Comparing Business Experts and Novices in Complex Problem Solving
Abstract
:1. Introduction
1.1. Complex Problem Solving and Dynamic Decision Making under Uncertainty Using Micro-Worlds
1.2. Expertise and Crystallized Intelligence
1.3. Expertise and CPS Performance
1.4. Rigidity versus Flexibility in CPS
1.5. Expertise and Problem Exploration
1.6. Research Questions
- How will performance differ between experts, semi-experts and novices in the CHOCO-FINE micro-world simulation?
- Are there differences in rigid and flexible decision making between experts, semi-experts and novices?
- What are the differences between experts, semi-experts and novices in their initial search and exploration for information in the CHOCO-FINE micro-world simulation?
- What are the effects of in-depth exploration and flexibility on CPS performance?
2. Materials and Method
2.1. Participants
2.2. CHOCO-FINE Simulation
- (1)
- Performance: Performance was operationalized as total monies at the end of each month. Total monies at Month 19 was chosen as the performance variable for the correlations.
- (2a)
- Decision-making/problem-solving areas: The focus on specific decision-making areas was operationalized through the amount of money spent during each month for each of the three decision-making areas: advertising, personnel and market research. For advertising, participants could have chosen general overall brand advertising or specific advertising for specific chocolates or specific product profile components. For market research, participants had access to view and purchase information related to their own products and clients, as well as their competitor products and clients. Regarding personnel, participants had expenses, such as salaries, hiring, firing and relocating.
- (2b)
- Decision-making changes: The changes in each specific decision-making area were operationalized through the number of changes from one month to the next month relative to the total number of months completed. Change were coded as binary, either as a “0” if no change happened from one month to the next month, or as “1” if a change happened. For example, 10 changes in representatives in 19 months would result in .53.
- (3)
- Exploration: Depth of exploration was operationalized as time spent working on the first two months of the simulation. Participants were able to control proceeding to the next month of the simulation by clicking the “continue” button after all decisions for the month were made. At first, we considered only taking the first month, but since 20% of all participants spent less than 10 min for both months and since the first month also requires adjustment to the screens, we decided that taking the first two month would provide a clearer picture on exploration.
2.3. Demographic Survey
2.4. Procedure and Data Analysis
3. Results
3.1. Face Validity of CHOCO-FINE
3.2. Expertise and Performance
3.3. Rigidity versus Flexibility in CPS
- (a)
- Three main areas were investigated; advertising, market research and personnel.
- (b)
- Changes in decision making.
3.4. Initial Problem Exploration
3.5. Predicators of CPS Performance
4. Discussion
Limitations and Future Directions
5. Conclusions
Acknowledgments
Author Contributions
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Broadbent, D.E. Levels, hierarchies, and the locus of control. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 1977, 29, 181–201. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Brehmer, B. Dynamic decision making: Human control of complex systems. Acta Psychol. 1992, 81, 211–241. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Brehmer, B.; Dörner, D. Experiments with computer-simulated micro-worlds: Escaping both the narrow straits of the laboratory and the deep blue sea of the field study. Comput. Hum. Behav. 1993, 9, 171–184. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dörner, D. On the difficulties people have in dealing with complexity. Simul. Gaming 1980, 11, 87–106. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Funke, J. Microworlds based on linear equation systems: A new approach to complex problem solving and experimental results. In The Cognitive Psychology of Knowledge; Strube, G., Wender, K.-F., Eds.; Elsevier: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 1993; pp. 313–330. [Google Scholar]
- Frensch, P.; Funke, J. (Eds.) Complex Problem Solving: The European Perspective; Lawrence Erlbaum: Hillsdale, MI, USA, 1995. [Google Scholar]
- Gonzalez, C.; Vanyukov, P.; Martin, M.K. The use of micro-worlds to study dynamic decision making. Comput. Hum. Behav. 2005, 21, 273–286. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sterman, J.D. Misperceptions of feedback in dynamic decision making. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 1989, 43, 301–335. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ellington, J.K.; Dierdorff, E.C. Individual learning in team training: Self-regulation and team context effects. Small Group Res. 2014, 45, 37–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schminke, M. Computer-based job simulation: A complementary approach to organizational research. J. Bus. Psychol. 1990, 4, 293–315. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schmid, U.; Ragni, M.; Gonzalez, C.; Funke, J. The challenge of complexity for cognitive systems. Cogn. Syst. Res. 2011, 12, 211–218. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dörner, D. The Logic of Failure; Holt: New York, NY, USA, 1996. [Google Scholar]
- Lyon, D.W.; Lumpkin, G.T.; Dess, G.D. Enhancing entrepreneurial orientation research: Operationalizing and measuring a key strategic decision making process. J. Manag. 2000, 26, 1055–1085. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dane, E. Reconsidering the trade-off between expertise and flexibility: A cognitive entrenchment perspective. Acad. Manag. Rev. 2010, 35, 579–603. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rosen, M.A.; Shuffler, M.; Salas, E. How experts make decisions: Beyond the JDM paradigm. Ind. Organ. Psychol. Perspect. Sci. Pract. 2010, 3, 438–442. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cattell, R.B. Theory of fluid and crystallized intelligence: A critical experiment. J. Educ. Psychol. 1963, 54, 1–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Horn, J.L.; Cattell, R.B. Refinement and test of the theory of fluid and crystallized intelligence. J. Educ. Psychol. 1966, 57, 253–270. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Postlethwaite, B.E. Fluid Ability, Crystallized Ability, and Performance across Multiple Domains: A Meta-Analysis. Ph.D. Thesis, University of Iowa, Iowa City, IA, USA, 2011. [Google Scholar]
- Sternberg, R.J. Intelligence as developing expertise. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 1999, 24, 359–375. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ericsson, K.A.; Krampe, R.T.; Tesch-Römer, C. The role of deliberate practice in the acquisition of expert performance. Psychol. Rev. 1993, 100, 363–406. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bilalic, M.; Mcleod, P.; Gobet, F. Specialization effect and its influence on memory and problem solving in expert chess players (English). Cogn. Sci. 2009, 33, 1117–1143. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Rudolph, J.W.; Morrison, J.; Carroll, J.S. The dynamics of actions-oriented problem solving: Linking interpretation and choice. Acad. Manag. Rev. 2009, 34, 733–756. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sternberg, R.J. Costs of expertise. In The Road to Excellence: The Acquisition of Expert Performance in the Arts and Sciences, Sports, and Games; Ericsson, K.A., Ed.; Lawrence Erlbaum Associates: Hillsdale, MI, USA, 1996; pp. 347–354. [Google Scholar]
- Ericsson, K.A. Adaptive expertise & cognitive readiness: A perspective from the expert-performance approach. In Teaching and Measuring Cognitive Readiness; O’Neil, H.F., Perez, R.S., Baker, E.L., Eds.; Springer: Houten, The Netherlands, 2013; pp. 179–197. [Google Scholar]
- Necka, E.; Kubik, T. How non-experts fail where experts do not: Implications of expertise for resistance to cognitive rigidity. Stud. Psychol. 2012, 54, 3–14. [Google Scholar]
- Chi, M.T.H.; Feltovich, P.J.; Glaser, R. Categorization and representation of physics problems by experts and novices. Cogn. Sci. 1981, 5, 121–152. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gobet, F.; Simon, H.A. Templates in chess memory: A mechanism for recalling several boards. Cogn. Psychol. 1996, 31, 1–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Dörner, D.; Güss, C.D. PSI: A computational architecture of cognition, motivation, and emotion. Rev. Gen. Psychol. 2013, 17, 297–317. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hinds, P.; Patterson, M.; Pfeffer, J. Bothered by abstraction: The effect of expertise on knowledge transfer and subsequent novice performance. J. Appl. Psychol. 2001, 86, 1232–1243. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- García López, L.M.; Díaz del Campo, D.G.; Hernández, J.A.; González-Villora, S.; Webb, L.A. Expert-novice differences in procedural knowledge in young soccer players from local to international level. J. Hum. Sport Exerc. 2010, 5, 444–452. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gil, A.; Moreno, M.P.; García-González, L.; Moreno, A.; Del Villar, F. Analysis of declarative and procedural knowledge in volleyball according to the level of practice and players’ age. Percept. Motor Skills 2012, 115, 632–644. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Jersild, A.T. Mental set and shift. Arch. Psychol. 1927, 89, 5–82. [Google Scholar]
- Öllinger, M.; Jones, G.; Knoblich, G. Investigating the effect of mental set on insight problem solving. Exp. Psychol. 2008, 55, 269–282. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Luchins, A.S. Mechanization in problem solving: The effect of Einstellung. Psychol. Monogr. 1942, 54, 95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bilalic, M.; McLeod, P.; Gobet, F. The mechanism of the Einstellung (Set) Effect: A pervasive source of cognitive bias. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2010, 19, 111–115. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hoffman, R.R. How can expertise be defined? Implications of research from Cognitive Psychology. In Exploring Expertise; Williams, R., Faulkner, W., Fleck, J., Eds.; University of Edinburgh Press: Edinburgh, UK, 1996; pp. 81–100. [Google Scholar]
- Wiley, J. Expertise as mental set: The effects of domain knowledge in creative problem solving. Mem. Cogn. 1998, 26, 716–730. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Simon, H.A. The structure of ill-structured problems. Artif. Intell. 1973, 4, 181–201. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Morrison, E.W.; Vancouver, J.B. Within-person analysis of information seeking: The effects of perceived costs and benefits. J. Manag. 2000, 26, 119–137. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Strohschneider, S.; Schaub, H. Können Manager wirklich so gut managen? Über die Effekte unterschiedlichen heuristischen Wissens beim Umgang mit komplexen Problemen [Are managers really good at managing? The effects of different types of heuristic knowledge in solving complex problems]. Z. Psychol. 1991, 11, 325–340. [Google Scholar]
- Holding, D.H. Theories of chess skill. Psychol. Res. 1992, 54, 10–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Krems, J.F.; Zierer, C. Sind Experten gegen kognitive Täuschungen gefeit? Zur Abhängigkeit des confirmation bias von Fachwissen [Are experts immune to cognitive bias? The dependence of confirmation bias on specialist knowledge]. Z. Exp. Angew. Psychol. 1994, 41, 98–115. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
- Feltovich, P.J.; Prietula, M.J.; Ericcson, K.A. Studies of expertise from psychological perspectives. In The Cambridge Handbook of Expertise and Expert Performance; Ericcson, K.A., Charness, N., Feltovich, P., Hoffman, R.R., Eds.; Cambridge University Press: New York, NY, USA, 1996; pp. 41–61. [Google Scholar]
- Vessey, I. Expertise in debugging computer systems: A process analysis. Int. J. Man-Mach. Stud. 1985, 23, 459–494. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dörner, D. SchokoFin. Computer Simulation; Otto-Friedrich Universität Bamberg: Bamberg, Germany, 2000. [Google Scholar]
- Güss, C.D.; Tuason, M.T.; Orduña, L.V. Strategies, tactics, and errors in dynamic decision making. J. Dyn. Decis. Mak. 2015, 1. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Muthén, L.K.; Muthén, B.O. Mplus 5. Computer Program. 2006. Available online: http://www.statmodel.com/index.shtml (accessed on 10 April 2017).
- Güss, C.D.; Fadil, P.; Strohschneider, S. The influence of uncertainty avoidance on dynamic business decision making across cultures: A growth mixture modeling approach. Int. Bus. Teach. Res. Pract. 2012, 6, 12–30. [Google Scholar]
- Cloyd, C.B. Performance in tax research tasks: The joint effects of knowledge and accountability. Account. Rev. 1997, 72, 111–131. [Google Scholar]
- Weick, K.E.; Sutcliffe, K.M. Managing the Unexpected: Resilient Performance in the Age of Uncertainty, 2nd ed.; Jossey-Bass: San Francisco, CA, USA, 2007. [Google Scholar]
- Del Campo, D.; Villora, S.; Lopez, L.; Mitchell, S. Differences in decision-making development between expert and novice invasion game players. Percept. Motor Skills 2011, 112, 871–888. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Mitchell, J.R.; Shephers, D.A.; Sharfman, M.P. Erratic strategic decisions: When and why managers are inconsistent in strategic decision making. Strat. Manag. J. 2011, 32, 683–704. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Beaman, J.M. A Qualitative Phenomenological Study of Emotional Intelligence: Effects of Stress on Small Business Leaders. Ph.D. Thesis, University of Phoenix, Tempe, AZ, USA, 2011. [Google Scholar]
- Korsgaard, M.A.; Diddams, M. The effect of process feedback and task complexity on personal goals, information searching, and performance improvement. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 1996, 26, 1889–1911. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- DiBello, L.; Lehmann, D.; Missildine, W. How do you find an expert? Identifying blind spots and complex mental models among key organizational decision makers using a unique profiling tool. In Informed by Knowledge: Expert Performance in Complex Situations; Mosier, K.L., Fischer, U.M., Eds.; Psychology Press: New York, NY, USA, 2011; pp. 261–274. [Google Scholar]
- Gary, M.S.; Wood, R.E. Mental models, decision rules, and performance heterogeneity. Strat. Manag. J. 2011, 32, 569–594. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Barrick, J.A.; Spilker, B.C. The relations between knowledge, search strategy, and performance in unaided and aided information search. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 2003, 90, 1–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Güss, C.D.; Tuason, M.T.; Gerhard, C. Cross-national comparisons of complex problem solving strategies in two micro-worlds. Cogn. Sci. 2010, 34, 489–520. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Sánchez-Manzanares, M.; Rico, R.; Gil, F. Designing organizations: Does expertise matter? J. Bus. Psychol. 2008, 23, 87–101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kröner, S.; Plass, J.L.; Leutner, D. Intelligence assessment with computer simulations. Intelligence 2005, 33, 347–368. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Süß, H.-M. Die Rolle von Intelligenz und Wissen für erfolgreiches Handeln in komplexen Problemsituationen [The role of intelligence and knowledge for successful performance in complex problem solving]. In Komplexität und Kompetenz: Ausgewählte Fragen der Kompetenzforschung; Franke, G., Ed.; Bertelsmann: Bielefeld, Germany, 2001; pp. 249–275. [Google Scholar]
- Burkolter, D.; Meyer, B.; Kluge, A.; Sauer, J. Assessment of structural knowledge as a training outcome in process control environments. Hum. Factors 2010, 52, 119–138. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Van Gog, T.; Paas, F.; Van Merriënboer, J.G. Effects of studying sequences of process-oriented and product-oriented worked examples on troubleshooting transfer efficiency. Learn. Instr. 2008, 18, 211–222. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Leemkuil, H.; de Jong, T. Adaptive advice in learning with a computer-based knowledge management simulation game. Acad. Manag. Learn. Educ. 2012, 11, 3–665. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gonzalez, C. Decision support for real-time dynamic decision making tasks. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 2005, 96, 142–154. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Haerem, T.; Rau, D. The influence of degree of expertise and objective task complexity on perceived task complexity and performance. J. Appl. Psychol. 2007, 92, 1320–1331. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Greiff, S.; Fischer, A.; Stadler, M.; Wüstenberg, S. Assessing complex problem solving skills with Multiple Complex Systems. Think. Reason. 2015, 21, 356–382. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Greiff, S.; Wüstenberg, S.; Funke, J. Dynamic problem solving: A new assessment perspective. Appl. Psychol. Meas. 2012, 36, 189–213. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Funke, J. Analysis of minimal complex systems and complex problem solving require different forms of causal cognition. Front. Psychol. 2014, 5, 739. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Scherer, R.; Greiff, S. Editorial on the Special Issue “Complex Problem Solving and its Position in the Wider Realm of the Human Intellect”. J. Intell. Unpublished work. 2017. [Google Scholar]
- Ramnarayan, S.; Strohschneider, S.; Schaub, H. Trappings of expertise and the pursuit of failure. Simul. Gaming 1997, 28, 28–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rasmussen, J. Human errors. A taxonomy for describing human malfunction in industrial installations. J. Occup. Accid. 1982, 4, 311–333. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Reason, J. Human Error; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 1990. [Google Scholar]
- Osman, M. Controlling uncertainty: A review of human behavior in complex dynamic environments. Psychol. Bull. 2010, 136, 65–86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Güss, C.D.; Dörner, D. Cultural differences in dynamic decision-making strategies in a nonlinear, time-delayed task. Cogn. Syst. Res. 2011, 12, 365–376. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Danner, D.; Hagemann, D.; Holt, D.V.; Hager, M.; Schankin, A.; Wüstenberg, S.; Funke, J. Measuring performance in a complex problem solving task: Reliability and validity of the Tailorshop simulation. J. Individ. Differ. 2011, 32, 225–233. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sitzmann, T. A meta-analytic examination of the instructional effectiveness of computer-based simulation games. Pers. Psychol. 2011, 64, 489–528. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Burke, C.S.; Pierce, L.G.; Salas, E. (Eds.) Understanding Adaptability: A Prerequisite for Effective Performance within Complex Environments; Elsevier: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2006. [Google Scholar]
- Evers, F.T.; Rush, J.C. The bases of competence: Skill development during the transition from university to work. Manag. Learn. 1996, 27, 275–299. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hoffman, R.R.; Ward, P.; Feltovich, P.J.; DiBello, L.; Fiore, S.M.; Andrews, D.H. Accelerated Expertise: Training for High Proficiency in a Complex World; Psychology Press: New York, NY, USA, 2014. [Google Scholar]
- Baring, E. Lieutenant. In Staff College Essays; Longmans, Green, Co.: London, UK, 1870. [Google Scholar]
- Lenin, V.I. Collected Works, Vol. 33 (English Translation); Progress Publishers: Moscow, Russia, 1973. [Google Scholar]
Wilk’s Lambda | F | df | p | ƞ2p | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Advertising expenses | |||||
Group × Month | .60 | 1.66 | (36, 206) | .02 | .23 |
Month | .60 | 3.83 | (18, 103) | <.001 | .40 |
Group | - | .01 | (2, 120) | .99 | .00 |
Market research expenses | |||||
Group × Month | .68 | 1.15 | (36, 198) | .27 | .17 |
Month | .61 | 3.57 | (18, 99) | <.001 | .39 |
Group | - | 1.63 | (2, 116) | .20 | .03 |
Personnel expenses | |||||
Group × Month | .66 | 1.36 | (36, 210) | .09 | .19 |
Month | .78 | 1.68 | (18, 105) | .05 | .22 |
Group | - | 1.50 | (2, 122) | .23 | .02 |
Advertising changes | - | 21.72 | (2, 137) | <.001 | .24 |
Market research changes | - | 3.92 | (2, 137) | .02 | .05 |
Personnel changes | - | 16.78 | (2, 135) | <.001 | .20 |
Initial problem exploration time (first 2 months) | - | 6.33 | (2, 137) | .002 | .09 |
Performance | Performance, Controlled for Age and Gender | M | SD | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Performance: Total monies at Month 19 | - | - | 157,428.63 | 1,699,632.51 |
Exploration: Time for first two months | .05 | .05 | 20.55 | 11.92 |
Total months completed | −.04 | −.04 | 23.03 | 3.02 |
Decision making: Mean of expenses for advertising (1 to 19) | −.17 * | −.17 * | 10,777.54 | 22,260.11 |
Decision making: Mean of expenses for market research (1 to 19) | −.04 | −.004 | 1,593.22 | 1638.49 |
Decision making: Mean of expenses for personnel (1 to 19) | −.28 ** | −.30 ** | 113,179.74 | 32,328.23 |
Advertising changes | .17 * | .15 | .43 | .27 |
Market research changes | −.15 | −.12 | .34 | .20 |
Personnel changes | .37 *** | .35 *** | .73 | .19 |
Standardized Beta | t | p | 95% Confidence Interval for B | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Lower | Upper | ||||
Age | −0.07 | −0.75 | .46 | −38,536.21 | 17,397.34 |
Gender | 0.21 | 2.46 | .02 | 140,710.90 | 1,303,929.19 |
Exploration: Time for first two months | 0.11 | 1.25 | .22 | −10,275.91 | 45,293.20 |
Total months completed | −0.02 | −0.25 | .81 | −398,608.24 | 310,715.24 |
Total advertising expenses | −0.11 | −1.30 | .20 | −20.71 | 4.31 |
Total market research expenses | 0.07 | 0.61 | .54 | −161.15 | 304.72 |
Total personnel expenses | −0.28 | −3.14 | .002 | −23.21 | −5.25 |
Advertising changes | 0.13 | 1.48 | .14 | −275,343.05 | 1,882,048.28 |
Market research changes | −0.04 | −0.30 | .77 | −2,221,426.93 | 1,642,531.64 |
Personnel changes | 0.36 | 4.01 | <.001 | 1,584,115.59 | 4,675,047.85 |
© 2017 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Güss, C.D.; Devore Edelstein, H.; Badibanga, A.; Bartow, S. Comparing Business Experts and Novices in Complex Problem Solving. J. Intell. 2017, 5, 20. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence5020020
Güss CD, Devore Edelstein H, Badibanga A, Bartow S. Comparing Business Experts and Novices in Complex Problem Solving. Journal of Intelligence. 2017; 5(2):20. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence5020020
Chicago/Turabian StyleGüss, C. Dominik, Hannah Devore Edelstein, Ali Badibanga, and Sandy Bartow. 2017. "Comparing Business Experts and Novices in Complex Problem Solving" Journal of Intelligence 5, no. 2: 20. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence5020020
APA StyleGüss, C. D., Devore Edelstein, H., Badibanga, A., & Bartow, S. (2017). Comparing Business Experts and Novices in Complex Problem Solving. Journal of Intelligence, 5(2), 20. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence5020020