Discrimination of False Response from Object Reality in False Belief Test in Preschool Children
Abstract
1. Introduction
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Participants
2.2. Materials
2.3. Procedures
2.4. Coding and Data Analysis
3. Results
3.1. The Differences in Responses to Four FB Tasks
Age; Month/N | Standard FB Test | Box Condition | Basket Condition | No-Object Condition | ||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Correct (Basket) | False (Object’s Location-Box) | False | Correct (Box) | First Location (Basket) | False | Correct (Box) | Object’s Location (Basket) | Correct (Box) | Object’s Location (Outside of the Scene) | First Location (Basket) | False | |
3–3; 5 N = 11 | 1 (9.1%) | 10 (90.9%) | 0 | 9 (81.8%) | 1 (9.1%) | 1 (9.1%) | 4 (36.4%) | 7 (63.6%) | 4 (36.4%) | 6 (54.5%) | 1 (9.1%) | 0 |
3; 6–3; 11 N = 11 | 1 (9.1%) | 10 (90.9%) | 0 | 10 (90.9%) | 1 (9.1%) | 0 | 4 (36.4%) | 7 (63.6%) | 0 | 8 (72.7%) | 3 (27.3%) | 0 |
4–4; 5 N = 24 | 5 (20.8%) | 19 (79.2%) | 0 | 21 (87.5%) | 3 (12.5%) | 0 | 7 (29.2%) | 17 (70.8%) | 8 (33.3%) | 14 (58.3%) | 1 (4.2%) | 1 (4.2%) |
4; 6–4; 11 N = 38 | 9 (23.2%) | 29 (76.3%) | 0 | 30 (78.9%) | 8 (21.1%) | 0 | 14 (36.8%) | 24 (63.2%) | 10 (26.3%) | 21 (55.3%) | 7 (18.4%) | 0 |
5–5; 5 N = 29 | 14 (48.3%) | 14 (48.3%) | 1 (3.4%) | 22 (75.9%) | 6 (20.7%) | 1 (3.4%) | 17 (58.6%) | 12 (41.4%) | 16 (55.2%) | 8 (27.6%) | 5 (17.2%) | 0 |
5; 6–5; 11 N = 28 | 14 (50%) | 14 (50%) | 0 | 25 (89.3%) | 3 (10.7%) | 0 | 22 (78.6%) | 6 (21.4%) | 21 (75%) | 4 (14.3%) | 3 (10.7%) | 0 |
6–6; 5 N = 8 | 3 (37.5%) | 5 (62.5%) | 0 | 7 (87.5%) | 1 (12.5%) | 0 | 7 (87.5%) | 1 (12.5%) | 6 (75%) | 2 (25%) | 0 | 0 |
Basket Condition | Total | χ2 | p | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Correct Response | Object’s Location | |||||
Standard FB test | Correct response | 36 | 12 | 48 | 0.31 | .000 *** |
Object’s location | 39 | 61 | 100 | |||
Total | 75 | 73 | 148 |
No Object | Total | χ2 | p | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Correct Response | Object’s Location | |||||
Standard FB test | Correct response | 31 | 6 | 37 | 0.31 | .000 *** |
Object’s location | 33 | 58 | 91 | |||
Total | 64 | 64 | 128 |
3.2. The Effect of Age and Receptive Vocabulary Acquisition on FB Performance
4. Discussion
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Conflicts of Interest
Abbreviations
ToM | Theory of mind |
FB | False belief |
BR | Belief reasoning |
RR | Reality reasoning |
PAR | Perceptual access reasoning |
PPVT | Peabody picture-vocabulary test |
ANOVA | Analysis of variance |
VIF | Variance inflation factor |
References
- Allen, Jedediah W. P. 2015. How to help: Can more active behavioral measures help transcend the infant false-belief debate? New Ideas in Psychology 39: 63–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Apperly, Ian A. 2012. What is “theory of mind”? Concepts, cognitive processes and individual differences. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 65: 825–39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Baron-Cohen, Simon. 2000. Theory of mind and autism: A review. International Review of Research in Mental Retardation 23: 169–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Baron-Cohen, Simon, Alan M. Leslie, and Uta Frith. 1985. Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind”? Cognition 21: 37–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bloom, Paul, and Tim P. German. 2000. Two reasons to abandon the false belief task as a theory of mind. Cognition 77: B25–B31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Brock, Laura L., Helyn Kim, Claire C. Gutshall, and David W. Grissmer. 2019. The development of theory of mind: Predictors and moderators of improvement ink indergarten. Early Child Development and Care 189: 1914–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Brune, Martin, and Ute Brune-Cohrs. 2006. Theory of mind—Evolution, ontogeny, brain mechanisms and psychopathology. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 30: 437–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Buttelmann, David. 2017. Calling for Careful Designs for the Evaluation of Interactive Behavioral Measures on Early False-Belief Reasoning. Frontiers in Psychology 8: 1302. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- de Villiers, Jill G. 2021. The role(s) of language in theory of mind. In The Neural Basis of Mentalizing. Edited by Michael Gilead and Kevin N. Ochsner. New York: Springer, pp. 423–48. [Google Scholar]
- Dörrenberg, Sebastian, Lisa Wenzel, Marina Proft, Hannes Rakoczy, and Ulf Liszkowski. 2019. Reliability and generalizability of an acted-out false belief task in 3-year-olds. Infant Behavior and Development 54: 13–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Dunn, Judy, Jane Brown, Cheryl Slomkowski, Caroline Tesla, and Lise Youngblade. 1991. Young children’s understanding of other people’s feelings and beliefs: Individual differences and their antecedents. Child Development 62: 1352–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dunn, Lloyd M., and Leota M. Dunn. 1997. Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test, 3rd ed. Circle Pines: American Guidance Service. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fabricius, William V. 2023. Development of representational theory of mind: Concepts of mental states, awareness of thinking, and self-permanence. Advances in Child Development and Behaviour 65: 35–68. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fabricius, William V., and Suzanne L. Khalil. 2003. False beliefs or false positives? Limits on children’s understanding of mental representations. Journal of Cognition and Development 4: 239–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fabricius, William V., Christopher R. Gonzales, Annelise Pesch, and Amy A. Weimer. 2023. Perceptual access reasoning: What are the alternatives? Cognitive Development 66: 101306. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fabricius, William V., Ty W. Boyer, Amy A. Weimer, and Kathleen Carroll. 2010. True or False: Do 5-Year-Olds Understand Belief? Developmental Psychology 46: 1402–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Flavell, John H. 1988. The development of children’s knowledge about the mind: From cognitive connections to mental representations. In Developing Theories of Mind. Edited by Janet W. Astington, Paul L. Harris and David R. Olson. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 244–67. [Google Scholar]
- Freeman, Norman H., Charlie Lewis, and Martin J. Doherty. 1991. Preschoolers’ grasp of a desire for knowledge in false-belief prediction: Practical intelligence and verbal report. British Journal of Developmental Psychology 9: 139–57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gaffan, Elizabeth A., Carla Martins, Sarah Healy, and Lynne Murray. 2010. Early social experience and individual differences in infants’ joint attention. Social Development 19: 369–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hale, Courtney Melinda, and Helen Tager-Flusberg. 2003. The influence of language on theory of mind: A training study. Developmental Science 6: 346–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hughes, Claire, and Alexandra L. Cutting. 1999. Nature, nurture, and individual differences in early understanding of mind. Psychological Science 10: 429–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hughes, Claire, Sara R. Jaffee, Francesca Happé, Alan Taylor, Avshalom Caspi, and Terrie E. Moffitt. 2005. Origins of individual differences in theory of mind: From nature to nurture? Child Development 76: 356–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Katz, Jack. 1974. A Turkish Peabody picture vocabulary test. Hacettepe Bulletin of Social Sciences & Humanities 6: 129–42. [Google Scholar]
- Kaysılı, Bahar Keçeli, and Funda Acarlar. 2011. Zihin kuramının 3–5 yaşları arasındaki çocuklarda gelişiminin yanlış inanç performansına göre incelenmesi. Kuram ve Uygulamada Eğitim Bilimleri 11: 1809–26. [Google Scholar]
- Legerstee, Maria, Gabriela Markova, and Tamara Fisher. 2007. The role of maternal effect attunement in dyadic and traidic communication. Infant Behavior and Development 30: 296–306. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Milligan, Karen, Janet Wilde Astington, and Lisa Ain Dack. 2007. Language and theory of mind: Meta-analysis of the relation between language ability and false-belief understanding. Child Development 78: 622–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mitchell, Peter, and Hazel Lacohee. 1991. Children’s early understanding of false belief. Cognition 39: 107–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Mo, Shuliang, Yanjie Su, Mark A. Sabbagh, and Xiu Jiaming. 2014. Sentential complements and false belief understanding in Chinese Mandarin-speaking preschoolers: A training study. Cognitive Development 29: 50–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Moses, Louis J. 1993. Young children’s understanding of belief constraints on intention. Cognitive Development 8: 1–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- O’Neill, Daniela K. 1996. Two-year-old children’s sensitivity to a parent’s knowledge state when making request. Child Development 67: 659–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Öner, Necla. 1997. Türkiye’de Kullanılan Psikolojik Testler: Bir Başvuru Kaynağı. İstanbul: Boğaziçi University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Özekes, Mustafa. 2016. Peabody resim kelime testi III (R) 4 yaş ve 5 yaş çocuklar için İzmir bölgesi standardizasyonu çalışması. Ege Eğitim Dergisi 17: 272–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Perner, Josef. 1991. Understanding the Representational Mind: Learning, Development, and Conceptual Change. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press. [Google Scholar]
- Perner, Josef. 1999. Theory of mind. In Developmental Psychology: Achievements and Prospects. Edited by Mark Bennett. New York: Psychology Press, pp. 205–30. [Google Scholar]
- Perner, Josef, Sandra Stummer, Manuel Sprung, and Martin Doherty. 2002. Theory of mind finds its Piagetian perspective: Why alternative naming comes with understanding belief. Cognitive Development 17: 1451–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Povinelli, Daniel J., Timothy J. Eddy, R. Peter Hobson, and Michael Tomasello. 1996. What young chimpanzees know about seeing? Monographs of the Society for Research in Children Development 61: i+iii+v-vi+1–189. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Roth, Daniel, and Alan M Leslie. 1998. Solving belief problems: Toward a task analysis. Cognition 66: 1–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Rubio-Fernandez, Paula, and Bart Geurts. 2016. Don’t Mention the Marble! The Role of Attentional Processes in False-Belief Tasks. Review of Philosophy and Psychology 7: 835–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Setoh, Peipei, Rose M. Scott, and Renée Baillargeon. 2016. Two-and-a-half-year-olds succeed at a traditional false-belief task with reduced processing demands. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 113: 13360–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Slaughter, Virginia. 2015. Theory of mind in infants and young children: A review. Australian Psychologist 50: 169–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sodian, Beate. 2021. Challenging PAR. [Peer Commentary on the Article “Perceptual Access Reasoning (PAR) in Developing a Representational Theory of Mind.” by Fabricius, Gonzales, Pesch, Weimer, Pugliese, Carroll, Bolnick, Kupfer, Eisenberg, and Spinrad]. Monograph Matters. Available online: https://monographmatters.srcd.org/2021/09/28/commentary-sodian-86-3 (accessed on 13 September 2024).
- Tomasello, Michael. 2008. Origins of Human Communication. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tomasello, Michael, Malinda Carpenter, Josep Call, Tanya Behne, and Henrike Moll. 2005. Understanding and sharing intentions: The origins of cultural cognition. Behavioural and Brain Sciences 28: 675–735. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Wellman, Henry M., David Cross, and Julanne Watson. 2001. Meta-analysis of theory-of-mind development: The truth about false belief. Child Development 72: 655–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wimmer, Heinz, and Josef Perner. 1983. Beliefs about beliefs: Representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children’s understanding of deception. Cognition 13: 103–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wimmer, Heinz, G.-Jurgen Hogrefe, and Josef Perner. 1988. Children’s understanding of informational access as source of knowledge. Child Development 59: 386–96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zaitchik, Deborah. 1991. Is only seeing really believing? Source of the true belief in the false belief task. Cognitive Development 6: 91–103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Age Group (Year; Month) | N | Age (Month) | Receptive Vocabulary Acquisition | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Girl | Boy | Total | Mean (SD) | Min–Max | Mean (SD) | |
3–3; 5 | 8 | 3 | 11 | 39.36 (1.50) | 34–97 | 65 (22.93) |
3; 6–3; 11 | 7 | 4 | 11 | 44.45 (1.91) | 38–94 | 71.55 (15.38) |
4–4; 5 | 13 | 11 | 24 | 50.29 (1.80) | 36–97 | 73.63 (15.84) |
4; 6–4; 11 | 18 | 20 | 38 | 56.47 (2.07) | 36–113 | 90.18 (16.86) |
5–5; 5 | 17 | 12 | 29 | 62.07 (1.87) | 34–113 | 95.28 (14.26) |
5; 6–5; 11 | 14 | 14 | 28 | 68 (2.95) | 85–121 | 104.71 (10.51) |
6–6; 5 | 3 | 5 | 8 | 74.25 (1.28) | 65–119 | 104.88 (18.53) |
Variables | Standard FB Test Reference Category: Correct Answer | Age Without Receptive Vocabulary Acquisition | Receptive Vocabulary Acquisition Without Age | Both Age and Receptive Vocabulary Acquisition | |||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
B | SE | p | OR | B | SE | p | OR | B | SE | p | OR | ||
— | Object’s location | ||||||||||||
Age | −0.069 | 0.021 | .001 *** | 0.933 | — | −0.060 | 0.026 | .021 * | 0.942 | ||||
Receptive vocabulary acquisition | — | −0.026 | 0.010 | .013 * | 0.975 | −0.007 | 0.012 | .993 | 0.969 | ||||
— | Wrong | ||||||||||||
Age | 0.020 | 0.120 | .869 | 0.020 | — | 0.004 | 0.152 | .980 | 1.004 | ||||
Receptive vocabulary acquisition | — | 0.018 | 0.071 | .804 | 1.018 | 0.015 | 0.083 | .861 | 1.015 | ||||
— | Model General Fit | ||||||||||||
χ2 (df) | χ2 (2) = 12.674 | χ2 (2) = 7.350 | χ2 (4) = 13.061 | ||||||||||
p-value | .002 ** | .025 * | .011 * |
Variables | Basket Condition Reference Category: Correct Answer | Age Without Receptive Vocabulary Acquisition | Receptive Vocabulary Acquisition Without Age | Both Age and Receptive Vocabulary Acquisition | |||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
B | SE | p | OR | B | SE | p | OR | B | SE | p | OR | ||
— | Object’s location | ||||||||||||
Age | −0.087 | 0.021 | .000 *** | 0.916 | — | −0.082 | 0.026 | .002 ** | 0.921 | ||||
Receptive vocabulary acquisition | — | −0.028 | 0.009 | .002 ** | 0.972 | −0.004 | 0.012 | .761 | 0.996 | ||||
— | Wrong | ||||||||||||
Age | 0.0295 | 0.248 | .243 | 0.343 | — | 13.19 | 0.660 | .984 | 0.535 | ||||
Receptive vocabulary acquisition | — | −0.062 | 0.042 | .142 | 0.940 | −3.40 | 0.625 | .996 | 0.033 | ||||
— | Model General Fit | ||||||||||||
χ2 (df) | χ2 (2) = 24.993 | χ2 (2) = 11.675 | χ2 (4) = 33.570 | ||||||||||
p-value | .000 *** | .003 ** | .000 *** |
Variables | No-Object Condition Reference Category: Correct Answer | Age Without Receptive Vocabulary Acquisition | Receptive Vocabulary Acquisition Without Age | Both Age and Receptive Vocabulary Acquisition | |||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
B | SE | p | OR | B | SE | p | OR | B | SE | p | OR | ||
— | Object’s location | ||||||||||||
Age | −0.092 | 0.022 | .000 *** | 0.912 | — | −0.070 | 0.027 | .009 ** | 0.932 | ||||
Receptive vocabulary acquisition | — | −0.039 | 0.011 | .000 *** | 0.962 | −0.018 | 0.013 | .174 | 0.982 | ||||
— | Object’s first location | ||||||||||||
Age | −0.068 | 0.029 | .021 * | 0.935 | — | −0.056 | 0.036 | .125 | 0.946 | ||||
Receptive vocabulary acquisition | — | −0.028 | 0.014 | .054 | 0.973 | −0.010 | 0.018 | .574 | 0.990 | ||||
— | Wrong | ||||||||||||
Age | −0.172 | 0.123 | .163 | 0.842 | — | −0.047 | 0.123 | .703 | 0.954 | ||||
Receptive vocabulary acquisition | — | −0.273 | 0.358 | .445 | 0.761 | −0.257 | 0.356 | .471 | 0.774 | ||||
— | Model General Fit | ||||||||||||
χ2 (df) | χ2 (3) = 22.344 | χ2 (3) = 21.529 | χ2 (6) = 29.170 | ||||||||||
p-value | .000 *** | .000 *** | .000 *** |
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2025 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Süngü, M.; Alıcı, T. Discrimination of False Response from Object Reality in False Belief Test in Preschool Children. J. Intell. 2025, 13, 124. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence13100124
Süngü M, Alıcı T. Discrimination of False Response from Object Reality in False Belief Test in Preschool Children. Journal of Intelligence. 2025; 13(10):124. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence13100124
Chicago/Turabian StyleSüngü, Melis, and Tevfik Alıcı. 2025. "Discrimination of False Response from Object Reality in False Belief Test in Preschool Children" Journal of Intelligence 13, no. 10: 124. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence13100124
APA StyleSüngü, M., & Alıcı, T. (2025). Discrimination of False Response from Object Reality in False Belief Test in Preschool Children. Journal of Intelligence, 13(10), 124. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence13100124