2. Why Might the Notion of Intelligence Be Useful in the Field of Education?
Integrating established information from one field into another. The notion of
g is among the most established, replicated, and influential predictors of numerous life outcomes, including educational performance and attainment (
Gottfredson 2004;
Jensen 1998). Therefore, acknowledging and accepting individual differences in intellectual capacities and learning rates among students can both lead to a more robust and coherent research literature in the broad field of education research (for some concrete examples of how intelligence can help education and its research, see
Wai et al. 2018) as well as finding solutions that will work, and ruling out those unlikely to help children. For example, understanding that intelligence or g is measured in pretty much any standardized ability or achievement test (by proxy; e.g.,
Carroll 1997;
Frey and Detterman 2004;
Koenig et al. 2008) suggests that literatures could be productively informed by one another just by this link alone. How would this reorient our thinking about the literature in education broadly? As
Gottfredson (
2004, p. 45) correctly notes: “By embracing rather than rejecting the scientific knowledge about
g, educators can develop curricula and classroom techniques that well serve the nation’s cognitively diverse students.” The investment of
g, after all, is key to the acquisition of content knowledge, and unfortunately, as we document in the next section, the field of education has little interest in intelligence research and considerable skepticism regarding content knowledge. Academic fields are often siloed, communicating mainly to one another. The body of research from the field of intelligence would greatly improve the research literature and rate of solution finding to improve the education and life outcomes of students. It could also improve education policy by helping suggest which areas of policy are likely to productively help students, and which areas probably will not (e.g.,
Asbury and Wai 2019). Accounting for the intelligence of students could positively affect the educational practices that impact students, teachers, principals, and school systems. Of course, intelligence researchers can also productively learn from the field of education, and we stress that interdisciplinary integration requires information crossing boundaries in all directions.
Accounting for intelligence among all groups of people in schools. Beyond integrating the information about the intelligence of students into education research and education policy, accounting for the intelligence of teachers, administrators, or other people who interact with students in schools may also be useful. For example, a large body of research literature from the field of industrial/organizational (I/O) psychology illustrates that
g is enormously important for job performance (e.g.,
Schmidt and Hunter 1998,
2004). Research has indicated that individuals who choose to become teachers in the U.S. also tend to be drawn from the lower end of the ability distribution, at least among college graduates (e.g.,
College Board 2014;
Wai et al. 2009;
Wolfle and Oxtoby 1952). This trend has remained consistent across the last several decades as demonstrated using different ability and achievement measures and different samples over time (
Wai 2015). Drawing from the large I/O psychology literature suggesting that
g matters for job performance, perhaps a better understanding of how
g matters for the performance of teachers and teaching effectiveness could be a worthwhile avenue of pursuit for education research and policy. We might hypothesize that all else equal, more highly able teachers, for example, might more effectively challenge students, particularly more intellectually advanced or gifted students. Such teachers might also innovate in ways better serving less intellectually advanced students, and those disadvantaged by class or race; indeed, the literature on so called “no-excuses” schools shows that high ability teachers developed and largely staff these schooling models (
Thernstrom and Thernstrom 2003).
3. How Education’s History Led to the Neglect of Intelligence
In American public schooling, the profession of Educational Leadership dominates both the daily routines of schooling, and educational policy-making, and in these respects the field has far more influence over education than do more systematic education subfields like Educational Psychology, which takes the study of intelligence more seriously. Principals and school superintendents, typically with graduate degrees in Educational Leadership, have considerable influence in state legislatures and in state departments of education, and thus over the certification of public school teachers and leaders. Likewise, former superintendents influence and not infrequently lead schools of education within the mainly second tier universities which train the vast majority of American teachers (
Lucas 1999). On a day to day basis, principals and superintendents select and retain (or occasionally, dismiss) teachers, and select which teachers gain promotion to administration. Accordingly, to understand the truncated role of intelligence in American schooling, one must understand Educational Leadership as a profession. Contrasting higher education (until recently) and private schools, American traditional public schools have very hierarchical structures, with slow promotion. As
Chubb and Moe (
1990) find, typical public high school principals have not taught a course in over a decade, while private school principals (like college presidents) sometimes still teach, and often return full time to the classroom. Through both their training in Educational Leadership programs and via selection and self-selection, as permanent administrators, public school leaders value compliance, teamwork and order (
Ingersoll 2003), as indeed have education policymakers since the early 20th Century (
Mehta 2013).
Professions are most malleable in their beginnings, when founders and founding institutions including the educational institutions which train and vet new professionals develop ideologies regarding the profession’s role in society, and its means for realizing those roles. Path development approaches suggest that once a profession institutionalizes, further changes tend to be incremental, with new entrants selected in part for their support of existing institutional norms, cultures, and practices (
Klyza 1996;
Mehta and Teles 2014). The field of Educational Leadership arose in the early 20th century, and it remains influenced by the doctrines of that time. As classic treatments of the development of Educational Leadership by
Labaree (
2003,
2004),
Callahan (
1962), and
Rousmaniere (
2013) show, the founders of the field lacked optimism regarding the importance of intelligence and its malleability. They conceptualized intelligence as largely predetermined; thus, efforts to improve intelligence through schooling were seen as both quixotic and elitist. While schools could sort students based on their intelligence, schools could not develop intelligence. This influences how modern schools operate on a daily basis, even today (e.g.,
Burkett 2001;
Powell et al. 1985).
Further, in part to distinguish themselves from other university based disciplines and to appeal to pragmatic school boards largely made up of businessmen, the founders of Educational Leadership adopted scientific management models imported from business. Scientific Management increased manufacturing efficiency and compared to Social Darwinism, which influenced prior management philosophies, it marked a significant evolution in a humanitarian direction. Yet it had muted regard for intelligence. As devised by Frederick Taylor and others, scientific management dictated that good managers observe the workplace, break down each step in a production process to find the most efficient techniques, then use bonuses (or threats) to persuade workers to learn and use that “one best way.” Scientific management assumes that all reasonable people agree on goals (more efficient production); thus, it demands managers (in the case of schools, principals and superintendents) with certain technical skills, but no need for the intellectual depth to understand that in education, not all capable people of good will agree on either the goals of schooling, nor the best means to achieve those goals. This management philosophy does not demand intelligence in managers, and positively scorns intelligence in employees. Ideal teachers should be trainable towards compliance rather than out of the box thinkers (
Perrow 1979; as regards schooling see
Ingersoll 2003;
Maranto 2001).
As
Callahan (
1962) and
Rousmaniere (
2013), in particular, show, early 20th century Progressive reforms in line with the scientific management administrative doctrines of the day developed large, differentiated public schools intended to function as factories in which male superiors (educational leaders) with the proper graduate Educational Leadership credentials managed largely female subordinates, a matter treated further below.
Maranto et al. (
2018) found that such cultural norms and personnel practices still have influence in public schooling. Further, as
Callahan (
1962) documents regarding the early 20th century and
Levine (
2006) demonstrates in the 21st century, doctoral dissertations in the field of Educational Leadership lack intellectual rigor relative to other areas. Many focus on such matters as the most efficacious bell schedules and the most efficient ratio of custodians to floor space. Indeed, as Ellwood Cubberley, dean of the school of education at Stanford University and one of the founders of Educational Leadership as a field, explained, before 1900 schools had been “a manufacturing establishment running at a low grade of efficiency.” The field of Educational Leadership aimed to change that (see also
Labaree 2003,
2004;
Mehta 2013;
Tyack 1974;
Rousmaniere 2013). As
Mehta (
2013) and
Labaree (
2004) show, in the early 20th century, many decades before
No Child Left Behind forced public schools to report test scores to the public, large school systems run by administrative progressives used widespread quantification in efforts to centralize control at the principal and even superintendent level, minimizing teacher autonomy and initiative. Yet, in practice, given the hundreds of decisions teachers must make daily, centralized efforts to manage their complex work are counterproductive. Moreover, scientific management-like efforts to control teachers led their unions through the 20th century to behave more like traditional industrial unions than professional organizations (
Ingersoll 2003;
Mehta 2013). This is precisely the opposite of how the most successful schools and school systems operate (
Ouchi 2009).
As
Labaree (
2004) illustrates,
pedagogical progressives like Dewey influenced schools of education and thus the training and selection of teachers; over time they also watered down course content. Yet the
administrative progressives who dominated Educational Leadership had far more real world influence. Neither group, as it happens, had enormous regard for intelligence, nor for academic content unless narrowly tailored to utilitarian and typically vocational ends. Further, while in the late 19th and early 20th centuries most principals and many superintendents were women, a key aspect of professionalizing education at a time when professional meant male was putting men in charge. In part to attract men to the field, public education developed athletics and the new field of athletic coaching; thus men could coach for a few years before becoming secondary principals and eventually superintendents. Even today, while most teachers are women, most educational administrators are men. Nationally, 53% of male principals and a higher percentage of superintendents are former coaches. Interestingly, those who select into the field tend to be relatively conservative, comfortable with predictable employment, and even traditional gender roles (
Maranto et al. 2018; see also
Rousmaniere 2013). Generally speaking, for both educational administrators and school board members, athletic competitions are centers of school life while academic competitions and day to day teaching are largely invisible (
Maeroff 2010, p. 18).
One could imagine an alternative past in which Educational Leadership as a field of study embraced relatively rigorous subfields of Psychology such as Cognitive or I/O Psychology, or perhaps even History and Government. These are relatively higher intelligence academic professions, on average, than Education, though all remain lower than the hard STEM disciplines and have so for decades (
Wai 2015;
Wai et al. 2009). Indeed
Maranto et al. (
2010) call for this remaking of the schools of education which train public school teachers and leaders, copying the successful reforms of business schools in the 1960s, which reorganized their requirements around three rigorous academic disciplines: I/O psychology, statistics, and economics. Such reforms did not, and likely could not occur in Educational Leadership, nor in education generally. The academic field of business within higher education cannot force private businesses to hire only managers with MBAs. In contrast, state departments of education, influenced by schools of education, have required that the vast majority of traditional public schools hire the teachers and administrators schools of education have trained and certified; thus, limiting the impetus for reform. As
Hirsch (
1996) and
Stotsky (
2015) detail, in this way K-12 public education has, like most professions, developed distinct selection and training pathways, organizations, certification systems (enforced by law), and specialized terminology. Stotsky notes that at times in the past, in some states, teacher certification requirements had significant rigor via teacher licensure tests. Unfortunately, since at least the 1950s, states have lowered standards to make sure public schools can recruit sufficient numbers of teachers to fill classrooms.
As
Koedel (
2011) and others have demonstrated, as a college major Education on both undergraduate and graduate levels lacks rigor; thus
Moe (
2011) portrays the field as having “phony professionalism” with positions based on non-challenging undergraduate, masters, and doctoral degrees rather than actual knowledge or achievement. Such certifications (and confusing terminology) lend a certain legitimacy, drawing support from teachers unions, school boards, and of course the schools of education that provide them. This role lacks credibility among academic elites, however. As
Labaree (
2004, p. 91) tartly observed:
“Those teaching in the university think of those in ed schools as being academically weak and narrowly vocational. They see ed school teachers not as peers in the world of higher education but as an embarrassment, who should not be part of the university at all. To them the ed school looks less like a school of medicine than a school of cosmetology.”
Elites increasingly attend and typically send their own children to elite private schools that do not use teacher or leader certification as criteria for employment. As
Maranto and McShane (
2012, p. 61) document, most recent presidents received their education in and sent their own children to such private schools, indicating their distrust of existing systems of education licensure and professionalism. Further, very few doctorates of education become college or university presidents: Among the eight Ivy League universities and eight “Public Ivies”, none is currently led by an Ed.D.
Generally, with the rise of mass schooling, educational administrators and school boards fashioned schools to focus on non-academic foci, since they assumed few students could handle serious academic work, which itself had little or no value unless it could be connected directly to vocation. Accordingly, schools would operate to provide custodial care for students to keep them off the streets and the job market, and to socialize them into eventual factory work. Historically,
Labaree (
2003,
2004) and
Maranto and McShane (
2012) consider the
Cardinal Principals of Secondary Education issued in 1918 by the U.S. Government and the National Education Association (then a school administrator’s organization) to be an important foundational document of American secondary education, enumerating and justifying the key objectives of schooling at a time when secondary schooling expanded rapidly. As noted, rapid growth meant that new organizational ideologies and operational practices could be imposed and institutionalized among new personnel and in new schools. Importantly, and in sharp contrast to prior justifications for secondary schooling, only one of the seven
Cardinal Principles concerned academic achievement. This single goal,
command of fundamental processes, was to cover nearly all scholarly disciplines. (The other goals are health, worthy use of leisure, citizenship, worthy home membership, vocation, and ethical character.) While few living teachers and educational leaders have ever heard of the
Cardinal Principles, the document still defines the field. Indeed, nearly 60 years after the
Principles’ publication, a superintendent told one of us (Maranto) “you do not understand the purpose of the public education system. The purpose of public education is
not to educate students. The purpose of public education is to provide an education for those
few who want it.” Other students would have to be kept in schools since otherwise “crime would go up, unemployment would go up” (quoted in
Maranto and McShane 2012, p. 11).
If the key goals of schools are sports and other extracurricular activities to keep students in custodial settings rather than traditional intellectual goals, then there is no particular need for schools to stress intelligence, or even recognize it as a concept; indeed this may well be seen as elitist, particularly at a time when schooling expanded rapidly. As
Labaree (
2004, p. 109) observes:
“If school subjects have to be adjusted to the capacities of the students and to the requirements of the job market, and if most students have modest capacities and most jobs have modest skill requirements, then only a few classes need provide a rigorous academic content for the college bound elite, while most students need classes that are less academic, less demanding, and better suited to their modest future roles in society. This is a straightforward prescription for diluting academic content.”
Though this area is under-studied, evidence suggests that in hiring teachers, typical public school principals do not take cognitive ability nor subject matter knowledge into account (
Shuls and Trivitt 2015;
Kramer 1991). In short, the ideology of education as a field largely explains its failure to embrace intelligence as a goal, or even as a concept. Further, until other professional occupations were open to women and minorities, high levels of female and minority teacher (though not white male administrator) human capital were assured. Only since 1970 has teacher quality (as measured by cognitive ability measures like the SAT) fallen. Estimates indicate that among female high school graduates in the top tenth of cognitive ability, the proportion entering teaching declined from roughly a quarter in the early 1970s to roughly a tenth in the early 2000s (
Gastic 2014;
Maranto 2015). This happened at the very time when elites began to stress the importance of students making gains in cognitive ability for national economic success. Yet the relatively low level of teacher cognitive ability that has stretched across many decades back to the 1940s (see
Wai 2015 for testing data across 5 independent sources) has itself played into the approaches of traditional education leaders, teachers unions, and many educational reformers. To varying degrees, all are stuck in the early 20th century factory models of schooling, and low cognitive ability teachers may be more compliant with regular and largely symbolic reforms developed by policy-makers and implemented by educational leaders; they will also offer more solidarity to unions (
Mehta 2013;
Moe 2011). Once a field gains a reputation for low cognitive ability, it attracts new entrants who do not value intelligence, starting with who chooses education as a college major (
Armstrong and Hamilton 2013;
Stotsky 2015;
Wai 2015). Further, school leaders rarely terminate teachers for poor teaching (
Moe 2011) and likely as a result, the field attracts the risk averse (
Bowen et al. 2015).
Stotsky (
2015) argues that, in part, reducing the rigor of teacher certification tests like the Praxis reflects the desire for a racially diverse teaching core. We argue that this was not really a factor until the 1970’s, long after the institutionalization of state level certification practices. Diversity goals did not lower barriers to entry into the teaching profession but were later used by education professors to justify the reduced academic rigor. Ironically, the existing teacher workforce is overwhelmingly non-diverse, so if low barriers to entry are meant to facilitate a diverse workforce, they are failing to achieve this goal (
Maranto et al. 2018).
Defining itself as distinct from and more pragmatic than the more scholarly segments of higher education, Education as a field values the trappings of education, non-rigorous degrees and certifications for employment—but not actual reflections of intelligence or content knowledge.
Hirsch (
1996) recounts how conventional educators often expressed astonishment that he actually enjoyed learning the facts and stories that went into his engaging Core Knowledge curricula. One contemptuous educator asked if Hirsch actually thought that learning more content made him a better person (
Hirsch 1996, p. 55). Decades before “fake news,” the field of Education had espoused limited appreciation for facts, instead embracing the approach that “we teach children, not subjects.” To be clear, academic skill development and subject matter knowledge should not be the sole goals of schooling. Personal relationships including school level affinities (“school spirit”) should also matter. Yet as
Maranto and McShane (
2012) document, over the past three decades of U.S. education reform, elites have consistently sought to make U.S. schooling more academic and less social and thus more oriented toward academic skill development, to resemble schooling in most European and Asian systems. The field of Educational Leadership, charged with implementing elite driven reforms, by and large does not support those reforms.
This does not mean that intelligence is entirely absent from schooling. If schools could not celebrate and develop intelligence, which was thought to be largely inherent and not terribly useful to vocation, schools could sort out the smarter students from the rest. In fact, according to
Tyack and Cuban (
1995, p. 58), in the 1920s “hype about the virtues of intelligence tests and tracking of students into differentiated courses of study led to rapid adoption of these practices.” Today this has largely fallen out of favor in educational theory, yet in actual schools academic tracking is alive and well, and arguably serves legitimate educational purposes despite its ostensible unpopularity (
Loveless 1999). Thus, education reformers 100 years ago took intelligence tests heavily into account for social efficiency reasons. These policies and practices have fallen out of favor today in the academy and among education policymakers but retain a quiet but widespread presence in actual schools in matters like tracking, entry into gifted and talented and advanced placement programs, entry in magnet schools, and college advisement. Moreover, it is true that within public schools there is considerable competition over academic awards such as National Merit Scholarships, though in practice educational administrators tend to see these as a function of student privilege (
Demerath 2009).
5. What Can Be Done about This?
What can intelligence researchers do on their end? In reference to scholars in the field of intelligence,
Hunt (
2009, p. 324) noted “We have a communication problem.” Given that the body of evidence supporting intelligence and its importance in life is hard to dispute, Hunt is correct that the field most likely has a communication issue. Intelligence researchers must more effectively communicate their research to subfields beyond their own and to the general public, and ideally, the general public would be more receptive to traditionally “controversial” areas of research such as intelligence, especially in the U.S., and particularly within U.S. education (e.g.,
Gottfredson 2004;
Wai et al. 2018).
Given the sensitive nature of the role of intelligence in education and life contexts, one important step that intelligence researchers could take is to communicate research evidence with the full awareness of the political and ideological forces that are at play within the U.S. education community at both the research and practice levels. For example, in the real world, intelligence is widely employed as an important screening tool for a wide range of jobs (
Gottfredson 2004) including in higher education (
Posselt 2016), though this is not openly acknowledged as such. Intelligence researchers could potentially be more effective in communicating the importance of intelligence research for education research and policy by working to continually teach others about intelligence research with a focus on how taking evidence into account, much like in health, can more rapidly lead to effective solutions to help children. As
Detterman (
2016) has pointed out, we may first need to understand intelligence to understand its impacts on education and possibilities in helping kids.
What can be done in regards to the education community and those who work in schools? One leverage point that may be useful in getting the concept of intelligence more integrated into the education community and schools is to leverage “school choice,” something many U.S. educators and parents care deeply about (e.g.,
Ladner 2018). For example, gifted education is connected to high intelligence and people who have kids who are bored or unchallenged (or often bullied) in K-12 traditional public schools would be motivated to find schooling options which would best challenge their kids to develop their talents to the fullest (e.g.,
Assouline et al. 2015). Interestingly, as
Ladner (
2018) and
Kronholz (
2014) point out, in relatively free education markets disproportionate numbers of American parents choose classical academies and similar schools with considerable emphasis on academic content, and where, as Kronholz puts it “teachers are scholars.” Accordingly, over the long term, more school choice might bring more consideration of intelligence into schooling.
Second, pipelines into school leadership prioritize athletic coaching, an area often antithetical to advancement placement and gifted education (
Maranto et al. 2018). Acknowledging the concept of intelligence as important to education could over the long-term lead to serious discussions regarding the cognitive capacities of school teachers and leaders. Indeed, one of us (
Maranto 2018) has argued for substantially higher teacher pay to attract more cognitively able individuals to the field, coupled with serious accountability systems. A related possibility, hinted at by
Stotsky (
2015), would be anything bringing additional transparency to the relatively low academic achievement of public educators as measured by tests such as the Praxis, which in part likely measure intelligence. Given the aforementioned public and elite demands for academically talented teachers and school leaders, as expressed in school choice, such transparency could lead to political pressures to reform pipelines into teaching and school leadership. For example, were the U.S. Department of Education to rank states by the mean ACTs and SATs of new entrants to teaching and school leadership, state level officials might feel incentives to reform the human capital pipeline into education. One could also imagine national level teacher and leader certification systems, which would be more transparent than state systems. This transparency could encourage reforms bringing higher levels of human capital in public education.
A third leverage point that may be useful is anything showing solid replicable evidence that intelligence is malleable. To date, such evidence remains unclear (e.g., see
Haier 2016), but both the Flynn effect (e.g.,
Flynn 1984,
1987) and how education may potentially improve intelligence (e.g.,
Ceci 1991;
Ceci and Williams 1997;
Ritchie and Tucker-Drob 2018) may have potential. Beyond
g, some of the literature on specific abilities like spatial ability, has shown some support for the idea of malleability (e.g.,
Uttal et al. 2013;
Stieff and Uttal 2015), though again the evidence remains unclear whether such changes in spatial reasoning will necessarily lead to lasting changes in the ability constructs and long-term predictive validity or if these changes might “fade out” over time (e.g., as they do for interventions targeting cognitive skills:
Bailey et al. 2016;
Protzko 2015).
Whitehurst (
2019), for example, has argued that policies targeting the malleability of ability or personality constructs, for example, are unlikely to be fruitful at scale. Of course, because the hope for the malleability of intelligence is so strong, the rapid adoption of ideas such as “brain training” into schools may have brought intelligence into education—just not good evidence or application of intelligence given the enormous lack of evidence supporting brain training (e.g., see
Simons et al. 2016). We emphasize ensuring that there is solid replicable evidence for any concept prior to its adoption in schools or interventions (e.g.,
Asbury and Wai 2019), though, in practice the field of education is quite susceptible to fads with little evidence at all (
Hirsch 1996;
Ravitch 2000), so new catchy ideas that lack evidence will in all likelihood continue to be adopted in schools.