Rapid Automatic Naming Performance of Young Spanish–English Speaking Children
Abstract
:1. Introduction
1.1. Language Assessment for Dual Language Learners
1.2. Rapid Automatic Naming
2. Theoretical Framework
2.1. Relationship between Rapid Automatic Naming and Performance on Emergent Literacy
2.2. Need for Additional Studies of Rapid Automatic Naming with Spanish–English speaking Dual Language Learners
2.3. Research Aims
- Is RAN a feasible task for kindergarten and first grade Spanish–English speaking DLLs who may have limited English skills?
- What is the average RAN performance of Spanish–English speaking DLLs in kindergarten and the first grade by types of symbols (numbers/letters and colors/objects)?
- What is the relationship between RAN and children’s performance in standardized language and literacy assessments for Spanish–English speaking DLLs?
3. Method
3.1. Participants
3.2. Family Characteristics
3.3. Measures
3.3.1. Rapid Automatic Naming
3.3.2. Reading Readiness
3.3.3. Nonverbal Intelligence
3.3.4. Receptive Vocabulary
3.3.5. Sentence Imitation
3.4. Procedures
4. Analyses
5. Results
5.1. Descriptive Statistics
5.2. Quantile Regression
6. Discussion
6.1. Key Findings
6.2. Interpretation and Comparison to Previous Findings
6.2.1. Feasibility of RAN for young DLLs
6.2.2. Relationship with English Vocabulary
6.2.3. Relationship to Sentence Imitation
6.3. Theoretical Considerations
6.4. Limitations
6.5. Implications
7. Recommendations for Future Studies
Acknowledgments
Author Contributions
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Kena, G., L. Musu-Gillette, J. Robinson, X. Wang, A. Rathbun, J. Zhang, S. Wilkinson-Flicker, A. Barmer, and E. Dunlop Velez. 2015. The Condition of Education 2015; NCES 2015-144; Washington, DC, USA: U.S. Department of Education. Available online: https://nces.ed.gov/pubsearch/pubsinfo.asp?pubid=2015144 (accessed on 6 July 2016).
- Kena, G., W. Hussar, J. McFarland, C. de Brey, L. Musu-Gillette, X. Wang, J. Zhang, A. Rathbun, S. Wilkinson-Flicker, M. Diliberti, and et al. 2016. The Condition of Education 2016; NCES 2016-144; Washington, DC, USA: U.S. Department of Education. Available online: https://nces.ed.gov/pubs2016/2016144.pdf (accessed on 6 July 2016).
- Everatt, J., I. Smythe, D. Ocampo, and E. Gyarmathy. 2004. Issues in the assessment of literacy-related difficulties across language backgrounds: A cross-linguistic comparison. J. Res. Read. 27: 141–151. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Branum-Martin, L., P.D. Mehta, C.D. Carlson, D.J. Francis, and C. Goldenberg. 2014. The nature of Spanish versus English language use at home. J. Educ. Psychol. 106: 181–199. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Castilla-Earls, A.P., M.A. Restrepo, A. Perez-Leroux, S. Gray, P. Holmes, D. Gail, and Z. Chen. 2016. Interactions between bilingual effects and language impairment: Exploring grammatical markers in Spanish-speaking bilingual children. Appl. Psycholinguist. 37: 1147–1173. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bedore, L., and E. Peña. 2008. Assessment of bilingual children for identification of language impairment: Current findings and implications for practice. Int. J. Biling. Educ. Biling. 11: 1–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hoff, E., C. Core, S. Place, R. Rumiche, M. Señor, and M. Parra. 2012. Dual language exposure and early bilingual development. J. Child Lang. 39: 1–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Boerma, T., and E. Blom. 2017. Assessmen of bilingual children: What if testing in both languages is not possible? J. Commun. Disord. 66: 65–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Boerma, T., S. Chiat, P. Leseman, M. Timmermeister, F. Wijnen, and E. Blom. 2015. A quasi universal nonword repetition task as a diagnostic tool for bilingual children learning Dutch as a second language. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 58: 1747–1760. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Cummins, J. 1994. The role of primary language development in promoting educational success for language minority students. In Schooling and Language Minority Students: A Theoretical Framework. Edited by C. Leyba. Los Angeles, CA, USA: California State University, pp. 3–49. [Google Scholar]
- Goodrich, M., and C. Lonigan. 2017. Language-Independent and language-specific aspects of early literacy: An evaluation of the Common Underlying Proficiency model. J. Educ. Psychol. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Language and Reading Research Consortium. 2015. The dimensionality of Spanish in young Spanish-English dual language learners. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 58: 754–766. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Edmonds, L.A., and N.J. Donovan. 2012. Item level psychometrics and predictors of performance for Spanish/English bilingual speakers on An Object and Action Naming Battery. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 55: 359–381. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Denckla, M.B., and R.G. Rudel. 1976. Naming of object-drawing by dyslexic and other learning disabled children. Brain Lang. 3: 1–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Denckla, M.B., and R.G. Rudel. 1976. Rapid ‘automatized’naming (RAN): Dyslexia differentiated from other learning disabilities. Neuropsychologia 14: 471–479. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wolf, M., G.B. Bowers, and K. Biddle. 2000. Naming-Speed processes, timing, and reading: A conceptual review. J. Learn. Disabil. 33: 387–407. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Denckla, M.B., and R. Rudel. 1974. Rapid “automatized” naming of pictured objects, colors, letters and numbers by normal children. Cortex 10: 186–202. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wolf, M., and M.B. Denckla. 2005. RAN/RAS: Rapid Automatized Naming and Rapid Alternating Stimulus Tests. Austin, TX, USA: Pro-Ed. [Google Scholar]
- González-Garrido, A.A., F.R. Gómez-Velázquez, D. Zarabozo, B.A. Ruiz-Villeda, and J.M. de la Tuya. 2011. Rapid automatized naming and lexical decision in children from an electrophysiological perspective. Clin. EEG Neurosci. 42: 14–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Badian, N. 1994. Do dyslexic and other poor readers differ in reading related cognitive skills? Read. Writ. 6: 45–63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Blachman, B.A. 1984. Relationship of rapid naming ability and language analysis skills to kindergarten and first-grade reading achievement. J. Educ. Psychol. 76: 610–622. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cornwall, A. 1992. The relationship of phonological awareness, rapid naming, and verbal memory to severe reading and spelling disability. J. Learn. Disabil. 25: 532–538. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Felton, R.H., and I.S. Brown. 1990. Phonological processes as predictors of specific reading skills in children at risk for reading failure. Read. Writ. 2: 39–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mann, V.A., and P. Ditunno. 1990. Phonological deficiencies: Effective predictors of future reading problems. In Perspectives on Dyslexia: Vol. 2: Cognition, Language, and Treatment. Edited by G. Pavlides. Chichester, UK: J. Willy & Sons, pp. 105–131. [Google Scholar]
- Wolf, M., H. Bally, and R. Morris. 1986. Automaticity, retrieval processes, and reading: A longitudinal study in average and impaired readers. Child Dev. 57: 988–1000. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Lervåg, A., and C. Hulme. 2009. Rapid automatized naming (RAN) taps a mechanism that places constraints on the development of early reading fluency. Psychol. Sci. 20: 1040–1048. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ackerman, P.T., and R.A. Dykman. 1993. Phonological processes, confrontational naming, and immediate memory in dyslexia. J. Learn. Disabil. 26: 597–609. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Badian, N.A. 1993. Phonemic awareness, naming, visual symbol processing, and reading. Read. Writ. Interdiscip. J. 5: 87–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- De Groot, B.J., K.P. Van den Bos, B.F. Van der Meulen, and A.E. Minnaert. 2015. Rapid naming and phonemic awareness in children with reading disabilities and/or specific language impairment: Differentiating Processes? J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 55: 1538–1548. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Denckla, M.B. 1972. Color-naming defects in dyslexic boys. Cortex 8: 164–176. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fawcett, A.J., and R.I. Nicolson. 1994. Naming speed in children with dyslexia. J. Learn. Disabil. 27: 641–646. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Korhonen, T.T. 1995. The persistence of rapid naming problems in children with reading disabilities: A nine-year follow-up. J. Learn. Disabil. 28: 232–239. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Plaza, M., and H. Cohen. 2005. Influence of auditory–verbal, visual–verbal, visual, and visual–visual processing speed on reading and spelling at the end of Grade 1. Brain Cognit. 57: 189–194. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Wimmer, H. 1993. Characteristics of developmental dyslexia in a regular writing system. Appl. Psycholing. 14: 1–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- López-Escribano, C. 2016. Training Reading Fluency and Comprehension of Spanish Children with Dyslexia. In Reading Fluency. Edited by A. Khateb and I. Bar-Kochva. Cham, Switzerland: Springer International Publishing, pp. 141–161. [Google Scholar]
- Catts, H.W., S.M. Adlof, T.P. Hogan, and S.E. Weismer. 2005. Are specific language impairment and dyslexia distinct disorders? J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 48: 1378–1396. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Conti-Ramsden, G., and K. Durkin. 2007. Phonological short-term memory, language and literacy: Developmental relationships in early adolescence in young people with SLI. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 48: 147–156. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Gorman, B.K. 2012. Relationships between vocabulary size, working memory, and phonological awareness in Spanish-speaking English language learners. Am. J. Speech-Lang. Pathol. 21: 109–123. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rispens, J., and A. Baker. 2012. Nonword repetition: The relative contributions of phonological short-term memory and phonological representations in children with language and reading impairment. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 55: 683–694. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kirby, J.R., G.K. Georgiou, R. Martinussen, and R. Parrila. 2010. Review of research: Naming speed and reading—From prediction to instruction. Read. Res. Q. 45: 341–362. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Torgesen, J.K., R.K. Wagner, and C.A. Rashotte. 1997. Prevention and remediation of severe reading disabilities: Keeping the end in mind. Sci. Stud. Read. 1: 217–234. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bowers, P.G. 1995. Tracing symbol naming speed’s unique contributions to reading disabilities over time. Read. Writ. 7: 189–216. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kail, R., and L.K. Hall. 1994. Speed of processing, naming speed, and reading. Dev. Psychol. 30: 949–954. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wimmer, H., H. Mayringer, and T. Raberger. 1999. Reading and Dual-Task Balancing Evidence against the Automatization Deficit Explanation of Developmental Dyslexia. J. Learn. Disabil. 32: 473–478. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ziegler, J.C., D. Bertrand, D. Tóth, V. Csépe, A. Reis, L. Faísca, and L. Blomert. 2010. Orthographic depth and its impact on universal predictors of reading a cross-language investigation. Psychol. Sci. 21: 551–559. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- López Escribano, C. 2007. Evaluation of the double-deficit hypothesis subtype classification of readers in Spanish. J. Learn. Disabil. 40: 319–330. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Norton, E.S., and M. Wolf. 2012. Rapid automatized naming (RAN) and reading fluency: Implications for understanding and treatment of reading disabilities. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 63: 427–452. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Powell, D., R. Stainthorp, and M. Stuart. 2014. Deficits in orthographic knowledge in children poor at rapid automatized naming (RAN) tasks? Sci. Stud. Read. 18: 192–207. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rodríguez, C., M. van den Boer, J.E. Jiménez, and P.F. de Jong. 2015. Developmental changes in the relations between RAN, phonological awareness, and Reading in Spanish children. Sci. Stud. Read. 19: 273–288. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Neuhaus, G.F., and P.R. Swank. 2002. Understanding the relations between RAN letter subtest components and word reading in first-grade students. J. Learn. Disabil. 35: 158–174. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bowers, P.G., and M. Wolf. 1993. Theoretical links among naming speed, precise timing mechanisms and orthographic skill in dyslexia. Read. Writ. 5: 69–85. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bowers, P.G., and L.B. Swanson. 1991. Naming speed deficits in reading disability: Multiple measures of a singular process. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 51: 195–219. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Young, A., and P.G. Bowers. 1995. Individual difference and text difficulty determinants of reading fluency and expressiveness. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 60: 428–454. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wolf, M., A.G. O’rourke, C. Gidney, M. Lovett, P. Cirino, and R. Morris. 2002. The second deficit: An investigation of the independence of phonological and naming-speed deficits in developmental dyslexia. Read. Writ. 15: 43–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Manis, F.R., L.M. Doi, and B. Bhadha. 2000. Naming speed, phonological awareness, and orthographic knowledge in second graders. J. Learn. Disabil. 33: 325–333. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kohnert, K.J., E. Bates, and A.E. Hernandez. 1999. Balancing bilinguals: Lexical-semantic production and cognitive processing in children learning Spanish and English. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 42: 1400–1413. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Gollan, T.H., R.I. Montoya, C. Fennema-Notestine, and S.K. Morris. 2005. Bilingualism affects picture naming but not picture classification. Mem. Cognit. 33: 1220–1234. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Reardon, S.F., D. Kalogrides, A. Ho, B. Shear, K. Shores, and E. Fahle. Standford Educational Data Archive, 2016. Stanford Digital Repository. Available online: https://purl.stanford.edu/db586ns4974 (accessed on 31 July 2017).
- Woodcock, R.W. 2011. Woodcock Reading Mastery Tests, Third Edition (WRMT). San Antonio, TX, USA: Pearson. [Google Scholar]
- Ehrler, D.J., and M.L. McGhee. 2008. Primary Test of Nonverbal Intelligence (PTONI). Austin, TX, USA: Clocktower Hill Research and Publishing Group, LLC. [Google Scholar]
- Dunn, L.M., and L.M. Dunn. 2007. Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-Fourth. Circle Pines, MN, USA: American Guidance Service. [Google Scholar]
- Dunn, L., S. Lugo, R. Padilla, and L. Dunn. 1986. Test de Vocabulario en Imágenes Peabody. [Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test]. Circle Pines, MN, USA: American Guidance Service. [Google Scholar]
- Peña, E.D., V.F. Gutiérrez-Clellen, A. Iglesias, B.A. Goldstein, and L.M. Bedore. 2013. Bilingual English Spanish Assessment (BESA). San Diego, CA, USA: AR-Clinical Publications. [Google Scholar]
- Petscher, Y. 2016. Do our means of inquiry match our intentions? Front. Psychol. 7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Petscher, Y., J.A.R. Logan, and C. Zhou. 2013. Extending conditional means modeling: An introduction to quantile regression. In Applied Quantitative Analysis in Education and Social Sciences. Edited by Y. Petscher, C. Schatschneider and D.I. Compton. New York, NY, USA: Routledge, pp. 3–33. [Google Scholar]
- Peña, E.D., V. Gutiérrez-Clellen, A. Iglesias, B. Goldstein, and L.M. Bedore. 2014. BESA: Bilingual English Spanish Assessment Manual. San Rafael, CA, USA: AR-Clinical Publications. [Google Scholar]
- Archibald, L.M.D., and M.F. Joanisse. 2009. On the sensitivity and specificity of nonword repetition and sentence recall to language and memory impairments in children. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 52: 899–914. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Stokes, S.F., A.M.Y. Wong, P. Fletcher, and L.B. Leonard. 2006. Nonword repetition and sentence repetition as clinical markers of SLI: The case of Cantonese. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 49: 219–236. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- De Jong, P.F. 2011. What discrete and serial rapid automatized naming can reveal about reading. Sci. Stud. Read. 15: 314–337. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Kindergarten (n = 128) | 1st Grade (n = 147) | |||
---|---|---|---|---|
n | M (SD) | n | M (SD) | |
Age (months) | 128 | 66.30 (4.27) | 147 | 79.17 (5.04) |
(60–83) | (72–98) | |||
n | % | n | % | |
Gender | 128 | 100 | 147 | 100 |
Male | 64 | 50 | 77 | 52 |
Female | 64 | 50 | 70 | 48 |
Reported Primary Language | 56 | 44 | 107 | 73 |
Spanish | 22 | 39 | 49 | 46 |
Both | 27 | 48 | 55 | 51 |
English | 7 | 13 | 3 | 3 |
Free/Reduced Lunch | 88 | 69 | 112 | 76 |
Free | 84 | 95 | 104 | 93 |
Reduced | 3 | 3 | 4 | 4 |
Ethnicity | 55 | 43 | 107 | 73 |
Hispanic/Latino | 54 | 98 | 106 | 99 |
Declined to report | 1 | 2 | 1 | 1 |
n | School Population 1 | % F/R Lunch 2 | % Hispanic in School | % F/R Lunch in District | % Hispanic in District | % EL in District | White–Hispanic Gap 3 | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
School A | 32 | 362 | 82 | 52 | 83.1 | 19.2 | 6.2 | 0.229 (SE = 0.161) |
School B | 32 | 145 | 99 | 93.3 | 39.1 | 25.9 | 12.2 | 0.708 (SE = 0.036) |
School C | 12 | 1042 | 86 | 63 | 51.9 | 31.4 | 11.3 | 0.614 (SE = 0.009) |
School D | 21 | 358 | 82 | 51.7 | 31.1 | 19.0 | 8.3 | 0.774 (SE = 0.014) |
School E | 48 | 1250 | 78 | 62.6 | 83.0 | 45.3 | 28.8 | 0.255 (SE = 0.016) |
School F | 44 | 907 | 82 | 52 | 51.9 | 31.4 | 11.3 | 0.614 (SE = 0.009) |
School G | 23 | 944 | 94 | 53 | 65.9 | 67.6 | 19.6 | 0.516 (SE = 0.009) |
School H | 6 | 258 | 77 | 25 | 83.1 | 19.2 | 6.2 | 0.229 (SE = 0.161) |
Kindergarten (n = 128) n% | 1st Grade (n = 147) n% | |||
---|---|---|---|---|
Country of Origin | 56 | 44 | 107 | 73 |
Mexico | 23 | 41 | 51 | 48 |
Guatemala | 11 | 20 | 19 | 18 |
Other | 16 | 28 | 29 | 26 |
Primary Household Language | 55 | 43 | 107 | 73 |
Spanish | 30 | 55 | 66 | 62 |
English | 1 | 2 | 5 | 5 |
Both | 24 | 43 | 36 | 33 |
Mother‘s Education | 53 | 41 | 102 | 69 |
Beyond high school | 6 | 11 | 16 | 16 |
High school/GED | 21 | 40 | 25 | 24 |
Some high school | 4 | 7 | 17 | 17 |
Less than high school | 22 | 42 | 44 | 43 |
Father‘s Education | 46 | 36 | 99 | 67 |
Beyond high school | 2 | 4 | 7 | 7 |
High school/GED | 14 | 30 | 19 | 19 |
Some high school | 10 | 22 | 21 | 21 |
Less than high school | 20 | 44 | 52 | 53 |
Mother‘s Occupation | 54 | 42 | 100 | 68 |
Professional | 0 | 0 | 3 | 2 |
Skilled Labor | 4 | 7 | 11 | 11 |
Unskilled Labor | 21 | 39 | 29 | 29 |
Unemployed | 29 | 54 | 57 | 57 |
Father‘s Occupation | 48 | 38 | 91 | 62 |
Professional | 0 | 0 | 2 | 2 |
Skilled Labor | 19 | 40 | 49 | 54 |
Unskilled Labor | 25 | 52 | 40 | 44 |
Unemployed | 4 | 8 | 0 | 0 |
Children with RAN | Children Unable to Complete RAN | Full Sample | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Measure | n | Raw M (SD) | SS M (SD) | n | Raw M (SD) | SS M (SD) | n | Raw M (SD) | SS M (SD) |
Age (mos) | 203 | 74.76 (7.73) | 72 | 68.71 (6.88) | 275 | 73.18 (7.96) | |||
RAN | 203 | 19.83 (7.12) | 96.61 (12.78) | 203 | 19.83 (7.12) | 96.61 (12.78) | |||
Vocabulary | |||||||||
PPVT-4 | 201 | 79.69 (21.89) | 85.10 (13.11) | 68 | 52.37 (25.82) | 71.19 (14.5) | 269 | 72.78 (25.80) | 81.59 (14.75) |
TVIP | 170 | 27.82 (19.61) | 80.03 (17.02) | 51 | 26.98 (18.20) | 83.91 (19.60) | 221 | 27.63 (19.26) | 81.09 (17.78) |
Nonverbal Intelligence | |||||||||
PTONI | 200 | 32.59 (11.10) | 93.13 (19.22) | 71 | 24.63 (10.60) | 88.07 (18.23) | 271 | 30.50 (11.49) | 91.79 (19.06) |
Reading | |||||||||
WRMT-LI | 96 | 15.45 (3.28) | 97.24 (12.72) | 32 | 9.25 (6.37) | 82.47 (15.68) | 128 | 13.90 (5.02) | 93.55 (14.91) |
WRMT-PA | 75 | 20.72 (6.68) | 95.40 (16.86) | 15 | 18.33 (8.40) | 91.13 (25.06) | 90 | 20.32 (7.00) | 94.69 (18.38) |
Readiness | 74 | 95.86 (14.67) | 74 | 95.86 (14.67) |
1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 | 6 | 7 | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1. RAN | --- | 0.202 | 0.001 | 0.260 ** | 0.049 | 0.237 ** | 0.105 |
2. Letter ID | 0.295 | --- | 0.154 | 0.191 | 0.227 | 0.078 | −0.075 |
3. PA | 0.062 | 0.316 | --- | 0.367 ** | 0.109 | 0.190 | 0.424 ** |
4. PPVT | 0.023 | 0.475 ** | 0.440 ** | --- | 0.065 | 0.286 ** | 0.413 ** |
5. TVIP | 0.281 * | 0.296 | −0.102 | −0.126 | --- | −0.011 | 0.107 |
6. PTONI | 0.196 | 0.478 ** | 0.553 ** | 0.504 ** | 0.005 | --- | 0.356 ** |
7. Sentence Repetition (English) | 0.019 | 0.131 | 0.528 * | 0.579 ** | −0.338 ** | 0.347 ** | --- |
© 2017 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Wood, C.L.; Bustamante, K.N.; Fitton, L.M.; Brown, D.M.; Petscher, Y. Rapid Automatic Naming Performance of Young Spanish–English Speaking Children. Languages 2017, 2, 13. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages2030013
Wood CL, Bustamante KN, Fitton LM, Brown DM, Petscher Y. Rapid Automatic Naming Performance of Young Spanish–English Speaking Children. Languages. 2017; 2(3):13. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages2030013
Chicago/Turabian StyleWood, Carla L., Kristina N. Bustamante, Lisa M. Fitton, Dana M. Brown, and Yaacov Petscher. 2017. "Rapid Automatic Naming Performance of Young Spanish–English Speaking Children" Languages 2, no. 3: 13. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages2030013
APA StyleWood, C. L., Bustamante, K. N., Fitton, L. M., Brown, D. M., & Petscher, Y. (2017). Rapid Automatic Naming Performance of Young Spanish–English Speaking Children. Languages, 2(3), 13. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages2030013