Next Article in Journal
Old and New Approaches to Animal Cognition: There Is Not “One Cognition”
Next Article in Special Issue
The Evidence for Geary’s Theory on the Role of Mitochondrial Functioning in Human Intelligence Is Not Entirely Convincing
Previous Article in Journal
The Contribution of Executive Functions in Predicting Mathematical Creativity in Typical Elementary School Classes: A Twofold Role for Updating
Previous Article in Special Issue
Déjà vu All Over Again: A Unitary Biological Mechanism for Intelligence Is (Probably) Untenable
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Commentary

The Mitochondrial Theory of g Is Incompatible with Genetic Evidence and Does Not Explain Statistical Phenomena

by
Péter Przemyslaw Ujma
1,2 and
Kristof Kovacs
3,*
1
Institute of Behavioural Sciences, Semmelweis University, 1089 Budapest, Hungary
2
National Institute of Clinical Neuroscience, 1145 Budapest, Hungary
3
Institute of Psychology, ELTE Eotvos Lorand University, 1064 Budapest, Hungary
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Submission received: 7 May 2020 / Revised: 18 June 2020 / Accepted: 22 June 2020 / Published: 28 June 2020
In two recent reviews (Geary 2018, 2019), Geary attributed a substantial role in generating individual differences in the general factor of intelligence, g, to mitochondrial functioning. While understanding the appeal of reducing a complex psychological phenomenon to an elementary biological cause and providing a new lease to Spearman’s theory of g as mental energy, we find the evidence supporting the theory to be rough-and-ready, indirect, or even contradictory. In particular, the theory lacks specificity in describing the causal path from mitochondria to g in two respects: (1) it would imply that genetic effects on g would exert their effect on mitochondria, which is at odds with current genetic evidence; (2) if g reflects variation in mitochondrial functioning and thus differences in g loadings necessarily indicate differences in the extent to which performance on a test depends on mitochondrial functioning, then the theory fails to account for why the effect of mitochondrial functioning on performance is greater in tests that have higher across-domain correlations.
First, the theory is contradicted by genetic studies of g. Cognitive ability is strongly heritable: based on quantitative genetic studies, in childhood around 50%, and in adulthood up to 80% of individual differences in cognitive ability or IQ scores can be ascribed to genetic differences (Plomin and Deary 2015; Polderman et al. 2015). Most studies use simple sum scores or first unrotated principal components of multiple cognitive tests as the dependent variable in quantitative genetic studies. However, when cognitive ability is decomposed to its hierarchical factor structure, g usually turns out to be even more heritable, while in less general abilities, genetic factors play a progressively weaker and environmental variables a progressively stronger role (Shikishima et al. 2009; Panizzon et al. 2014).
Quantitative genetic studies remain agnostic about the nature of the genetic determinants of a trait. However, recent large-scale genome-wide association studies (GWASs) revealed a large number of single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) associated with cognitive ability (Lam et al. 2017; Savage et al. 2017; Trampush et al. 2017; Zabaneh et al. 2017; Davies et al. 2018; Hill et al. 2018). Even larger studies investigating the genetic correlates of educational attainment (Rietveld et al. 2013; Okbay et al. 2016; Lee et al. 2018) also found genetic variants which predicted cognitive ability. GWAS-derived polygenic scores currently predict up to 10% of cognitive performance (Lee et al. 2018; Allegrini et al. 2019). Within-family studies indicate that this effect size may be inflated due to population stratification, but a substantial proportion is still retained (Selzam et al. 2019). The functional interpretation of these genetic variants is not simple, because (1) many SNP hits are in intergenic regions with an unknown function, and (2) SNP hits are not necessarily causal for g; it is very likely that they are merely in linkage disequilibrium with truly functional variants in neighboring genomic regions. Still, multiple attempts have been made to at least approximately interpret the biological function of g-associated SNPs. These studies have unequivocally shown that g-associated genetic variants are primarily expressed in the brain, in specific brain regions and specific cell types, and they are implicated in very specific cellular functions, none of which concern mitochondria (Lam et al. 2017; Sniekers et al. 2017; Davies et al. 2018; Hill et al. 2018; Lee et al. 2018; Savage et al. 2018; Coleman et al. 2019). These findings are summarized in Table 1.
The current evidence suggests that the known genetic variants associated with individual differences in g affect specific areas of the brain, more specifically the frontal and anterior cingulate cortex, the cerebellum and certain subcortical structures. They seem to be expressed in specific cell types, and their functional role seems to be concentrated in neurogenesis, neuronal development and synaptic functions. This is in accordance with the watershed model that proposes that the causal effect of genotypes on intelligence as an observed phenotype is exerted through intermediate endophenotypes (Kievit et al. 2016).
In our view, these results are incompatible with individual differences in mitochondrial functioning playing a major role in creating individual differences in g. Mitochondria are present in all cells in all human tissues. If differences in mitochondrial function were to underlie individual differences in g, then g-associated genetic variants would not be expressed in specific tissues and cell types only. This would especially be the case if—as the hypothesis put forward by Geary (Geary 2018, 2019) suggests—the correlation between g and physical health (Calvin et al. 2011; Deary et al. 2019) exists because the same differences in mitochondrial functioning that create higher g in the central nervous system result in better physical health and greater longevity through their effects in other tissues. In our view, the functional role of g-associated SNPs is more consistent with the hypothesis that a large number of diverse, minor tissue- and cell-specific differences in the nervous system underlie g.
Second, since reflective latent variable models require a realistic ontology (Borsboom et al. 2003), if g in fact represents mitochondrial functioning, then in reflective models, g loadings must represent the extent of mitochondrial involvement. Since the general factor is a simple algebraic consequence of the positive manifold (Krijnen 2004), “it is always important to remember that it is the positive manifold, not g as such, that needs explanation” (Mackintosh 2011, p. 165). Therefore, translating g loadings in terms of the positive manifold itself, this means that the tests that correlate most strongly with other tests that have different content are the ones in which variation in performance depends most on mitochondrial functioning—according to Geary’s theory.
Hence, the theory should provide hypotheses regarding why such differences between g loadings in different tests depend on the relative involvement of mitochondrial function or energy. For instance, it is generally found that the more complex a task, the higher its g loading. However, complexity is not identical to difficulty. There are a number of manipulations that are able to increase g loadings, and the theory should be able to account for these. Why is mitochondrial functioning more relevant for backward digit span than for forward digit span? For odd-one-out reaction time than for simple reaction time?
Additionally, the factor Gf is found to be identical or near-identical to g (Gustafsson 1984; Kan et al. 2011), pointing to the centrality of fluid/inductive reasoning in g. Why does an inductive reasoning task, such as the completion of a number series or an incomplete matrix, require much more energy than a difficult short term memory task or a speed test, which requires one to work as fast as one can?
Even if the ultimate cause of differences in g loadings is mitochondrial functioning, a proximate psychological or physiological mechanism is needed that mediates this effect. Simply presuming that the stronger g loading is the result of such tasks’ higher “energy requirement”, without further explanation, would be tautological. If mitochondrial energy is more important for certain tasks and specific ability factors that are also most strongly related to g, then it should be explained why that is the case.
For the above reasons, our view is that individual differences in mitochondrial functioning probably do not underlie individual differences in g. The quest for the equivalent of psychological g, the common cause for the covariance in the performance on diverse cognitive tests, is still ongoing. In the meantime, theories and models that actually explain such covariance without assuming a common cause in the first place (Kovacs and Conway 2016; Savi et al. 2019; Van Der Maas et al. 2006) should probably also be considered.

Author Contributions

P.P.U. and K.K. both contributed to the writing of the manuscript with P.P.U. focusing on the first, K.K. on the second part of the argument. Both authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

The second author received funding by the National Research, Development and Innovation Office of Hungary: Grant PD-17-125360 and Grant KH-18-130424.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Allegrini, Andrea G., Saskia Selzam, Kaili Rimfeld, Sophie von Stumm, Jean-Baptiste Pingault, and Robert Plomin. 2019. Genomic prediction of cognitive traits in childhood and adolescence. Molecular Psychiatry 24: 819–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Borsboom, Denny, Gideon J. Mellenbergh, and Jaap Van Heerden. 2003. The theoretical status of latent variables. Psychological Review 110: 203–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  3. Calvin, Catherine M., Ian J. Deary, Candida Fenton, Beverly A. Roberts, Geoff Der, Nicola Leckenby, and G. David Batty. 2011. Intelligence in youth and all-cause-mortality: Systematic review with meta-analysis. International Journal of Epidemiology 40: 626–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Coleman, Jonathan R. I., Julien Bryois, Héléna A. Gaspar, Philip R. Jansen, Jeanne E. Savage, Nathan Skene, Robert Plomin, Ana B. Muñoz-Manchado, Sten Linnarsson, Greg Crawford, and et al. 2019. Biological annotation of genetic loci associated with intelligence in a meta-analysis of 87,740 individuals. Molecular Psychiatry 24: 182–97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  5. Davies, Gail, Max Lam, Sarah E. Harris, Joey W. Trampush, Michelle Luciano, W. David Hill, Saskia P. Hagenaars, Stuart J. Ritchie, Riccardo E. Marioni, Chloe Fawns-Ritchie, and et al. 2018. Study of 300,486 individuals identifies 148 independent genetic loci influencing general cognitive function. Nature Communications 9: 2098. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  6. Deary, Ian J., Sarah E. Harris, and W. David Hill. 2019. What genome-wide association studies reveal about the association between intelligence and physical health, illness, and mortality. Current Opinion in Psychology 27: 6–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Geary, David C. 2018. Efficiency of mitochondrial functioning as the fundamental biological mechanism of general intelligence (g). Psychological Review 125: 1028–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Geary, David C. 2019. Mitochondria as the Linchpin of General Intelligence and the Link between g, Health, and Aging. Journal of Intelligence 7: 25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  9. Gustafsson, Jan-Eric. 1984. A unifying model for the structure of intellectual abilities. Intelligence 8: 179–203. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Hill, W. David, Ruben C. Arslan, Charley Xia, Michelle Luciano, Carmen Amador, Pau Navarro, Caroline Hayward, R. Nagy, D. J. Porteous, Andrew M. McIntosh, and et al. 2018. Genomic analysis of family data reveals additional genetic effects on intelligence and personality. Molecular Psychiatry 23: 2347–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  11. Kan, Kees-Jan, Rogier A. Kievit, Conor Dolan, and Han van der Maas. 2011. On the interpretation of the CHC factor Gc. Intelligence 39: 292–302. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Kievit, Rogier A., Simon W. Davis, John Griffiths, Marta M. Correia, and Richard N. Henson. 2016. A watershed model of individual differences in fluid intelligence. Neuropsychologia 91: 186–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  13. Kovacs, Kristof, and Andrew R. A. Conway. 2016. Process Overlap Theory: A Unified Account of the General Factor of Intelligence. Psychological Inquiry 27: 151–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Krijnen, Wim P. 2004. Positive loadings and factor correlations from positive covariance matrices. Psychometrika 69: 655–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Lam, Max, Joey W. Trampush, Jin Yu, Emma Knowles, Gail Davies, David C. Liewald, John M. Starr, Srdjan Djurovic, Ingrid Melle, Kjetil Sundet, and et al. 2017. Large-Scale Cognitive GWAS Meta-Analysis Reveals Tissue-Specific Neural Expression and Potential Nootropic Drug Targets. Cell Reports 21: 2597–613. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  16. Lee, James J., Robbee Wedow, Aysu Okbay, Edward Kong, Omeed Maghzian, Meghan Zacher, Tuan Anh Nguyen-Viet, Peter Bowers, Julia Sidorenko, Richard Karlsson Linnér, and et al. 2018. Gene discovery and polygenic prediction from a genome-wide association study of educational attainment in 1.1 million individuals. Nature Genetics 50: 1112–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  17. Mackintosh, Nicholas J. 2011. IQ and Human Intelligence. Oxford: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
  18. Okbay, Aysu, Jonathan P. Beauchamp, Mark Alan Fontana, James J. Lee, Tune H. Pers, Cornelius A. Rietveld, Patrick Turley, Guo-Bo Chen, Valur Emilsson, S. Fleur W. Meddens, and et al. 2016. Genome-wide association study identifies 74 loci associated with educational attainment. Nature 533: 539–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  19. Panizzon, Matthew S., Eero Vuoksimaa, Kelly M. Spoon, Kristen C. Jacobson, Michael J. Lyons, Carol E. Franz, Hong Xian, Terrie Vasilopoulos, and William S. Kremen. 2014. Genetic and Environmental Influences of General Cognitive Ability: Is g a valid latent construct? Intelligence 43: 65–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  20. Plomin, Robert, and Ian J. Deary. 2015. Genetics and intelligence differences: Five special findings. Molecular Psychiatry 20: 98–108. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  21. Polderman, Tinca J. C., Beben Benyamin, Christiaan A. De Leeuw, Patrick F. Sullivan, Arjen Van Bochoven, Peter M. Visscher, and Danielle Posthuma. 2015. Meta-analysis of the heritability of human traits based on fifty years of twin studies. Nature Genetics 47: 702–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  22. Rietveld, Cornelius A., Sarah E. Medland, Jaime Derringer, Jian Yang, Tõnu Esko, Nicolas W. Martin, Harm-Jan Westra, Konstantin Shakhbazov, Abdel Abdellaoui, Arpana Agrawam, and et al. 2013. GWAS of 126,559 Individuals Identifies Genetic Variants Associated with Educational Attainment. Science (New York, NY) 340: 1467–71. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  23. Savage, Jeanne E., Philip R. Jansen, Sven Stringer, Kyoko Watanabe, Julien Bryois, Christiaan A. de Leeuw, Mats Nagel, Swapnil Awasthi, Peter B Barr, Jonathan R. I. Coleman, and et al. 2017. GWAS meta-analysis (N = 279,930) identifies new genes and functional links to intelligence. bioRxiv. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Savage, Jeanne E., Philip R. Jansen, Sven Stringer, Kyoko Watanabe, Julien Bryois, Christiaan A. De Leeuw, Mats Nagel, Swapnil Awasthi, Peter B. Barr, Jonathan R. I. Coleman, and et al. 2018. Genome-wide association meta-analysis in 269,867 individuals identifies new genetic and functional links to intelligence. Nature Genetics 50: 912–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  25. Savi, Alexander O., Maarten Marsman, Han L. J. van der Maas, and Gunter K. J. Maris. 2019. The Wiring of Intelligence. Perspectives on Psychological Science 14: 1034–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  26. Selzam, Saskia, Stuart J. Ritchie, Jean-Baptiste Pingault, Chandra A. Reynolds, Paul F. O’Reilly, and Robert Plomin. 2019. Comparing Within- and Between-Family Polygenic Score Prediction. The American Journal of Human Genetics 105: 351–63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  27. Shikishima, Chizuru, Kai Hiraishi, Shinji Yamagata, Yutaro Sugimoto, Ryo Takemura, Koken Ozaki, Mitsuhiro Okada, Tatsushi Toda, and Juko Ando. 2009. Is g an entity? A Japanese twin study using syllogisms and intelligence tests. Intelligence 37: 256–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Sniekers, Suzanne, Sven Stringer, Kyoko Watanabe, Philip R. Jansen, Jonathan R. I. Coleman, Eva Krapohl, Erdogan Taskesen, Anke R. Hammerschlag, Aysu Okbay, Delilah Zabaneh, and et al. 2017. Genome-wide association meta-analysis of 78,308 individuals identifies new loci and genes influencing human intelligence. Nature Genetics 49: 1107–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Trampush, Joey W., Max Lam Zhan Yang, Jin Yu, Emma Knowles, Gail Davies, David C. Liewald, John M. Starr, S. Djurovic, I. Melle, K. Sundet, and et al. 2017. GWAS meta-analysis reveals novel loci and genetic correlates for general cognitive function: A report from the COGENT consortium. Molecular Psychiatry 22: 336. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Van Der Maas, Han L. J., Conor V. Dolan, Raoul PPP Grasman, Jelte M. Wicherts, Hilde M. Huizenga, and Maartje E. J. Raijmakers. 2006. A dynamical model of general intelligence: The positive manifold of intelligence by mutualism. Psychological Review 113: 842–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Zabaneh, Delilah, Eva Krapohl, H. A. Gaspar, Charles Curtis, S. Hong Lee, Hamel Patel, Stephen Newhouse, H. M. Wu, M. A. Simpson, M. Putallaz, and et al. 2017. A genome-wide association study for extremely high intelligence. Molecular Psychiatry 23: 1226–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
Table 1. Selected studies about the biological function of g-associated genetic variants. The last three columns highlight the organs, organ regions (typically brain regions) and cell types in which the genes mapped to g-associated single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) were significantly enriched. We note that most of these studies used genetic data from multiple overlapping cohorts, hence cannot be considered independent.
Table 1. Selected studies about the biological function of g-associated genetic variants. The last three columns highlight the organs, organ regions (typically brain regions) and cell types in which the genes mapped to g-associated single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) were significantly enriched. We note that most of these studies used genetic data from multiple overlapping cohorts, hence cannot be considered independent.
StudyData SourceNOrganRegionCell Type or Function
Lam et al. 2017Multiple cohorts also used in the Sniekers et al. 2017; Trampush et al. 2017; Okbay et al. 2016 GWASs.107,207Brain, pituitaryCerebellar hemisphere, cerebellum, frontal cortex, cortex, anterior cingulate, nucleus accumbens, caudate nucleus, hypothalamus, hippocampus, putamen, amygdalaNeuron, neuron projection, neurogenesis, synapses, dendrites, synapse organization
Savage et al. 2018UK Biobank, COGENT consortium and 12 other sources269,867BrainAmygdala, anterior cingulate cortex, caudate nucleus, cerebellar hemisphere, cerebellum, cortex, frontal cortex, hippocampus, hypothalamus, nucleus accumbens, putamenMedium spiny neuron, pyramidal (somatosensory, hippocampal CA1)
Davies et al. 2018CHARGE and COGENT consortia, UK Biobank300,486Brain, pituitaryCerebellum, cerebellar hemisphere, cortex, frontal cortex, hippocampus, nucleus accumbens, hypothalamus, amygdala, caudate nucleus, putamen, substantia nigra, pituitaryNeurogenesis, regulation of nervous system development, neuron projection, nervous system development, neuron differentiation, regulation of cell development, dendrites
Hill et al. 2018Meta-analysis of the Sniekers et al. 2017; Okbay et al. 2016 GWASs, UK Biobank248,482Brain, pituitaryCerebellar hemisphere, cerebellum, frontal cortex, cortex, anterior cingulate, nucleus accumbens, hippocampus, amygdala, hypothalamus, caudate nucleus, putamen, substantia nigraNeurogenesis, nervous system development, cell development, neuron projection, CNS neuron differentiation, synapse, neuron differentiation, oligodendrocyte differentiation
Coleman et al. 2019Meta-analysis of the Zabaneh et al. 2017; Sniekers et al. 2017 GWASs87,740Brain, pituitaryFrontal cortexPyramidal (somatosensory, hippocampal CA1), medium spiny neuron, embryonic GABAergic neuron, serotonergic neuron

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Przemyslaw Ujma, P.; Kovacs, K. The Mitochondrial Theory of g Is Incompatible with Genetic Evidence and Does Not Explain Statistical Phenomena. J. Intell. 2020, 8, 27. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence8030027

AMA Style

Przemyslaw Ujma P, Kovacs K. The Mitochondrial Theory of g Is Incompatible with Genetic Evidence and Does Not Explain Statistical Phenomena. Journal of Intelligence. 2020; 8(3):27. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence8030027

Chicago/Turabian Style

Przemyslaw Ujma, Péter, and Kristof Kovacs. 2020. "The Mitochondrial Theory of g Is Incompatible with Genetic Evidence and Does Not Explain Statistical Phenomena" Journal of Intelligence 8, no. 3: 27. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence8030027

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop