The Relation of Scientific Creativity and Evaluation of Scientific Impact to Scientific Reasoning and General Intelligence
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Theoretical Basis
3. Study 1
3.1. Method
3.1.1. Participants
3.1.2. Materials
- (a)
- Letter Sets. For each item, participants saw five sets of four letters. They had to circle one set of letters that did not belong with the other four sets. For example, it might be that four sets of letters each had one vowel and one set of letters had no vowels. This test, with 15 items, was timed for 7 min. The test measures the fluid aspect of general intelligence.
- (b)
- Number Series. Participants saw series of numbers. They had to indicate which number came next in each series. For example, they might be given a series of numbers in which each successive number was a multiple of 3 of the previous number. They then would have to figure out that the rule was “multiples of 3” and indicate the next multiple of 3. They wrote down what they believed to be the correct number. This test, with 18 items, was timed for 7 min. The test, like Letter Sets, measures the fluid aspect of general intelligence.
“What is a study about human behavior that you might like to design and conduct? What is a question about human behavior that you consider important that you would like to answer? How might you answer it through research?”
- 0 = missing
- 1 = answer unsatisfactory
- 2 = minimally satisfactory; answers question but is weak
- 3 = highly satisfactory; goes a step beyond minimum
- 4 = good; well beyond satisfactory answer
- 5 = outstanding
What are some alternative hypotheses regarding why Laura starts to cry when she is picked up from nursery school by the nanny?”
Please suggest an experimental design to test this hypothesis and describe the experiment in some detail. Assume you have the resources you need to be able to do the experiment (e.g., access to students and their academic records, sufficient funds to pay subjects, etc.).”
Is this conclusion correct? Why or why not?”
3.1.3. Design
3.1.4. Procedure
3.2. Results
3.2.1. Basic Statistics
3.2.2. Reliabilities
3.2.3. Correlations
- (1)
- Our new Scientific-Creativity scores did not correlate significantly with scores on any of the conventional ability tests (SAT, ACT, Number Series) except Letter Sets (r = 0.32, p < 0.05) or with scores on our Scientific Impact measure. However, our Scientific Creativity scores did correlate significantly with our total Scientific Reasoning score (r = 0.49, p < 0.01).
- (2)
- Our new Scientific Impact measure also did not correlate significantly with any of the conventional ability tests but did correlate significantly with our Scientific Reasoning scores (r = 0.27, p < 0.05). As always, one cannot draw any clear conclusions from nonsignificant correlations.
- (3)
- Our Scientific Reasoning measure (total score) further did not correlate significantly with any of the conventional ability tests.
- (4)
- Surprisingly, the SAT and ACT scores did not correlate significantly with the Letter Sets and Number Series scores (see Appendix A), although the samples were reduced because not everyone took either the SAT or ACT, some took one test of the other, and some took both. Letter Sets did correlate significantly with Number Series (r = 0.38, p < 0.01); SAT Reading and SAT Math correlated significantly with each other (r = 0.39, p < 0.05) and SAT Math correlated significantly with Number Series (r = 0.49, p < 0.01).
3.2.4. Factor Analyses
- (1)
- A first factor comprised Letter Sets, Number Series, and Scientific Impact (the last more weakly in the principal-factor analysis).
- (2)
- A second factor comprised Scientific Creativity and Scientific Reasoning.
3.3. Discussion
4. Study 2
4.1. Method
4.1.1. Participants
4.1.2. Materials
4.1.3. Design and Procedure
4.2. Results
4.2.1. Basic Statistics
4.2.2. Reliabilities
4.2.3. Correlations
- (1)
- Scientific Creativity correlated significantly with Scientific Reasoning—Generating Experiments (r = 0.33, p < 0.05), which makes sense because both assessments required participants to generate experimental designs, with the former requiring participants to generate their own scientific problem and the latter providing the problem and hypothesis.
- (2)
- Unlike in Study 1, Scientific Reasoning correlated significantly with SAT Reading (r = 0.33, p < 0.05).
- (3)
- SAT Reading and Math correlated highly with each other (r = 0.77, p < 0.01); ACT Reading and ACT Math also correlated highly with each other (r = 0.53, p < 0.05). (SAT and ACT were generally taken by different participants, so the correlations between them are based on small N’s and are not meaningful.)
- (4)
- Letter Sets and Number Series correlated moderately with each other (r = 0.32, p < 0.05). Letter Sets also correlated moderately with Scientific Reasoning—Conclusions (r = 0.29, p < 0.05) but it did not correlate significantly with Scientific Reasoning overall.
- (5)
- Scientific Impact correlated with ACT-Reading (0.51, p < 0.05) but did not correlate significantly with any of the other psychometric tests.
4.2.4. Factor Analyses
- (1)
- The first factor was for the fluid-intelligence tests.
- (2)
- The second factor was for Scientific Creativity and Scientific Reasoning.
- (3)
- The third factor was a specific factor for Scientific Impact.
4.3. Discussion
5. Study 3
5.1. Method
5.1.1. Participants
5.1.2. Materials
5.1.3. Design
5.1.4. Procedure
5.2. Results
5.2.1. Basic Statistics
5.2.2. Reliabilities
5.2.3. Correlations and Factor Analyses
5.3. Discussion
6. General Discussion
Author Contributions
Funding
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
Appendix A
Assessment | Sci. Creativ. | GPA | SATRea | SATM | ACTRea | ACTMat | Let Sets | Num Ser | Hyp | Exper | Concl | SciRe Tot | Imp Hi | Imp Lo | Tot Imp |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Sci. Creat | 1.00 | −0.16 | 0.23 | 0.00 | 0.30 | 0.36 | 0.32 * | 0.09 | 0.36 ** | 0.46 ** | 0.34 ** | 0.49 ** | −0.01 | 0.11 | 0.07 |
GPA | −0.16 | 1.00 | 0.36 * | 0.39 * | 0.50 ** | 0.20 | −0.01 | 0.27 * | 0.06 | 0.15 | −0.12 | 0.06 | 0.23 | 0.08 | 0.16 |
SATR | 0.23 | 0.36 * | 1.00 | 0.39 * | 0.82 ** | −0.30 | −0.03 | 0.11 | 0.22 | 0.11 | 0.11 | 0.20 | 0.11 | −0.09 | −0.01 |
SATM | 0.00 | 0.39 * | 0.39 * | 1.00 | 0.49 | 0.53 | 0.25 | 0.49 ** | 0.06 | 0.19 | −0.02 | 0.10 | 0.13 | 0.06 | 0.10 |
ACTR | 0.30 | 0.50 ** | 0.82 ** | 0.49 | 1.00 | 0.34 | 0.00 | −0.03 | 0.30 | 0.40 * | −0.15 | 0.29 | 0.16 | −0.02 | 0.07 |
ACTM | 0.36 | 0.20 | −0.30 | 0.53 | 0.34 | 1.00 | 0.17 | 0.35 | 0.18 | 0.35 | 0.27 | 0.32 | 0.07 | 0.09 | 0.10 |
Let Sets | 0.32 * | −0.01 | −0.03 | 0.25 | 0.00 | 0.17 | 1.00 | 0.38 ** | −0.13 | 0.22 | 0.10 | 0.05 | 0.12 | 0.23 | 0.21 |
Numb Ser. | 0.09 | 0.27 * | 0.11 | 0.49 ** | −0.03 | 0.35 | 0.38 ** | 1.00 | 0.08 | 0.40 ** | 0.02 | 0.22 | 0.17 | 0.23 | 0.23 |
Hyp | 0.36 ** | 0.06 | 0.22 | 0.06 | 0.30 | 0.18 | −0.13 | 0.08 | 1.00 | 0.50 ** | 0.42 ** | 0.88 ** | 0.23 | 0.12 | 0.19 |
Exps | 0.46 ** | 0.15 | 0.11 | 0.19 | 0.40 * | 0.35 | 0.22 | 0.40 * | 0.50 ** | 1.00 | 0.30 * | 0.78 ** | 0.18 | 0.15 | 0.18 |
Concs | 0.34 ** | −0.12 | 0.11 | −0.02 | −0.15 | 0.27 | 0.10 | 0.02 | 0.42 ** | 0.30 * | 1.00 | 0.65 ** | 0.21 | 0.32 * | 0.31 * |
Sci Reason Total | 0.49 ** | 0.06 | 0.20 | 0.10 | 0.29 | 0.32 | 0.05 | 0.22 | 0.88 ** | 0.78 ** | 0.65 ** | 1.00 | 0.26 * | 0.22 | 0.27 * |
Impact HI | −0.01 | 0.23 | 0.11 | 0.13 | 0.16 | 0.07 | 0.12 | 0.17 | 0.23 | 0.18 | 0.21 | 0.26 * | 1.00 | 0.53 ** | 0.83 ** |
Impact Lo | 0.11 | 0.08 | −0.09 | 0.06 | −0.02 | 0.09 | 0.23 | 0.23 | 0.12 | 0.15 | 0.32 * | 0.22 | 0.53 ** | 1.00 | 0.92 ** |
Sci Impact Total | 0.07 | 0.16 | −0.01 | 0.10 | 0.07 | 0.10 | 0.21 | 0.23 | 0.19 | 0.18 | 0.31 * | 0.27 * | 0.83 ** | 0.92 ** | 1.00 |
Assessment | Sci. Creat | GPA | SATR | SATM | ACTR | ACTM | LS | NS | Hyp | Exp | Concl | Sci Reas | Imp Hi | Imp Lo | Sci Imp Tot |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Sci. Creat. | 1.00 | 0.07 | 0.19 | 0.05 | −0.20 | −0.11 | 0.22 | 0.03 | 0.02 | 0.33 * | 0.17 | 0.22 | 0.00 | 0.00 | 0.00 |
GPA | 0.07 | 1.00 | 0.32 * | 0.20 | −0.06 | 0.51 * | 0.14 | 0.36 ** | 0.01 | 0.15 | −0.18 | 0.03 | −0.02 | 0.15 | 0.08 |
SATR | 0.19 | 0.32 * | 1.00 | 0.77 ** | 0.75 * | 0.94 ** | 0.22 | 0.37 * | 0.37 * | 0.15 | 0.09 | 0.33 * | 0.08 | 0.15 | 0.14 |
SATM | 0.05 | 0.20 | 0.77 ** | 1.00 | 0.21 | 0.35 | 0.12 | 0.54 ** | 0.23 | −0.01 | 0.10 | 0.17 | −0.05 | −0.06 | −0.06 |
ACTR | −0.20 | −0.06 | 0.75 * | 0.21 | 1.00 | 0.53 * | −0.38 | −0.03 | 0.28 | 0.27 | −0.26 | 0.23 | 0.46 * | 0.53 * | 0.51 * |
ACTM | −0.11 | 0.51 * | 0.94 ** | 0.35 | 0.53 * | 1.00 | −0.01 | 0.64 ** | −0.04 | 0.09 | −0.54 | −0.21 | 0.34 | 0.42 | 0.40 |
Letter Sets | 0.22 | 0.14 | 0.22 | 0.12 | −0.38 | −0.01 | 1.00 | 0.32 * | 0.07 | 0.07 | 0.29 * | 0.16 | −0.04 | −0.13 | −0.10 |
Num. Series | 0.03 | 0.36 ** | 0.37 * | 0.54 ** | −0.03 | 0.64 ** | 0.32 * | 1.00 | 0.02 | 0.05 | 0.01 | 0.04 | 0.14 | 0.06 | 0.11 |
Hyp | 0.02 | 0.01 | 0.37 * | 0.23 | 0.28 | −0.04 | 0.07 | 0.02 | 0.00 | 0.19 | 0.17 | 0.83 ** | 0.22 | 0.06 | 0.15 |
Exp | 0.33 * | 0.15 | 0.15 | −0.01 | 0.27 | 0.09 | 0.07 | 0.05 | 0.19 | 1.00 | 0.13 | 0.64 ** | 0.00 | 0.23 | 0.14 |
Concl | 0.17 | −0.18 | 0.09 | 0.10 | −0.26 | −0.53 * | 0.29 * | 0.01 | 0.17 | 0.13 | 1.00 | 0.46 ** | −0.09 | −0.08 | −0.10 |
Sc. Reas. Tot | 0.22 | 0.03 | 0.33 * | 0.17 | 0.23 | −0.21 | 0.16 | 0.04 | 0.83 ** | 0.64 ** | 0.46 ** | 1.00 | 0.12 | 0.12 | 0.14 |
Impac Hi | 0.00 | −0.02 | 0.08 | −0.05 | 0.46 * | 0.34 | −0.04 | 0.14 | 0.22 | 0.00 | −0.09 | 0.12 | 1.00 | 0.64 ** | 0.90 ** |
ImpactLow | 0.00 | 0.15 | 0.15 | −0.06 | 0.53 * | 0.42 | −0.13 | 0.06 | 0.06 | 0.23 | −0.08 | 0.12 | 0.64 ** | 1.00 | 0.91 ** |
Sci. Imp. Tot | 0.00 | 0.08 | 0.14 | −0.06 | 0.51 * | 0.40 | −0.10 | 0.11 | 0.15 | 0.14 | −0.10 | 0.14 | 0.90 ** | 0.91 ** | 1.00 |
Assessment | LS | NS | Sci Reas | Sci. Creat. | SATReading | SATMath | Impact |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Letter Sets | 1 | 0.26 * | 0.32 * | 0.04 | 0.27 * | 0.02 | 0.21 |
Number Ser. | 0.26 * | 1 | 0.26 * | 0.07 | 0.38 ** | 0.41 ** | 0.10 |
Sci. Reason. | 0.32 * | 0.26 * | 1 | 0.32 * | 0.06 | 0.04 | 0.08 |
Sci. Creativ. | 0.04 | 0.07 | 0.32 * | 1 | 0.06 | 0.04 | 0.06 |
SATReading | 0.27 * | 0.38 ** | 0.06 | 0.06 | 1 | 0.39 ** | 0.15 |
SATMath | 0.02 | 0.41 ** | 0.04 | 0.04 | 0.39 ** | 1 | -0.04 |
Impact | 0.21 | 0.10 | 0.08 | 0.06 | 0.15 | -0.04 | 1 |
References
- Abdulla, Ahmed M., and Bonnie Cramond. 2018. The creative problem finding hierarchy: A suggested model for understanding problem finding. Creativity Theories–Research-Applications 5: 197–229. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Abdulla, Ahmed M., Sue Hyeon Paek, Bonnie Cramond, and Mark A. Runco. 2018. Problem finding and creativity: A meta-analytic review. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts 14: 3–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Amabile, Teresa M. 1996. Creativity in Context: Update to the Social Psychology of Creativity. Boulder: Westview Press. [Google Scholar]
- Arlin, Patricia K. 1975. Cognitive development in adulthood: A fifth stage? Developmental Psychology 11: 602–6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Arlin, Patricia K. 1975–1976. A cognitive process model of problem finding. Educational Horizons 54: 99–106. [Google Scholar]
- Bargh, John A., Katelyn YA McKenna, and Grainne M. Fitzsimons. 2002. Can you see the real me? Activation and expression of the “true self” on the Internet. Journal of Social Issues 58: 33–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Coon, Dennis, and John O. Mitterer. 2013. Introduction to Psychology: Gateways to Mind and Behavior, 14th ed. Boston: Cengage Learning. [Google Scholar]
- Dennis, Wayne. 1958. The age decrement in outstanding scientific contributions: Fact or artifact? American Psychologist 13: 457–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Feist, Gregory J. 1997. Quantity, quality, and depth of research as influences on scientific eminence: Is quantity most important? Creativity Research Journal 10: 325–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ioannidis, John PA, Jeroen Baas, Richard Klavans, and Kevin W. Boyack. 2019. A standardized citation metrics author database annotated for scientific field. PLOS Biology 17: e3000384. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Kaufman, Allison B., and James C. Kaufman, eds. 2019. Psuedoscience: The Conspiracy Against Science. Cambridge: MIT Press. [Google Scholar]
- Kaufman, James C., and Robert J. Sternberg, eds. 2019. Cambridge Handbook of Creativity, 2nd ed. New York: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Kolbert, Elizabeth. 2019. Louisiana’s disappearing cost. The New Yorker, April 1. [Google Scholar]
- Liu, Lu, Yang Wang, Roberta Sinatra, C. Lee Giles, Chaoming Song, and Dashun Wang. 2018. Hot streaks in artistic, cultural, and scientific careers. Nature 559: 396–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Lubart, Todd I. 2001. Models of the creative process: Past, present and future. Creativity Research Journal 13: 295–308. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mumford, Michael D., and Tristan McIntosh. 2017. Creative thinking processes: The past and the future. The Journal of Creative Behavior 51: 317–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mumford, Michael D., Michele I. Mobley, Roni Reiter-Palmon, Charles E. Uhlman, and Lesli M. Doares. 1991. Process analytic models of creative capacities. Creativity Research Journal 4: 91–122. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mumford, Michael D., Kelsey E. Medeiros, and Paul J. Partlow. 2012. Creative thinking: Processes, strategies, and knowledge. The Journal of Creative Behavior 46: 30–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Myers, David G. 2011. Myers’ Psychology, 2nd ed. New York: Worth. [Google Scholar]
- Posselt, Julie R. 2018. Inside Graduate Admissions: Merit, Diversity, and Graduate Gate-Keeping. Cambridge: Harvard University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Rettner, R. 2019. More than 250,000 people may die each year due to climate change. Live Science, January 17. [Google Scholar]
- Sackett, Paul R., Oren R. Shewach, and Jeffrey A. Dahlke. 2020. The predictive value of general intelligence. In Human Intelligence: An Introduction. Edited by Robert J. Sternberg. New York: Cambridge University Press, pp. 381–414. [Google Scholar]
- Shermer, M. 2002. Why People Believe Weird Things. New York: Holt. [Google Scholar]
- Simonton, Dean Keith. 2003. Scientific creativity as constrained stochastic behavior: The integration of product, process, and person perspectives. Psychological Bulletin 129: 475–94. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Simonton, Dean Keith. 2004. Creativity in Science: Chance, Logic, Genius, and Zeitgeist. New York: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Simonton, Dean Keith. 2010. Creative thought as blind-variation and selective-retention: Combinatorial models of exceptional creativity. Physics of Life Reviews 7: 156–79. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Sternberg, Robert J. 1997. Successful Intelligence. New York: Plume. [Google Scholar]
- Sternberg, Robert J. 2003a. Afterword: How much impact should impact have? In Anatomy of Impact: What has Made the Great Works of Psychology Great? Edited by R. J. Sternberg. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association, pp. 223–28. [Google Scholar]
- Sternberg, Robert J., ed. 2003b. Anatomy of Impact: What has Made the Great Works of Psychology Great? Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. [Google Scholar]
- Sternberg, Robert J. 2016. “Am I famous yet?” Judging scholarly merit in psychological science: An introduction. Perspectives on Psychological Science 11: 877–81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sternberg, Robert J. 2017. Measuring creativity: A 40+ year retrospective. Journal of Creative Behavior. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sternberg, Robert J. 2018a. Creative giftedness is not just what creativity tests test: Implications of a triangular theory of creativity for understanding creative giftedness. Roeper Review 40: 158–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sternberg, Robert J. 2018b. Evaluating merit among scientists. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition 7: 209–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sternberg, Robert J. 2018c. Teaching and assessing gifted students in STEM disciplines through the augmented theory of successful intelligence. High Ability Studies 30: 103–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sternberg, Robert J. 2018d. The scientific work we love: A duplex theory of scientific impact and its application to the top-cited articles in the first 30 years of APS journals. Perspectives on Psychological Science 30: 103–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sternberg, Robert J. 2018e. What’s wrong with creativity testing? Journal of Creative Behavior. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sternberg, Robert J. 2019. The psychology of creativity. In Secrets of Creativity: What Neuroscience, the Arts, and Our Minds Reveal. Edited by S. Nalbantian and P. M. Matthews. New York: Oxford University Press, pp. 64–85. [Google Scholar]
- Sternberg, Robert J. 2020. The augmented theory of successful intelligence. In Cambridge Handbook of Intelligence, 2nd ed. Edited by Robert J. Sternberg. New York: Cambridge University Press, vol. 2, pp. 679–708. [Google Scholar]
- Sternberg, Robert J., and Tamara Gordeeva. 1996. The anatomy of impact: What makes an article influential? Psychological Science 7: 69–75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sternberg, Robert J., and Nicky Hayes. 2018. The road to writing a textbook. Teaching of Psychology 45: 278–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sternberg, Robert J., and James C. Kaufman, eds. 2018. The Nature of Human Creativity. New York: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Sternberg, Robert J., and Karin Sternberg. 2017. Measuring scientific reasoning for graduate admissions in psychology and related disciplines. Journal of Intelligence 5: 29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Sternberg, Robert J., and The Rainbow Project Collaborators. 2006. The Rainbow Project: Enhancing the SAT through assessments of analytical, practical and creative skills. Intelligence 34: 321–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sternberg, Robert J., S. T. Fiske, and D. J. Foss, eds. 2016. Scientists Making a Difference: One Hundred Eminent Behavioral and Brain Scientists Talk about Their Most Important Contributions. New York: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Sternberg, Robert J., Karin Sternberg, and Rebel J.E. Todhunter. 2017. Measuring reasoning about teaching for graduate admissions in psychology and related disciplines. Journal of Intelligence 5: 34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Sternberg, Robert J., Chak Haang Wong, and Karin Sternberg. 2019. The relation of tests of scientific reasoning to each other and to tests of fluid intelligence. Journal of Intelligence 7: 20. [Google Scholar]
- The Guardian. 2018. Rising Seas: “Florida is about to be Wiped off the Map”. The Guardian. June 26. Available online: https://www.theguardian.com/environment/2018/jun/26/rising-seas-florida-climate-change-elizabeth-rush (accessed on 13 April 2020).
- Tulving, Endel, and Stephen A. Madigan. 1970. Memory and verbal learning. Annual Review of Psychology 21: 437–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ward, T. B., and Y. Kolomyts. 2019. Creative cognition. In Cambridge Handbook of Creativity, 2nd ed. Edited by James C. Kaufman and Robert J. Sternberg. New York: Cambridge University Press, pp. 175–99. [Google Scholar]
- Weiten, W. 2011. Psychology: Themes and Variations, 9th ed. Belmont: Wadsworth Cengage Learning. [Google Scholar]
- Worland, J. 2019. Donald Trump called climate change a hoax. Now he’s awkwardly boasting about fighting it. Time, July 9. [Google Scholar]
- Xia, R. 2019. The California coast is disappearing under the rising sea. Our choices are grim. Los Angeles Times. July 7. Available online: https://www.latimes.com/projects/la-me-sea-level-rise-california-coast/ (accessed on 13 April 2020).
- Zuckerman, H. 1977. Scientific Elite. New York: Free Press. [Google Scholar]
Assessment | N | Mean | SD |
---|---|---|---|
Scientific Creativity | 58 | 2.60 | 0.79 |
Letter Sets | 59 | 10.19 | 2.51 |
Number Series | 59 | 10.49 | 3.01 |
Scientific Reasoning--Hypotheses | 59 | 6.59 | 2.91 |
Scientific Reasoning--Experiments | 59 | 6.34 | 2.19 |
Scientific Reasoning--Conclusions | 59 | 6.97 | 1.60 |
Scientific Reasoning (Total) | 59 | 19.90 | 5.32 |
Impact Ratings High | 58 | 7.34 | 1.38 |
Impact Ratings Low | 58 | 7.17 | 1.92 |
Average of confidence ratings for high impact items | 59 | 2.21 | 0.32 |
Average of confidence ratings for low impact items | 57 | 2.16 | 0.35 |
Average of creativity ratings for high impact items | 59 | 1.94 | 0.29 |
Average of creativity ratings for low impact items | 59 | 2.24 | 0.35 |
Average of rigor ratings for high impact items | 59 | 2.09 | 0.33 |
Average of rigor ratings for low impact items | 59 | 1.74 | 0.25 |
Average of usefulness ratings for high impact items | 59 | 2.36 | 0.29 |
Average of usefulness ratings for low impact items | 58 | 1.70 | 0.27 |
Age | 59 | 20.24 | 1.78 |
GPA | 59 | 3.39 | 0.47 |
SAT Reading | 40 | 702.25 | 70.51 |
SAT Math | 40 | 734.75 | 49.30 |
ACT Reading | 28 | 32.07 | 4.15 |
ACT Math | 28 | 33.25 | 3.09 |
What is the number of lab courses you have taken? | 55 | 2.18 | 2.39 |
How many scientific articles do you read per month? | 58 | 5.55 | 8.64 |
Assessment | Sci. Creat. | LS | NS | Sci Reas. | Sci Imp. |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Sci. Creat. | 1.00 | 0.32 * | 0.09 | 0.49 ** | 0.07 |
Letter Sets | 0.32 * | 1.00 | 0.38 ** | 0.05 | 0.21 |
Num. Ser. | 0.09 | 0.38 * | 1.00 | 0.22 | 0.23 |
Sci. Reas. | 0.49 ** | 0.05 | 0.22 | 1.00 | 0.27 * |
Sci. Imp. | 0.07 | 0.21 | 0.23 | 0.27 * | 1.00 |
Assessment | Component | |
---|---|---|
I | II | |
Letter Sets | 0.77 | 0.16 |
Number Series | 0.77 | 0.04 |
Scientific Reasoning | 0.15 | 0.84 |
Scientific Creativity | 0.09 | 0.88 |
Scientific Impact | 0.64 | 0.10 |
Extraction Method: Principal-component analysis | ||
2 Components Extracted Rotation Method: Varimax with Kaiser Normalization | ||
Rotation converged in 3 iterations Percentage of Variance Account for: 62%. |
Assessment | N | Mean | Std. Deviation |
---|---|---|---|
Scientific Creativity | 54 | 2.46 | 0.77 |
Letter Sets | 54 | 9.72 | 2.29 |
Number Series | 54 | 10.63 | 3.13 |
Hypotheses | 54 | 6.85 | 2.80 |
Experiments | 54 | 6.39 | 1.90 |
Conclusions | 54 | 6.24 | 1.16 |
Scientific Reasoning | 54 | 19.48 | 4.06 |
Impact_high | 53 | 7.72 | 1.47 |
Impact_low | 53 | 7.55 | 1.60 |
Average of confidence ratings for high impact items | 54 | 2.18 | 0.37 |
Average of confidence ratings for low impact items | 54 | 2.19 | 0.39 |
Average of creativity ratings for high impact items | 54 | 1.88 | 0.37 |
Average of creativity ratings for low impact items | 54 | 2.34 | 0.33 |
Average of rigor ratings for high impact items | 54 | 2.20 | 0.29 |
Average of rigor ratings for low impact items | 54 | 1.74 | 0.29 |
Average of usefulness ratings for high impact items | 54 | 2.47 | 0.29 |
Average of usefulness ratings for low impact items | 54 | 1.74 | 0.31 |
Age | 54 | 20.59 | 2.00 |
GPA | 54 | 3.39 | 0.50 |
SATReading | 38 | 702.89 | 67.50 |
SATMath | 40 | 718.50 | 84.02 |
ACTMath | 20 | 32.60 | 5.01 |
ACTReading | 20 | 32.50 | 3.89 |
GRE | 4 | 300.50 | 50.72 |
What is the number of lab courses you have taken? | 52 | 1.90 | 1.76 |
How many scientific articles do you read per month? | 50 | 6.06 | 11.40 |
Assessement | Sci. Creat. | LS | NS | Sci Reas. | Sci Imp. |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Sci. Creat. | 1.00 | 0.22 | 0.03 | 0.22 | 0.00 |
Letter Sets | 0.22 | 1.00 | 0.32 * | 0.16 | −0.10 |
Num. Ser. | 0.03 | 0.32 * | 1.00 | 0.04 | 0.11 |
Sci. Reas. | 0.22 | 0.16 | 0.04 | 1.00 | 0.14 |
Sci. Imp. | 0.00 | −0.10 | 0.11 | 0.14 | 1.00 |
Assessment | I | II | III |
---|---|---|---|
Letter Sets | 0.75 | 0.31 | −0.26 |
Number Series | 0.86 | −0.13 | 0.22 |
Scientific Reasoning | 0.03 | 0.72 | 0.33 |
Scientific Creativity | 0.05 | 0.78 | −0.16 |
Scientific Impact | 0.02 | 0.05 | 0.92 |
Extraction Method: Principal-component analysis. | |||
3 components extracted. Rotation Method: Varimax with Kaiser normalization. 78Rotation converged at 5 iterations. Components accounted for 73% of variance in data. Factors included for Eigenvalues >1. These were also the most interpretable factors. |
Assessment | N | Mean | Standard Deviation |
---|---|---|---|
s | 106 | 10.12 | 2.51 |
Number Series | 106 | 10.58 | 2.92 |
LS+NS | 106 | 20.70 | 4.45 |
Hypotheses | 105 | 7.40 | 3.35 |
Experiments | 106 | 6.80 | 1.38 |
Conclusions | 105 | 6.91 | 1.47 |
Sci. Reas. Tot. | 104 | 21.10 | 4.80 |
Impact_Low | 106 | 7.09 | 1.61 |
Impact_High | 105 | 7.51 | 1.61 |
Impact Total | 105 | 14.62 | 2.75 |
Confidence_High | 106 | 23.16 | 3.12 |
Confidence_Low | 106 | 22.70 | 3.44 |
Creativity_High | 106 | 20.28 | 3.22 |
Creativity_Low | 105 | 20.81 | 3.81 |
Rigor_High | 103 | 21.86 | 3.32 |
Rigor_Low | 106 | 19.57 | 3.13 |
Practicality_High | 103 | 24.68 | 2.96 |
Practicality_Low | 106 | 18.58 | 3.41 |
Age | 106 | 19.41 | 1.128 |
GPA | 77 | 3.56 | 0.36 |
SATReading | 60 | 712.67 | 55.54 |
SATMath | 63 | 745.87 | 63.90 |
SAT/ACTCombined | 96 | 64.63 | 5.39 |
ACTReading | 48 | 32.79 | 2.91 |
ACTMath | 49 | 33.00 | 2.82 |
LabCourses | 105 | 1.76 | 1.75 |
Articles | 101 | 3.81 | 5.55 |
Assessment | LS | NS | Sci. Rea. | Sci. Creat. | Impact |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Letter Sets | 1 | 0.26 * | 0.32 * | 0.04 | 0.21 |
Number Ser. | 0.26 * | 1 | 0.26 * | 0.07 | 0.10 |
Sci. Reason. | 0.32 * | 0.26 * | 1 | 0.32 * | 0.08 |
Sci. Creativ. | 0.04 | 0.07 | 0.32 * | 1 | 0.06 |
Impact | 0.21 | 0.10 | 0.08 | 0.06 | 1 |
Component | |||
---|---|---|---|
I | II | III | |
Letter Sets | 0.20 | 0.23 | 0.71 |
Number Series | 0.73 | 0.20 | 0.19 |
Sci. Reason. | 0.08 | 0.79 | 0.26 |
Sci. Creativity | 0.02 | 0.80 | −0.11 |
SATReading | 0.73 | −0.05 | 0.27 |
SATMath | 0.83 | −0.00 | −0.22 |
Impact | −0.02 | −0.07 | 0.76 |
© 2020 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Sternberg, R.J.; Todhunter, R.J.E.; Litvak, A.; Sternberg, K. The Relation of Scientific Creativity and Evaluation of Scientific Impact to Scientific Reasoning and General Intelligence. J. Intell. 2020, 8, 17. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence8020017
Sternberg RJ, Todhunter RJE, Litvak A, Sternberg K. The Relation of Scientific Creativity and Evaluation of Scientific Impact to Scientific Reasoning and General Intelligence. Journal of Intelligence. 2020; 8(2):17. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence8020017
Chicago/Turabian StyleSternberg, Robert J., Rebel J. E. Todhunter, Aaron Litvak, and Karin Sternberg. 2020. "The Relation of Scientific Creativity and Evaluation of Scientific Impact to Scientific Reasoning and General Intelligence" Journal of Intelligence 8, no. 2: 17. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence8020017
APA StyleSternberg, R. J., Todhunter, R. J. E., Litvak, A., & Sternberg, K. (2020). The Relation of Scientific Creativity and Evaluation of Scientific Impact to Scientific Reasoning and General Intelligence. Journal of Intelligence, 8(2), 17. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence8020017