The Search for Personality–Intelligence Relations: Methodological and Conceptual Issues
Abstract
:1. Background
1.1. Why Should Personality Traits and Intellectual Abilities Be Related to One Another?
“With few or no exceptions superiority in one desirable trait implies superiority in any other. The various sorts of intelligence (with abstractions and symbols, with things and mechanisms, with people and their motives) are positively related; intelligence in general is correlated with virtue and goodwill toward men; both are correlated with skill in control of hand, eye, voice, etc.; all these are correlated with health, poise, sanity, and sensitiveness to beauty. Some of these intercorrelations are low, but they are rarely zero or negative. There is, I think, no demonstrated case of a negative correlation in all the work so far done.”.(pp. 273–274, [4])
1.2. Which Traits?
2. The Current State of the Field
3. Why Substantial Relations Might Not Be Seen
3.1. Measurement Context
3.2. Non-Linear Relations
3.3. Bandwidth Issues and Brunswik Symmetry
3.4. Aggregation Issues
4. Strategies for Finding Personality-Intelligence Relations
4.1. Bipolar Personality Traits
4.2. Missing Linkages
4.3. Other Ability Criteria
4.4. Beyond Self-Report Personality Assessments of Typical Behaviors
4.5. Expand the Domains—Trait Complexes
5. Conclusions
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Johansen, T.K. Plato’s Natural Philosophy; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2004. [Google Scholar]
- Lorge, I. Intelligence: Its Nature and Nurture. 39th Yearbook of the National Society for Study of Education, Part I; Public School Publ. Co.: Bloomington, IL, USA, 1940; pp. 275–281. [Google Scholar]
- Ackerman, P.L.; Heggestad, E.D. Intelligence, personality, and interests: Evidence for overlapping traits. Psychol. Bull. 1997, 121, 219–245. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Thorndike, E.L. Human Nature and the Social Order; Macmillan: New York, NY, USA, 1940. [Google Scholar]
- Goldberg, L.R.; Johnson, J.A.; Eber, H.W.; Hogan, R.; Ashton, M.C.; Cloninger, C.R.; Gough, H.G. The International Personality Item Pool and the future of public-domain personality measures. J. Res. Personal. 2006, 40, 84–96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cattell, R.B. Personality traits associated with abilities. I. With intelligence and drawing ability. Educ. Psychol. Meas. 1945, 5, 131–147. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ziegler, M.; Cengia, A.; Mussel, P.; Gerstorf, D. Openness as a buffer against cognitive decline: The openness-Fluid-Crystallized-Intelligence (OFCI) model applied to late adulthood. Psychol. Aging 2015, 30, 573–588. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ziegler, M.; Danay, E.; Heene, M.; Asendorpf, J.; Bühner, M. Openness, fluid intelligence, and crystallized intelligence: Toward an integrative model. J. Res. Personal. 2012, 46, 173–183. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cattell, R.B. The measurement of adult intelligence. Psychol. Bull. 1943, 40, 153–193. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cattell, R.B. Personality and Motivation Structure and Measurement; World Book Company: Yonkers-on-Hudson, NY, USA, 1957. [Google Scholar]
- Cattell, R.B. Abilities: Their Structure, Growth and Action. [Revised and Reprinted as Intelligence: Its Structure, Growth, and Action]; North-Holland: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 1971/1987. [Google Scholar]
- Hebb, D.O. The effect of early and late brain injury upon test scores, and the nature of normal adult intelligence. Proc. Am. Philos. Soc. 1942, 85, 275–292. [Google Scholar]
- Spearman, C. “General intelligence”, objectively determined and measured. Am. J. Psychol. 1904, 15, 201–293. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Spearman, C. The Abilities of Man: Their Nature and Measurement; Macmillan: New York, NY, USA, 1927. [Google Scholar]
- Welsh, G.S. Creativity and Intelligence: A Personality Approach; Institute for Research in Social Science: Chapel Hill, NC, USA, 1975. [Google Scholar]
- Goff, M.; Ackerman, P.L. Personality-intelligence relations: Assessing typical intellectual engagement. J. Educ. Psychol. 1992, 84, 537–552. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cacioppo, J.T.; Petty, R.E. The need for cognition. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1982, 42, 116–131. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Von Stumm, S.; Ackerman, P.L. Investment and intelligence: A review and meta-analysis. Psychol. Bull. 2013, 139, 841–869. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Mussel, P. Intellect: A theoretical framework for personality traits related to intellectual achievements. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2013, 104, 885–906. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Carroll, J.B. Human Cognitive Abilities: A Survey of Factor-Analytic Studies; Cambridge University Press: New York, NY, USA, 1993. [Google Scholar]
- Cronbach, L.J. Essentials of Psychological Testing; Harper: New York, NY, USA, 1949. [Google Scholar]
- Ackerman, P.L. A theory of adult intellectual development: Process, personality, interests, and knowledge. Intelligence 1996, 22, 229–259. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cattell, R.B. Personality traits associated with abilities. II. With verbal and mathematical abilities. J. Educ. Psychol. 1945, 36, 475–486. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Mischel, W. On the future of personality measurement. Am. Psychol. 1977, 32, 246–254. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wallace, J. An abilities conception of personality: Some implications for personality measurement. Am. Psychol. 1966, 21, 132–138. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Paulhus, D.L.; Martin, C.L. The structure of personality capabilities. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1987, 52, 354–365. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Turner, R.G. Consistency, self-consciousness, and the predictive validity of typical and maximal personality measures. J. Res. Personal. 1978, 12, 117–132. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Willerman, L.; Turner, R.G.; Peterson, M. A comparison of the predictive validity of typical and maximal personality measures. J. Res. Personal. 1976, 10, 482–492. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Horn, J.L. Organization of abilities and the development of intelligence. Psychol. Rev. 1968, 75, 242–259. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Spangler, W.D. Validity of questionnaire and TAT measures of need for achievement: Two meta-analyses. Psychol. Bull. 1992, 112, 140–154. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tett, R.P.; Burnett, D.D. A personality trait-based interactionist model of job performance. J. Appl. Psychol. 2003, 88, 500–517. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Flanagan, D.P.; Genshaft, J.L.; Harrison, P.L. (Eds.) Contemporary Intellectual Assessment: Theories, Tests, and Issues; Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 1997. [Google Scholar]
- Church, A.T. Relating the Tellegen and five-factor models of personality structure. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1994, 67, 898–909. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Paulhus, D.L.; Williams, K.M. The dark triad of personality: Narcissism, Machiavellianism, and psychopathy. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2002, 36, 556–563. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Moutafi, J.; Furnham, A.; Crump, J. What facets of openness and conscientiousness predict fluid intelligence score? Learn. Individ. Differ. 2006, 16, 34–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wittmann, W.W.; Süß, H.-M. Investigating the paths between working memory, intelligence, knowledge, and complex problem-solving performances via Brunswik Symmetry. In Learning and Individual Differences: Process, Trait, and Content Determinants; Ackerman, P.L., Kyllonen, P.C., Roberts, R.D., Eds.; American Psychological Association: Washington, DC, USA, 1999; pp. 77–108. [Google Scholar]
- Beauducel, A.; Liepmann, D.; Felfe, J.; Nettelnstroth, W. The impact of different measurement models for fluid and crystallized intelligence on the correlation with personality traits. Eur. J. Psychol. Assess. 2007, 23, 71–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rushton, J.P.; Irwing, P. A general factor of personality in 16 sets of the big five, the Guilford-Zimmerman Temperament Survey, the California Psychological Inventory, and the Temperament and Character Inventory. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2009, 47, 558–564. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dunkel, C.S. The general factor of personality and general intelligence: Evidence for substantial association. Intelligence 2013, 41, 423–427. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schermer, J.A.; Vernon, P.A. The correlation between general intelligence (g), a general factor of personality (GFP), and social desirability. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2010, 48, 187–189. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Danay, E.; Ziegler, M. Is there really a single factor of personality? A multirater approach to the apex of personality. J. Res. Personal. 2011, 45, 560–567. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ackerman, P.L.; Bowen, K.R.; Beier, M.B.; Kanfer, R. Determinants of individual differences and gender differences in knowledge. J. Educ. Psychol. 2001, 93, 797–825. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Epstein, S. Aggregation and beyond: Some basic issues on the prediction of behavior. J. Personal. 1983, 51, 360–392. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Slavin, R.E. Best-evidence synthesis: An alternative to meta-analytic and traditional reviews. Educ. Res. 1986, 15, 5–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Toker, Y.; Ackerman, P.L. Utilizing occupational complexity levels in vocational interest assessments: Assessing interests for STEM areas. J. Vocat. Behav. 2012, 80, 524–544. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Thorndike, R.L.; Stein, S. An evaluation of the attempts to measure social intelligence. Psychol. Bull. 1937, 34, 275–285. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Poropat, A.E. A meta-analysis of the five-factor model of personality and academic performance. Psychol. Bull. 2009, 135, 322–338. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Mischel, W.; Shoda, Y.; Mendoza-Denton, R. Situation-behavior profiles as a locus of consistency in personality. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2002, 11, 50–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Murtha, T.C.; Kanfer, R.; Ackerman, P.L. Towards an interactionist taxonomy of personality and situations: An integrative situational-dispositional representation of personality traits. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1996, 71, 193–207. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Furnham, A.; Taylor, J.; Chamorro-Premuzic, T. Personality and intelligence correlates of assessment center exercises. Individ. Differ. Res. 2008, 6, 181–192. [Google Scholar]
- Sherif, M.; Harvey, O.J.; White, B.J.; Hood, W.R.; Sherif, C.W. The Robbers Cave Experiment: Intergroup Conflict and Cooperation; Wesleyan University Press: Middletown, CT, USA, 1954/1988. [Google Scholar]
- Goldfried, M.R.; Kent, R.N. Traditional versus behavioral personality assessment: A comparison of methodological and theoretical assumptions. Psychol. Bull. 1972, 77, 409–420. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bersoff, D.N. “Current functioning” myth: An overlooked fallacy in psychological assessment. J. Couns. Clin. Psychol. 1971, 37, 391–393. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ackerman, P.L. Personality, self-concept, interests, and intelligence: Which construct doesn’t fit? J. Personal. 1997, 65, 171–204. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hogan, R. Personality Theory: The Personalogical Tradition; Prentice-Hall: Englewood Cliffs, NJ, USA, 1976. [Google Scholar]
- Snow, R.E. Effects of Learner Characteristics in Learning from Instructional Films. Unpublished Doctoral Thesis, University Microfilms #6404928, Purdue University, West Lafayette, IN, USA, 1963. [Google Scholar]
- Beier, M.E.; Ackerman, P.L. Current events knowledge in adults: An investigation of age, intelligence and non-ability determinants. Psychol. Aging 2001, 16, 615–628. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Beier, M.E.; Ackerman, P.L. Determinants of health knowledge: An investigation of age, gender, abilities, personality, and interests. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2003, 84, 439–448. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cronbach, L.J.; Snow, R.E. Aptitudes and Instructional Methods: A Handbook on Interactions; Irvington: Oxford, UK, 1977. [Google Scholar]
1 | A similar point was made in passing by Cattell [23]. “... there has been a tacit or unconscious conspiracy to maintain certain influences constant when giving tests, without mentioning—often within realizing—that such artificial conditions have been set up. We correlate ability tests under conditions of quiet, of concentration, of common intention to one’s best. We correlate emotional responses in, say, nursery school children, observed under conditions in which cognitive abilities are not required in order to manifest emotion.” (p. 133). |
© 2018 by the author. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Ackerman, P.L. The Search for Personality–Intelligence Relations: Methodological and Conceptual Issues. J. Intell. 2018, 6, 2. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence6010002
Ackerman PL. The Search for Personality–Intelligence Relations: Methodological and Conceptual Issues. Journal of Intelligence. 2018; 6(1):2. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence6010002
Chicago/Turabian StyleAckerman, Phillip L. 2018. "The Search for Personality–Intelligence Relations: Methodological and Conceptual Issues" Journal of Intelligence 6, no. 1: 2. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence6010002
APA StyleAckerman, P. L. (2018). The Search for Personality–Intelligence Relations: Methodological and Conceptual Issues. Journal of Intelligence, 6(1), 2. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence6010002