus allowing for more effective retrieval [63] since it this double coding involves increased neural activity in the left inferior prefrontal cortex. Increased activity in this region, regardless of the individual’s intention to remember, leads to a more easily retrievable memory trace [64].
Concept maps cannot provide a solution for classrooms where rote learning predominates; but they do help, through the hierarchization of knowledge, to help students create meaning and reduce exogenous cognitive load, a kind of spatial contiguity which can promote a more semantically integrated understanding of the concept [65,66].
Cognitive function emerges from the hierarchical and embedded architecture of the nervous system [67]. One of the very important uses of concept maps is not only as a learning tool, but also an assessment tool, thus encouraging students, or teachers, to use meaningful ways of learning. The highly significant results (p < 0.001), 38.90% of the return on error, show that concept maps are equally effective in identifying correct ideas and incorrect ideas in learners [66,68,69]. Concept maps can be as effective as lengthy clinical interviews [70], since these tools involve both hemispheres of the brain: the left brain for language, logic, rationality and the right brain for imagination, creativity, global vision and analogy as well as the spatialization of information. It combines the use of these two hemispheres in perfect synergy [71].

5. Limits

Although our neuro-pedagogical methods have given satisfactory results, it is important to address certain limitations that will be dealt with later. Indeed, our methods are not yet validated by the scientific community, and our study is limited to a sample that does not include the primary and university cycles. It would also be wise to look at teaching and to take that into consideration because it is specifically limited to the teachers’ feedback.

6. Conclusions

The contribution of neuroscience is extremely interesting. Learning has long been considered a cognitive phenomenon in the strongest sense; placed at the heart of “intelligence”, it should be studied under its “functional” aspects and not exclusively under the behavioral angle. The results of our experimental research show a significant improvement of the learning parameters, for example, the exam marks which have improved from 57.01% to 440.54%. But it should not obscure all the other insights necessary for the pedagogical act, nor should it make teachers specialists (neuropsychologist, logopedist, orthopedagogue), or experts in the functioning of the nervous system, but rather help them to know how the brain processes information and what are the cognitive and metacognitive processes involved in the learning process. This will be the subject of our next article, in which we hope to accompany teachers in the introduction of cognitive neuroscience into the school world. This would be the launch of a beneficial opportunity insofar as it would make it possible to question and renew all practices, from the division of course sequences, their content, their rhythm, and the duration of their execution, to the objectives and the mode of evaluation.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, S.L. and L.E.; methodology, S.L. and M.T.; software, S.L.; validation, S.L., M.T. and L.E.; formal analysis, S.L.; investigation, L.E.; resources, L.E.; data curation, L.E.; writing—original draft preparation, L.E.; writing—review and editing, L.E.; visualization, S.L.; supervision, S.L. and M.T.; project administration, M.T. and S.L.; All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

Ethical review and approval were waived for this study. In fact, one of the authors is a teacher. Therefore, there is no problem because he has the right to do this kind of research on his own student.

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The data presented in this study are available on request from the corresponding author. The data are not publicly available due to confidential reasons.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Cruickshank, W.M. A New Perspective in Teacher Education: The Neuroeducator: The Neuroeducator. J. Learn. Disabil. 1981, 14, 337–341. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Fischer, K.W.; Goswami, U.; Geake, J.; The Task Force on the Future of Educational Neuroscience. The Future of Educational Neuroscience. Mind Brain Educ. 2010, 4, 68–80. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Samuels, B.M. Can the Differences between Education and Neuroscience Be Overcome by Mind, Brain, and Education? Mind Brain Educ. 2009, 3, 45–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Lagemann, E.C. An Elusive Science: The Troubling History of Education Research; University of Chicago Press: Chicago, IL, USA, 2000. [Google Scholar]
  5. Bruer, J.T. Education and the Brain: A Bridge Too Far. Educ. Res. 1997, 26, 4. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Masson, S. La Neuroéducation: Mieux Comprendre Le Cerveau Pour Mieux Enseigner. Neuroeducation 2012, 1, 3–4. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  7. Ansari, D.; Coch, D. Bridges over Troubled Waters: Education and Cognitive Neuroscience. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2006, 10, 146–151. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  8. Goswami, U. Neuroscience and Education: From Research to Practice? Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2006, 7, 406–411. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  9. Howard-Jones, P.A. Neuroscience and Education: Myths and Messages. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2014, 15, 817–824. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  10. Rato, J.R.; Abreu, A.M.; Castro-Caldas, A. Achieving a Successful Relationship between Neuroscience and Education: The Views of Portuguese Teachers. Procedia Soc. Behav. Sci. 2011, 29, 879–884. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  11. Toscani, P. Editorial Du Dossier “Neurosciences et Éducation”. Éduc. Soc. 2018. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Arboix-Calas, F. Neurosciences Cognitives et Sciences de l’éducation: Vers Un Changement de Paradigme? Éduc. Soc. 2018, 49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Masson, S. Activer Ses Neurones: Pour Mieux Apprendre et Enseigner; Éditions Odile Jacob: Paris, France, 2020. [Google Scholar]
  14. Dehaene, S.; Charles, L.; King, J.-R.; Marti, S. Toward a Computational Theory of Conscious Processing. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 2014, 25, 76–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  15. Goswami, U. Neuroscience and Education. Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 2004, 74, 1–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  16. Masson, S. Enseigner Les Sciences En S’appuyant Sur La Neurodidactique Des Sciences; Potvin, D.P., Riopel, M., Masson, S., Eds.; Université du Québec à Montréal: Montreal, QC, Canada, 2007; pp. 308–321. [Google Scholar]
  17. Organisation for Economic Co-operation and Development. Annexes. In Regards sur L’éducation; Éditions OCDE: Paris, France, 2007; pp. 443–449. [Google Scholar]
  18. Ansari, D.; De Smedt, B.; Grabner, R.H. Neuroeducation–A Critical Overview of an Emerging Field. Neuroethics 2012, 5, 105–117. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Tokuhama-Espinosa, T.N. The Scientifically Substantiated Art of Teaching: A Study in the Development of Standards in the New Academic Field of Neuroeducation (Mind, Brain, and Education Science). Ph.D. Thesis, Capella University, Minneapolis, MN, USA, 2008. [Google Scholar]
  20. Masson, S. Les Apports de La Neuroéducation à l’enseignement: Des Neuromythes Aux Découvertes Actuelles. Approch. Neuropsychol. Apprentiss. Chez L’enfant 2015, 134, 11–22. [Google Scholar]
  21. Goswami, U. Mind, Brain, and Literacy: Biomarkers as Usable Knowledge for Education. Mind Brain Educ. 2009, 3, 176–184. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Diamond, A.; Amso, D. Contributions of Neuroscience to Our Understanding of Cognitive Development. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2008, 17, 136–141. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  23. Coch, D.; Fischer, K.W.; Dawson, G. (Eds.) Human Behavior, Learning, and the Developing Brain: Typical Development; Guilford Publications: New York, NY, USA, 2010. [Google Scholar]
  24. Immordino-Yang, M.H.; Christodoulou, J.A.; Singh, V. Rest Is Not Idleness: Implications of the Brain’s Default Mode for Human Development and Education. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 2012, 7, 352–364. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  25. Toscani, P. J’optimise Mes Intelligences: Les Neurosciences Au Coeur de La Classe (Cahier D’exercices); CHRONIQUE SOCIA: Angers, France, 2014. [Google Scholar]
  26. Masson, S.; Borst, G. Méthodes de Recherche En Neuroéducation; PU QUEBEC: Quebec City, QC, Canada, 2018. [Google Scholar]
  27. Jean-Luc, B.; Grégoire, B.; Mickaël, D.; Frédéric, G. Les Neurosciences Cognitives Dans La Classe: Guide Pratique Pour Experimenter et Innover; ESF Sciences Humaines: Paris, France, 2018. [Google Scholar]
  28. Tabensky, A. Spontanéité et Interaction: Le Jeu de Rôle Dans L’enseignement Des Langues Étrangères; Editions L’Harmattan: Paris, France, 1997. [Google Scholar]
  29. Leblanc, M. JEU DE RÔLE ET ENGAGEMENT: Evaluation de Linteraction Dans Les Jeux de Rôles de Français Langue Étrangère (Sémantiques); L’Harmattan: Paris, France, 2002. [Google Scholar]
  30. Bar, M. The Proactive Brain: Using Analogies and Associations to Generate Predictions. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2007, 11, 280–289. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Leech, N.L.; Onwuegbuzie, A.J. Qualitative Data Analysis: A Compendium of Techniques and a Framework for Selection for School Psychology Research and Beyond. Sch. Psychol. Q. 2008, 23, 587–604. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Cohen, M.X.; Elger, C.E.; Ranganath, C. Reward Expectation Modulates Feedback-Related Negativity and EEG Spectra. Neuroimage 2007, 35, 968–978. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  33. Gentner, D.; Holyoak, K.J. Reasoning and Learning by Analogy: Introduction. Am. Psychol. 1997, 52, 32–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  34. Miller, L. Role-Play Games as a Tool for STEM Career Inspiration and the Development of Scientific Possible Selves. In Global Learn; Association for the Advancement of Computing in Education (AACE): Chesapeake, VA, USA, 2015; pp. 385–389. [Google Scholar]
  35. Bunge, S.A.; Wendelken, C.; Badre, D.; Wagner, A.D. Analogical Reasoning and Prefrontal Cortex: Evidence for Separable Retrieval and Integration Mechanisms. Cereb. Cortex 2005, 15, 239–249. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  36. Urbanski, M.; Bréchemier, M.-L.; Garcin, B.; Bendetowicz, D.; Thiebaut de Schotten, M.; Foulon, C.; Rosso, C.; Clarençon, F.; Dupont, S.; Pradat-Diehl, P.; et al. Reasoning by Analogy Requires the Left Frontal Pole: Lesion-Deficit Mapping and Clinical Implications. Brain 2016, 139 Pt 6, 1783–1799. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Foulon, C. Implementing Advanced Neo-Associationist Analyses of the Brain. Ph.D. Thesis, Sorbonne Université, Sorbonne, France, 2018. [Google Scholar]
  38. Bilik, E. Neuropsychodrama: What Is Happening in Our Brains in Psychodrama? Rev. Bras. Psicodrama 2019, 27, 165–173. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Moreno, J.L. Who Shall Survive? Foundations of Sociometry, Group Psychotherapy and Sociodrama, 1st ed.; Mental Health Resources: Bucarest, Romania, 1977. [Google Scholar]
  40. Tremblay, L.; Schultz, W. Relative Reward Preference in Primate Orbitofrontal Cortex. Nature 1999, 398, 704–708. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  41. Elliot, T.; Plank, T.; Zindler, A.; White, W.; Bourdon, B. Element Transport from Slab to Volcanic Front at the Mariana Arc. J. Geophys. Res. 1997, 102, 14991–15019. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Bandura, A. Albert Bandura. In A History of Psychology in Autobiography; American Psychological Association: Washington, DC, USA, 2007; Volume IX, pp. 43–75. [Google Scholar]
  43. Watanabe, M. Motivational control of learning in the prefrontal cortex. Brain Nerve 2008, 60, 815–824. [Google Scholar]
  44. Deci, E.L.; Ryan, R.M. Intrinsic Motivation and Self-Determination in Human Behavior; Springer: Boston, MA, USA, 1985. [Google Scholar]
  45. Jonas, E.; McGregor, I.; Klackl, J.; Agroskin, D.; Fritsche, I.; Holbrook, C.; Nash, K.; Proulx, T.; Quirin, M. Threat and Defense: From Anxiety to Approach; Olson, J.M., Zanna, M.P., Eds.; Elsevier Academic Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2014; Volume 49, pp. 219–286. [Google Scholar]
  46. Chen, X.; Huang, Y.; Xiao, M.; Luo, Y.-J.; Liu, Y.; Song, S.; Gao, X.; Chen, H. Self and the Brain: Self-Concept Mediates the Effect of Resting-State Brain Activity and Connectivity on Self-Esteem in School-Aged Children. Pers. Individ. Differ. 2021, 168, 110287. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Decety, J.; Jackson, P.L.; Sommerville, J.A.; Chaminade, T.; Meltzoff, A.N. The Neural Bases of Cooperation and Competition: An FMRI Investigation. Neuroimage 2004, 23, 744–751. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  48. Sousa, D.A. How the Brain Learns, 5th ed.; Corwin: Thousand Oaks, CA, USA, 2016. [Google Scholar]
  49. Puce, A.; Allison, T.; Asgari, M.; Gore, J.C.; McCarthy, G. Differential Sensitivity of Human Visual Cortex to Faces, Letterstrings, and Textures: A Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Study. J. Neurosci. 1996, 16, 5205–5215. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  50. Biology, G.; Neural Networks Fossella, J.; Sommer, T.; Fan, J.; Pfaff, D.; Posner, M.I. Synaptogenesis and Heritable Aspects of Executive Attention. Ment. Retard. Dev. Disabil. Res. Rev. 2003, 9, 78–183. [Google Scholar]
  51. Tallon-Baudry, C. Attention and Awareness in Synchrony. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2004, 8, 523–525. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Lewin, D. The Pharmakon of Educational Technology: The Disruptive Power of Attention in Education. Stud. Philos. Educ. 2016, 35, 251–265. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  53. Mazô-Darné, N. Mémoriser Grâce à Nos Sens. Rech. Et Prat. Pédagogiques En Lang. De Spécialité. Cah. De L’apliut 2006, 25, 28–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  54. Kokotovich, V. Problem Analysis and Thinking Tools: An Empirical Study of Non-Hierarchical Mind Mapping. Des. Stud. 2008, 29, 49–69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Noonan, M. Mind Maps: Enhancing Midwifery Education. Nurse Educ. Today 2013, 33, 847–852. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  56. Régnard, D. Apports pédagogiques de l’utilisation de la carte heuristique en classe. ÉLA 2010, 2, 215–222. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Merchie, E.; Van Keer, H. Spontaneous Mind Map Use and Learning from Texts: The Role of Instruction and Student Characteristics. Procedia Soc. Behav. Sci. 2012, 69, 1387–1394. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Budd, J.W. Mind Maps as Classroom Exercises. J. Econ. Educ. 2004, 35, 35–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Eriksson, L.T.; Hauer, A.M. Mind Map Marketing: A Creative Approach in Developing Marketing Skills. J. Mark. Educ. 2004, 26, 174–187. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Buzan, T.; Buzan, B. The Mind Map Book: How to Use the Radiant Thinking to Maximize Your Brain’s Untapped Potential; Penguin Book Ltd.: London, UK, 1993. [Google Scholar]
  61. Buzan, T.; Griffiths, C. Le Mind Mapping Au Service Du Manager; Eyrolles, Éditions d’Organisation: Paris, France, 2011. [Google Scholar]
  62. Blunt, J.R.; Karpicke, J.D. Learning with Retrieval-Based Concept Mapping. J. Educ. Psychol. 2014, 106, 849–858. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  63. Karpicke, J.D.; Blunt, J.R. Retrieval Practice Produces More Learning than Elaborative Studying with Concept Mapping. Science 2011, 331, 772–775. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  64. Kapur, N. Remembering Norman Schwarzkopf: Evidence for Two Distinct Long-Term Fact Learning Mechanisms. Cogn. Neuropsychol. 1994, 11, 661–670. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Schroeder, N.L.; Nesbit, J.C.; Anguiano, C.J.; Adesope, O.O. Studying and Constructing Concept Maps: A Meta-Analysis. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 2018, 30, 431–455. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  66. Novak, J.D. Concept Mapping: A Useful Tool for Science Education. J. Res. Sci. Teach. 1990, 27, 937–949. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Dehaene, S. Vers Une Science de La Vie Mentale: Leçon Inaugurale Prononcée Le Jeudi 6 Avril 2006; Collège de France: Paris, France, 2006. [Google Scholar]
  68. Novak, J.D.; Gowin, D.B. Preface. In Learning How to Learn; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 1984; pp. xi–xiii. [Google Scholar]
  69. Mintzes, J.; Wandersee, J.; Novak, J. Assessing Science Understanding: A Human Constructivist View; Academic Press: San Diego, CA, USA, 2000. [Google Scholar]
  70. Edwards, J.; Fraser, K. Concept Maps as Reflectors of Conceptual Understanding. Res. Sci. Educ. 1983, 13, 19–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Meyer, P. Les cartes conceptuelles: Un outil créatif en pédagogie. Rech. Soins Infirm. 2010, 3, 35–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Percentage fields of the variations of the psycho-pedagogical parameters between the beginning and the end of the experiment in (%) for the “role play” method.
Figure 1. Percentage fields of the variations of the psycho-pedagogical parameters between the beginning and the end of the experiment in (%) for the “role play” method.
Education 11 00373 g001
Figure 2. Percentage fields of variations in psycho-pedagogical parameters between the beginning and the end of the experiment in (%) for the method “vary the access to information”.
Figure 2. Percentage fields of variations in psycho-pedagogical parameters between the beginning and the end of the experiment in (%) for the method “vary the access to information”.
Education 11 00373 g002
Figure 3. Percentage fields of the variations of the psycho-pedagogical parameters between the beginning and the end of the experiment in (%) for the “expert student” method.
Figure 3. Percentage fields of the variations of the psycho-pedagogical parameters between the beginning and the end of the experiment in (%) for the “expert student” method.
Education 11 00373 g003
Figure 4. Percentage fields of variation of psycho-pedagogical parameters between the beginning and the end of the experiment in (%) for the “mind map” method.
Figure 4. Percentage fields of variation of psycho-pedagogical parameters between the beginning and the end of the experiment in (%) for the “mind map” method.
Education 11 00373 g004
Figure 5. Percentage fields of the variations of the psycho-pedagogical parameters between the beginning and the end of the experiment in (%) for each of the neuro-pedagogical methods.
Figure 5. Percentage fields of the variations of the psycho-pedagogical parameters between the beginning and the end of the experiment in (%) for each of the neuro-pedagogical methods.
Education 11 00373 g005
Table 1. Description of teaching methods.
Table 1. Description of teaching methods.
Purpose of the MethodPedagogical ActivitiesRoles of the Teacher and the Student
Developing active engagement1. Role playingThe teacher explains the scientific notions related to the concept of the lesson (photosynthesis) and then distributes the roles.
Each student represents the role of an organelle or an element that comes into play in the process of photosynthesis.
The teacher orchestrates the game, ensuring that the students respect each other and the time.
Fostering involvement and motivation, developing personal effectiveness2. The expert studentIn the first instance small production groups are formed and composed by free choice.
The members of each group have to choose a part of the course during a previous session with their teacher, which they will prepare at home. In class, during the course session, the members of the group of the same part are called to discuss it, enrich it, and improve its presentation, so that each of them becomes an “expert”.
In a second phase, the experts of the different parts of the course are formed into “synthesis islands”, which exchange ideas, discuss, and formulate a synthesis.
The students must prepare the topics of the course before hand at home in order to understand it, enrich it, and present it better, and to agree on the modalities of work and intervention within the framework of a harmonious distribution of tasks.
Improve attention and working memory3. Varying information access channels
  • A video presenting different concepts
  • A flash simulation to get in touch with the notion or concept to be learned
  • A real experiment
  • The software for the simulation
  • A restitution on the board with the class group
Develop memorization4. Mind mapTo help the student to be in the heart of the learning process and to be an actor of the educational system. According to the OAC approach, as follows:
O: detect the characteristics of a learning situation helped by a preliminary questioning (individual or by group or brainstorming) around a documentation or a situation
A: study and decode the previous observations, conduct a common research in the same direction, and problematize through comparisons, hypotheses, tests…
C: formulate a definition of a concept or a common course summary (formulation guided by the teacher)
Table 2. Evaluation grid of the experiments conducted.
Table 2. Evaluation grid of the experiments conducted.
General Student InformationTechnical InformationVariables Measured in Pre-Test and in Post-Test
  • Sex
  • Level
Theory and practical experience
  • Attention
  • Active engagement
  • Return on error
  • Number of words memorized
Table 3. Effects of pedagogical approaches on variations in means of psychoeducational parameters.
Table 3. Effects of pedagogical approaches on variations in means of psychoeducational parameters.
Psycho-Pedagogical Parameters
Neuropedagogical MethodsPeriodAttentionActive EngagementNumber of Memorized KeywordsError CorrectionSatisfactionResult of the Written Test
Role playing (N = 26)Pre-test0.92 ± 0.71.04 ± 0.89.19 ± 3.80.65 ± 0.80.85 ± 0.510.5 ± 3.9
Post-test1.38 ± 0.8 *1.46 ± 0.8 *13.2 ± 5.2 ***1.08 ± 1 **1.65 ± 0.8 ***13.9 ± 5.1 ***
The expert student (N = 28)Pre-test1.37 ± 0.561.48 ± 0.642.38 ± 10.110.71 ± 0.91.07 ± 0.477.04 ± 6.62
Post-test2.00 ± 0.00 ***2.00 ± 0.00 ***13.74 ± 1.75 ***1.18 ± 0.9 *1.68 ± 0.72 **12.04 ± 8.04 *.
Vary the ways in which information is accessed (N = 38)Pre-test37 ± 0.60.45 ± 0.76.21 ± 3.90.32 ± 0.60.37 ± 0.66.92 ± 3.5
Post-test1.24 ± 0.5 ***1.42 ± 0.6 ***11.8 ± 2.9 ***0.88 ± 0.8 ***2 ± 0 ***10.3 ± 4.9 ***
Post-test1.79 ± 0.41 ***1.92 ± 0.27 ***15.7 ± 2.76 ***1.74 ± 0.55 ***2 ± 0 ***15.7 ± 2.58 ***
* (p < 0.05), ** (p < 0.01) and *** (p < 0.001).
Table 4. The % effect of measured variables between pre-test and post-test.
Table 4. The % effect of measured variables between pre-test and post-test.
Psycho-Pedagogical ParametersRole PlayingThe Expert StudentVary the Ways Access to InformationMind Map
Attention50%19.30%235.13%61.26%
Engagement40.38%5.15%215.55%38.13%
Return on error43.09%18.51%89.37%36.02%
Number of memorized keywords80%66.20%175%163.64%
Result of the written test94.12%57.01%440.54%181.69%
Satisfaction with the method by the students32.19%12.19%49.13%35.31%
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Elouafi, L.; Lotfi, S.; Talbi, M. Progress Report in Neuroscience and Education: Experiment of Four Neuropedagogical Methods. Educ. Sci. 2021, 11, 373. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci11080373

AMA Style

Elouafi L, Lotfi S, Talbi M. Progress Report in Neuroscience and Education: Experiment of Four Neuropedagogical Methods. Education Sciences. 2021; 11(8):373. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci11080373

Chicago/Turabian Style

Elouafi, Leila, Said Lotfi, and Mohammed Talbi. 2021. "Progress Report in Neuroscience and Education: Experiment of Four Neuropedagogical Methods" Education Sciences 11, no. 8: 373. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci11080373

APA Style

Elouafi, L., Lotfi, S., & Talbi, M. (2021). Progress Report in Neuroscience and Education: Experiment of Four Neuropedagogical Methods. Education Sciences, 11(8), 373. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci11080373

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop