Next Article in Journal
Probabilistic Representation Differences between Decisions from Description and Decisions from Experience
Previous Article in Journal
Testing the Deliberate Practice Theory: Does Practice Reduce the Heritability of Musical Expertise?
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Evidence for the Beneficial Effect of Reward on Working Memory: A Meta-Analytic Study

Department of Psychology, Sun Yat-sen University, #132 Waihuan Dong Road, Panyu District, Guangzhou 510006, China
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
These authors contributed equally to this work.
J. Intell. 2024, 12(9), 88; https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence12090088
Submission received: 28 April 2024 / Revised: 1 September 2024 / Accepted: 5 September 2024 / Published: 11 September 2024

Abstract

:
Rewards act as external motivators and can improve performance in various cognitive tasks. However, previous research demonstrated mixed findings regarding the effect of reward on working memory (WM) performance, and the question of whether reward enhances WM performance is arguable. It remains unclear how the effect of reward on WM can be influenced by various factors, such as types of reward and experimental paradigms. In this meta-analytic study, we systematically investigated the effect of reward on WM by analyzing data from 51 eligible studies involving a total of 1767 participants. Our results showed that reward robustly enhanced WM performance, with non-monetary rewards inducing more benefits than monetary rewards. This may be because, while both types of reward could induce extrinsic motivation, non-monetary rewards enhanced intrinsic motivation while monetary rewards reduced it. Notably, all three reward methods—reward binding, reward expectation, and subliminal reward—effectively improved WM performance, with the reward binding paradigm exhibiting the greatest effects. This finding suggests that the reward effect can be attributed to both increasing the total amount of WM resources and improving the flexibility of resource reallocation. Moreover, the type of WM, the experimental paradigms, and the outcome measures are three moderators that should be jointly considered when assessing the reward effects on WM. Overall, this meta-analytic study provides solid evidence that reward improves WM performance and reveals possible mechanisms underlying these improvements.

1. Introduction

Working memory (WM) is a short-term, flexible storage system for manipulating information. It plays an essential role in human adaptive behavior and has been a central focus in cognitive psychology research. Extensive studies have shown that there is a fixed limit on the amount of information that can be stored (Bays and Husain 2008; Miller 1956; Schneegans and Bays 2016; van den Berg et al. 2012). However, it is worth noting that the WM capacity is not rigid; it varies among individuals and can be altered by various factors, such as attention (Fang et al. 2023; Kotyusov et al. 2023; Li et al. 2021; Qian et al. 2020), familiarity (Brady et al. 2016), motivation (Krawczyk et al. 2007; Kawasaki and Yamaguchi 2013), etc.
Recent studies have shown that motivation can boost performance across various cognitive domains, including motor control and WM (Codol et al. 2020; Manohar et al. 2019). Reward, as a prevalent source of motivation, has been shown to be effective in this regard. Reward refers to a positive or desirable outcome that follows a behavior, which can be a tangible object, such as food or money, or an intangible experience, such as praise or social approval (Berridge and Kringelbach 2015). Recently, research investigating the influence of reward on WM has gained increasing attention. A wealth of studies has found that reward improves WM performance (Gilbert and Fiez 2004; Rowe et al. 2008; Beck et al. 2010; Marquand et al. 2011; Gaillard et al. 2019; Sandry and Ricker 2020). There are two common ways of manipulating reward. In some studies, participants were required to memorize multiple targets within a single trial, with differential reward assignments. For instance, participants were informed of target values prior to stimuli presentation, e.g., a target on the upper-left quadrant of the screen had a value of five points while others were worth one point (Zheng et al. 2022), resulting in better memory recall for targets with higher rewards. In other studies, a higher reward was assigned to certain trials, e.g., participants were informed at the onset of a trial that correct recognition on this trial could gain ‘100’ or ‘30’ reward points (Sanada et al. 2013). On trials with higher rewards, task performance could be better enhanced compared to low- or non-reward trials.
Researchers have proposed two potential explanations to account for how reward enhances WM performance: increasing the attentional priority for processing or augmenting the total amount of resources for processing. On the one hand, for studies with varying reward levels within the same trial, rewards may function as motivational incentives, guiding the deployment of cognitive resources to effectively orient attention and prioritize the processing of task-relevant information (Engelmann et al. 2009; Pessoa 2009; Pessoa and Engelmann 2010; Watanabe 2007). Allen and Ueno (2018) demonstrated that during the encoding phase, stimuli with higher rewards were likely to lead to higher resource concentration, and the degree of attention deployment could be controlled by the value of the reward. On the other hand, in studies manipulating rewards across different trials, rewards may enhance the total amount of resources available for WM in the rewarded trials. The resource rational theory of WM (van den Berg and Ma 2018) challenges the conventional view of a fixed WM capacity. Their theory posits that neural coding is costly (Lennie 2003; Sterling 2017), prompting the brain to trade the behavioral benefits of high memory precision against the cost of the memory resources invested in stimulus encoding. Consequently, it is the cost of reducing WM errors that ultimately constrains WM performance, suggesting that rewards can offset this cost and thus improve WM performance. In previous studies that employed rewards, participants were often strongly motivated to invest more cognitive effort and fully deploy resources to offset the cost of reducing memory errors, thereby improving the performance of WM. Indeed, several studies have shown that individuals can improve their WM performance when incentivized (Gilbert and Fiez 2004; Kawasaki and Yamaguchi 2013; Sanada et al. 2013). However, these studies either did not find or did not report any associated changes in reaction times, making it hard to tell whether the observed improvements represent genuine improvements or simply reflect a trade-off with processing speed.
Although the notion that rewards can improve WM performance is commonly held, there remains a body of research showing contradictory findings. Some studies failed to observe a significant effect of reward on WM accuracy (Krawczyk et al. 2007; Hager et al. 2015; Di Rosa et al. 2019). Meanwhile, others found that the influence of reward expectation on WM diminished when the tasks were relatively simple and the WM load was relatively low (Fairclough et al. 2018; Gaillard et al. 2019). The beneficial effects of reward were not consistently reported, with some studies indicating an effect for participants with better WM capacity, while others did not (Thurm et al. 2018; Manga et al. 2020).
To our knowledge, no previous study has systematically and quantitatively synthesized the empirical studies that have investigated whether and how reward can improve performance in WM. Therefore, in the present meta-analysis, we systematically and quantitatively summarized the results of previous studies and examined the nature and extent of reward on WM performance given different influencing factors. In particular, we aimed to address two questions: (a) Whether a reward is effective in improving WM overall and (b) What kind of influencing factors can modulate the effect of reward on WM.
The investigation of the impact of rewards on WM performance holds important theoretical and practical significance. On the one hand, rewards may increase the motivational salience of the task, which might make the task appear more challenging or engaging, thereby potentially influencing task performance. On the other hand, rewards may bias the allocation of cognitive resources and attention control, temporarily affecting one’s WM ability towards certain stimuli (e.g., enhanced ability for the rewarded stimuli and maybe reduced ability for the unrewarded ones), thus influencing one’s WM performance. From a theoretical perspective, it offers insight into the mechanisms underlying the interaction between motivation and cognitive functions, contributing to resolving the ongoing debate regarding the nature of the relationship between the two. Understanding how rewards influence WM performance may inform evidence-based decision-making across various domains, which may have practical implications on work efficiency enhancement and self-fulfillment.

2. Possible Moderators of Reward Effects on WM

2.1. The Type of Reward

External rewards can be categorized into two types: monetary rewards and non-monetary rewards. Monetary rewards, such as pay increases and bonuses, usually have substantial cash values (Martin and Harder 1994). For example, Sanada et al. (2013) instructed participants to detect changes between two sequentially presented stimuli and they were told that reward points could be earned if their answers were correct, and the final monetary reward was calculated based on the total points acquired. Non-monetary rewards usually take the form of positive feedback in experimental instructions provided by experimenters, which often serve to satisfy socioemotional needs (Martin and Harder 1994). It has been found in a belief updating task that, compared to non-monetary rewards, monetary rewards resulted in a faster learning rate (i.e., the speed of belief updating) and greater changes in neural indicators (e.g., feedback-related negativity and late positive potential; Bennett et al. 2019). In addition, compared to non-monetary rewards, monetary rewards have been found to activate the thalamus more strongly but to activate the amygdala weakly (Rademacher et al. 2010). Given these findings, the two types of rewards may be processed in divergent ways, and it is possible that they have different impacts on WM. For instance, Zheng et al. (2022) used a memory recall task and observed better performance in the monetary reward condition than in the non-monetary reward condition. Therefore, in this meta-analysis, the effects of the two types of rewards on WM were systematically summarized and compared.

2.2. Reward Methods

There are three commonly used reward methods to explore the impact of rewards on WM performance. The reward binding method is an approach that involves associating particular stimuli features with rewards. For instance, when memorizing a variety of items, only specific attributes of these items (e.g., certain colors among various colors) are linked with rewards (Morey et al. 2011). As a result, participants tended to allocate more WM resources to these targets that presumably had a high priority, and the recall accuracy for these specific stimuli can be improved (Hitch et al. 2018; Klink et al. 2017).
The reward expectation method is a technique that involves informing participants about the existence or magnitude of the reward at the beginning of a trial (Manga et al. 2020), and the rewards are indiscriminately assigned to every stimulus within the trial. The memory resources are not priorly oriented to particular targets; instead, rewards may act as catalysts, bolstering mental efforts within the trial (van den Berg and Ma 2018; Capa and Bouquet 2018). For example, mnemonic strategies and other higher-order cognitive processes may be selectively adopted based on task demands.
The subliminal reward method is similar to the reward expectation method, except for a key difference: cues about the existence or magnitude of the reward are presented subliminally at the start of the trial. In other words, while both methods involve pairing rewards with all stimuli on one trial, unlike the reward expectation method, the distinguishing feature of the subliminal reward method is that participants are unaware of the reward cues (Xu et al. 2022). As a result, any noticeable improvement in working memory resulting from this method is likely driven solely by unconscious cognitive processes.

2.3. The Type of WM

It has long been suggested that visual and verbal stores are two separate, limited-capacity subsystems of the WM model (Baddeley and Hitch 1974; Luck and Vogel 1997; Smith et al. 1996). Verbal WM utilizes subvocal rehearsal to maintain verbal material and is typically assessed using the N-back paradigm (Belayachi et al. 2015; Hager et al. 2015), while visual WM maintains visual material through visualization and is usually evaluated using the change detection paradigm or recall/estimation tasks (Allen and Ueno 2018; Morey et al. 2011). Visual and verbal WM differ in several ways. For example, the capacity of verbal WM (Miller 1956) is commonly considered to be larger than that of visual WM (Luck and Vogel 1997; Cowan 2001). Using a dual-task paradigm, verbal and visual-spatial WM tasks could be performed largely in parallel and the two could be performed simultaneously with few interferences (Baddeley and Hitch 1974). Moreover, there is no significant correlation between performance on visual and verbal WM tasks at the individual level (Shaw et al. 2006). Neurologically, there are distinct cerebro-cerebellar activation patterns in verbal and visual WM. It has been shown that verbal WM activates a cerebrocerebellar circuitry that comprises left-lateralized language-related brain regions, including the inferior frontal and posterior parietal areas and right-lateralized superior and inferior cerebella areas. In contrast, visual WM activates a distributed network of bilateral inferior frontal and inferior temporal areas, as well as bilateral superior and inferior cerebellar areas (Ng et al. 2016). Pessoa (2009) suggested that reward mechanisms can modulate cognitive processes differently depending on the neural substrates involved. Given that visual and verbal WM rely on distinct neural substrates, it is plausible that rewards might influence these systems differently. In summary, considering the differences in capacity, function, and neural substrates between visual and verbal WM, we speculate that rewards may have differential effects on these two types of WM.

2.4. Paradigm of WM

Different experimental paradigms serve to investigate various aspects of WM. Two frequently used paradigms for assessing memory performance are recall and recognition tasks. In both tasks, memory items are presented first and participants’ memories are tested after a delay. The main difference lies in the way their memory is being tested. In a recognition task, participants are typically presented with either the item previously shown (target) or another new item not shown before (foil) during the test stage. Participants are then required to identify the target by judging whether the test item is old (previously presented) or new (e.g., Qian et al. 2019, 2020). In a recall task, participants are typically asked to reproduce the target without the target or foils being presented during the test stage (e.g., Zhang et al. 2021).
There can be several variants in the experimental paradigm used in both tasks. For example, retrieval cues can be applied during memory maintenance in both recall and recognition tasks (Fang et al. 2023; Li et al. 2021). For recall tasks, a free recall task indicates that participants are required to recall as many items in memory lists as possible in the absence of any external cues, while in a cued recall task, they are provided with retro-cues to aid memory retrieval. However, in both types of recall tasks, one needs to rely on internal retrieval cues to generate and reproduce the specific value in the tested dimension of the target item. As a result, recall tasks may demand strategic operations, as participants must develop internal strategies to guide the memory search (Stebbins et al. 1999). In comparison, recognition tasks require minimal strategic operations, as participants only need to identify the target among foils (Unsworth and Engle 2007).
The change detection task, which can be considered a variant of the recognition task in the domain of visual WM, was developed by Luck and Vogel in 1997. In this task, participants are presented with a memory display, followed by a test stimulus that may be identical or different. The participant’s task is to judge whether the test display matches the memory display. The estimate of memory capacity K can be calculated by hits and false alarm rates in the task (Pashler 1988; Unsworth et al. 2014), and an individual’s memory capacity K is often correlated with one’s change detection accuracy given a fixed set size of stimuli (see Section 2.5).
Another commonly used task in WM studies is the n-back task (Chen et al. 2008). In the n-back task, stimuli are sequentially presented and participants are asked to report whether the item in the current display matches the item presented ‘n’-display back. Typically, ‘n’ varies across trials to assess the effect of different memory demands upon behavioral performance and physiological correlates (Redick and Lindsey 2013).
In addition, the operation-word-span task (OSPAN) is also an experimental paradigm commonly used to measure WM capacity, in which participants need to carry out a series of simple mathematical operations while they attempt to remember a list of unrelated words (Turner and Engle 1989). By varying the length of the word list, WM capacity for verbal words can be determined.
The delayed estimation task is frequently used for examining the fidelity of visual representations in WM (Bays et al. 2009; Wilken and Ma 2004). In this task, a set of memory items is presented (e.g., color or orientation), and participants must reproduce the target feature value after a brief delay (Ricker et al. 2023). The estimation error is often used as the outcome measure, which is a continuous variable. Various theoretical frameworks have been proposed to model the estimation error, such as the standard mixture model (Zhang and Luck 2008), the joint-representation model (Bae et al. 2015), etc. (Bays et al. 2009; Zhang and Qian 2022), in an attempt to clarify the underlying mechanisms of WM representation.
Considering the distinctions and interconnections among these paradigms, we hypothesize that they may exert a moderating effect on the impact of rewards on WM performance.

2.5. Outcome Measures

Typical measures of WM performance include recall error, accuracy, capacity, and reaction time. Depending on different experimental paradigms, different types of measures are preferably used. Error is usually used in memory recall studies that require participants to reproduce the feature value of the target (e.g., orientation of a bar, hue of a color, etc.). It is conventionally calculated as the absolute value of the difference between the actual feature value of the target and the reproduced one. Accuracy is usually used in change detection paradigms that require participants to detect whether there has been a change between the memory display and the test display and is defined as the proportion of correct responses. Capacity is often reported in addition to accuracy, and its calculation is usually based on hit rates and false alarm rates, according to Pashler (1988) and Cowan (2001). Reaction time refers to the amount of time it takes for a person to respond to a specific stimulus, typically measured from the onset of the stimulus to the onset of the response. Generally, smaller errors, higher accuracy rates, higher memory capacity, and a shorter reaction time indicate better WM performance.
Bays and Husain (2008) suggested that memory capacity and accuracy are indications of the maximum amount of information or number of items stored in WM, whereas recall error indicates the fidelity and precision of memory representation; therefore, the two measures reflect two distinct aspects of WM ability. However, the above four measures are not completely independent. Since both accuracy and capacity can be calculated by hit rates and false alarm rates, these two share some common properties and often yield similar result patterns (Li et al. 2018; Qian et al. 2019).
In addition, rewards may have different effects on accuracy/error and reaction time. For example, providing a reward can lead to a reduction in errors, but it may also induce participants to adopt a more cautious approach, leading to longer response times (Grogan et al. 2022). These effects are often known as the cost–benefit trade-off (Shadmehr et al. 2010) and the speed–accuracy trade-off (Fitts 1954). Previous studies have shown that the two kinds of trade-offs change as the value of the reward varies, suggesting a common underlying mechanism in the brain that maximizes the expected utility resulting from the potential reward and the cost (Peternel et al. 2017).

2.6. Participant Group

Rewards can enhance WM performance by increasing attention and memory priority or offsetting energetic costs. However, it is important to note that the impact of rewards can vary across different populations. Studies have shown that in the case of elderly individuals (Rhodes and Katz 2017) and individuals with schizophrenia (Barch 2005), the positive effect of rewards diminishes. This divergence in outcomes is likely due to the fact that, in comparison to young adults, older individuals typically exhibit less flexible WM ability and lower memory capacity, making it more challenging for them to benefit from cognitive training. On the other hand, individuals with schizophrenia often show reduced motivation, which can be attributed to abnormalities in their processing of reward. Compared to healthy adults, brain regions relevant to reward processing in schizophrenia tend to exhibit weaker activation in response to monetary reward (Juckel et al. 2006). Based on these findings, we speculate that the participant group may moderate the effect of reward on WM. Compared to healthy young adults, rewards are likely to have a less pronounced effect on the WM performance of both elderly individuals and those with schizophrenia.

3. Methods

3.1. Literature Search

To locate relevant articles, we first conducted online searches of the Web of Science database, using the keywords WM & reward/rewards, WM & reward expectation, and WM reward & motivation, as well as the Chinese National Knowledge Infrastructure (CNKI), using the same keywords in Chinese. Secondly, we read the titles and abstracts to determine the relevance of each article. These preliminary searches yielded a database of 89 articles covering the years 1981 to 2022.

3.2. Criteria for Selection of Studies

The studies were included if they met the following criteria: (1) the participants were human; (2) the study object was WM; (3) the study involved manipulation of rewards or reward expectations; (4) the study provided behavioral data of WM performance, such as accuracy and reaction time; (5) the study used quantifiable variables and was not purely descriptive or anecdotal in nature (such as a case study); and (6) the study should provide the mean, standard deviation (s.d.), sample size, test statistics, or other data that can be calculated for effect size.
Among the 89 articles identified through the literature search, 39 met all of the specified criteria and were included in the meta-analysis. The remaining 50 articles were excluded for various reasons, including 14 that involved animal participants, 10 that did not manipulate rewards or reward expectations, 21 where the study did not investigate WM (e.g., they investigated long-term memory or source memory), and 5 due to lack of sufficient information to estimate effect sizes, such as missing standard deviations or incomplete statistical outcomes.

3.3. Coding Procedures

Three of the authors coded the following information from the included studies: (1) basic information about the research, including author(s), journal (or dissertation), and publication year; (2) demographic information, including the sample size, mean age, sex, and types of participants; (3) study characteristics, including types of reward, reward level, paradigms, types of WM, outcome measures, and experimental design; and (4) statistical results used for estimating effect sizes, including the mean and s.d. for the experimental conditions, the t-test, F test, or other statistics, and p-values. To ensure coding reliability, the three coders cross-checked all the coding sheets for each study.

3.4. Moderator Variables

Due to the divergence in the experimental design and the stimuli used in the various studies, moderator analysis was necessary to examine the possible factors influencing the effect of reward on WM performance. Six moderators were taken into consideration: (1) type of reward: rewards were coded as two main types: monetary rewards and non-monetary rewards; (2) reward methods: the reward methods were classified into three categories: reward binding, reward expectation, and subliminal reward; (3) type of WM: visual WM and verbal WM; (4) paradigm of WM: five paradigms were coded: n-back, change detection, recognition, recall, and delay estimation. It is important to note that there was one study using the OSPAN paradigm and two studies whose paradigms did not fit into either of these categories, so we categorized them as ‘others’; (5) outcome measures: three outcome measures were coded: error, reaction time (RT), and accuracy (ACC). It is worth noting that one study did not report accuracy and only reported capacity. Since the calculation of capacity was based on accuracy, we combined the effect size of capacity in this study into the category of accuracy. To ensure the independence of effect sizes, when a study contributed more than one effect size using different outcome measures, we assigned the effect size of that study to the group with a smaller total number of studies; and (6) participant group: although participants included in most of the included studies were healthy young adults, there were a small number of elderly, schizophrenic, and ADHD participants. Due to the relatively small number of studies involving these participants and to avoid a potential low statistical power when conducting moderator analysis, participants were categorized into two groups: healthy young adults and others.

3.5. Data Analysis

The standardized mean difference, Cohen’s d, was used as the measure of effect size. Cohen (1988) proposed that a value of d of 0.2, 0.5, or 0.8 can be considered as a small, moderate, or large effect size, respectively. Since we hypothesized that rewards would enhance WM performance, effect sizes were given positive signs when they were consistent with this expectation, and negative signs when they were not. When means and s.d. were available, effect sizes were calculated using the formula given by Cohen. When an inferential statistic (typically the t-test, p, r, or F) was available, the formula provided by Lipsey and Wilson (2001) was used. When a study provided only a significance level and did not provide data that could be used to calculate the effect size, the minimum d value that would enable the study to reach that significance level was calculated as the effect size for the study. When a study’s p-value was not significant but no specific p-value was provided, the effect size was coded as 0 (Kalma 1991).
To ensure the independence of effect sizes, each independent sample was coded only once to produce one effect size. When a study produced multiple effect sizes, their combined effect sizes were calculated using weighted averages.
The Stouffer method of adding standard normal deviation zs was used to estimate significance levels for combined studies (Mosteller and Bush 1954). In addition to computing combined effect sizes and probability levels, a fail-safe N was also computed for each analysis (Rosenthal 1979). The fail-safe N is an estimate of the number of unpublished, non-significant studies that would have to exist for the obtained probability value to be rendered non-significant. Publication bias was also assessed using trim-and-fill analysis.
Comprehensive Meta Analysis Version 3.3.070 (CMA, 3.0) was used to conduct the data analysis. A mixed-effects model implemented in CMA 3.0 was used to analyze moderating effects.

4. Results

4.1. The Overall Effect

This meta-analysis was conducted on 39 articles. Effect sizes were obtained from 55 studies with independent samples, with a total of 1802 participants. Each sample size ranged from eight to 146. The characteristics of these studies are summarized in Table 1.
The results of this analysis revealed a mean estimated Cohen’s d of 1.03 with 95% CI = [0.86–1.20], which could be considered a fairly large effect. The Stouffer method of significance testing showed that reward significantly improved WM performance, p < 0.001 (Figure 1).

4.2. Publication Bias

We used a trim-and-fill analysis to assess the publication bias. As the funnel plot (Figure 2A) has shown, the majority of effect sizes were well-balanced around the mean effect size, except for four studies that had large effects (d > 2.8). We suspected that the exceedingly large effect size might result from individual differences and large sampling errors. Therefore, we performed the Kolmogorov–Smirnov normality test and stem-leaf plot analysis, which showed that the normality hypothesis did not hold (p = 0.008) and that there were four extremes of Cohen’s d. After removing the extreme values, the Shapiro–Wilk test (N > 50) was performed again, and the results showed adherence to the assumption of normality (p = 0.200). As shown in the updated funnel plot (Figure 2B), the remaining studies were evenly distributed, with appropriate sample sizes and relatively good quality. The remaining 51 studies were then used as the total sample for moderator analyses, with a total of 1711 participants.
Results revealed a mean estimated Cohen’s d of 0.93 with 95% CI = [0.77–1.08], which was a fairly large effect. Null hypothesis significance testing showed that reward significantly improved WM performance, p < 0.001 (Table 2). The test of heterogeneity was significant, Q(50) = 314.88, p < 0.001, suggesting that the effect sizes varied significantly across different studies. Examining the potential moderators might therefore clarify the factors that account for this variability. A file drawer analysis or fail-safe N indicated that 9362 missing or non-published studies that averaged null findings would have to exist to cancel this effect. However, the existence of so many studies was unlikely given the number of the included studies in the current meta-analysis and therefore the current result can be considered to be robust.

4.3. Moderator Analyses

The results of moderator analyses are presented in Table 2 and Figure 3. Overall, we found that the type of reward, reward methods, the type of WM, paradigms of WM, outcome measures, and participant group influenced the effect of reward on WM.

4.3.1. The Type of Reward

Thirty-nine studies used monetary rewards and the other ten studies used non-monetary ones. The homogeneity test showed that the between-subgroup variance was significant (Q(1) = 19.10, p < 0.001). Specifically, if the reward was monetary, the effect size was moderate (d = 0.78, p < 0.001); if not, the effect size was large (d = 1.63, p < 0.001). File-drawer analysis showed that fail-safe N was 4440 for the monetary reward group and 713 for the non-monetary reward group.

4.3.2. Reward Methods

Twenty-three studies employed the reward binding method, twenty-eight utilized the reward expectation method, and only four applied the subliminal reward method. The homogeneity test was marginally significant (Q(2) = 4.27, p = 0.10), suggesting a trend that variance differed significantly across different subgroups.
Specifically, when the reward binding method was used, the effect size was large (d = 1.10, p < 0.001). In contrast, for both reward expectation (d = 0.82, p < 0.001) and subliminal reward (d = 0.72, p = 0.002) methods, the effect size was moderate. In other words, WM performance showed the greatest improvement when using the reward binding paradigm, followed by the reward expectation paradigm, and the least pronounced effect was found for the subliminal reward paradigm. File-drawer analysis showed that fail-safe N was 2581 for the reward binding group, 2307 for the reward expectation group, and 59 for the subliminal reward group.

4.3.3. Type of WM

Thirty-seven studies focused on measuring visual WM, while fourteen studies assessed verbal WM. The homogeneity test showed that the between-subgroup variance was significant (Q(1) = 6.80, p = 0.009). For visual WM, the effect size was large (d = 1.05, p < 0.001); whereas for verbal WM, the effect size was moderate (d = 0.62, p < 0.001). File-drawer analysis showed that fail-safe N was 5712 for the visual WM group and 433 for the verbal WM group.

4.3.4. Paradigm of WM

Seven studies employed n-back tasks, six used change detection tasks, thirteen used recognition tasks, ten utilized recall tasks, eight used delay estimation tasks, and three other studies were categorized as other paradigms. The homogeneity test showed that the between-subgroup variance was marginally significant (Q(5) = 10.42, p = 0.064). For studies that used n-back tasks, the effect size was small (d = 0.43, p = 0.025); whereas for studies that employed change detection (d = 0.94, p < 0.001), recognition (d = 0.79, p < 0.001), recall (d = 1.23, p < 0.001), and delay estimation (d = 0.78, p < 0.001), the effect size was large. For studies that used other paradigms, the effect size was larger (d = 1.24, p = 0.002). File-drawer analysis showed that fail-safe N was 46 for the n-back task group, 118 for the change detection task group, 477 for the recognition task group, 508 for the recall task group, 240 for the delay estimation task group, and 52 for the other task group.

4.3.5. Outcome Measures

Eight studies utilized estimation error as the outcome, 42 studies employed accuracy as the outcome, and 20 studies adopted reaction time as the outcome. The homogeneity test was not significant (Q(2) = 3.10, p = 0.212), suggesting that variance differed significantly across different subgroups.
Specifically, if accuracy was used as the outcome, the effect size was large (d = 0.95, p < 0.001); if error (d = 0.75, p < 0.001) or reaction time (d = 0.69, p < 0.001) was used, the effect size was moderate. The result showed that all three outcomes could effectively capture the beneficial effect of reward on WM. File-drawer analysis showed that fail-safe N was 250 for the error group, 6265 for the accuracy group, and 806 for the response time group.

4.3.6. Participant Group

Forty-nine studies involved healthy young adults as participants while six studies did not. The homogeneity test showed that the between-subgroup variance was significant (Q(1) = 11.40, p = 0.001). For studies with healthy young adults, the effect size was large (d = 0.95, p < 0.001); otherwise, the effect size was small but significant (d = 0.33, p = 0.047). File-drawer analysis showed that fail-safe N was 8503 for the healthy adults and 23 for the others.

4.3.7. Secondary Subgroup Analysis

Because the heterogeneity (Q) remained high in several subgroups even after moderator analyses, we performed secondary subgroup analyses to further reduce within-group variances and to account for variances that may influence the reward effects. To control for Type-I errors that potentially resulted from small sample sizes (insufficient number of studies), we chose four subgroups that had large Qs and sufficient numbers of studies to perform further subgroup division (Table 3).
First, we performed a secondary subgroup analysis for the monetary reward subgroup (Q = 285.13) based on outcome measures. Out of 42 studies that employed monetary rewards, eight studies utilized error as the outcome, 19 studies used accuracy, and 15 studies used reaction time. The homogeneity test was marginally significant (Q(2) = 5.66, p = 0.06), suggesting a trend that variance differed significantly across different subgroups. Specifically, if the outcome measure was accuracy (d = 0.89, p < 0.001) or error (d = 0.86, p < 0.001), the effect size was large; if reaction time (d = 0.48, p < 0.001) was used, the effect size was moderate. File-drawer analysis showed that fail-safe N was 247 for the error group, 1230 for the accuracy group, and 227 for the response time group.
Second, a secondary subgroup analysis was performed for the visual WM subgroup (Q = 296.60) based on outcome measures. Out of 38 studies that focused on visual WM, seven studies utilized error as the outcome, 21 studies employed accuracy, and 10 studies used reaction time. The homogeneity test was marginally significant (Q(2) = 6.47, p = 0.039). If the outcome measure was reaction time (d = 0.75, p < 0.001) or error (d = 0.79, p < 0.001), the effect size was large; if accuracy (d = 1.24, p < 0.001) was used, the effect size was much larger. File-drawer analysis showed that fail-safe N was 252 for the error group, 2006 for the correct/capacity group, and 170 for the response time group.
Third, we performed two separate secondary subgroup analyses for the reward binding (Q = 55.43) and reward expectation (Q = 145.74) subgroups based on the type of reward. Out of 23 studies that employed the reward binding method, 15 used monetary rewards and the other eight studies used non-monetary ones. The homogeneity test showed that the between-subgroup variance was significant (Q(1) = 5.59, p = 0.018). Specifically, the effect size was large (d = 1.03, p < 0.001) if the reward was monetary, and it was even larger (d = 1.53, p < 0.001) if the reward was non-monetary. File-drawer analysis showed that fail-safe N was 1155 for the monetary reward group and 400 for the non-monetary reward group.
On the other hand, out of 22 studies that employed the reward expectation method, 20 used monetary rewards and only two studies used non-monetary ones. The homogeneity test showed that the between-subgroup variance was significant (Q(1) = 38.06, p < 0.001). Interestingly, the effect size was small (d = 0.52, p < 0.001) if the reward was monetary, and it was fairly large (d = 1.99, p < 0.001) if the reward was non-monetary. File-drawer analysis showed that fail-safe N was 512 for the monetary reward group.

5. Discussion

Pulling from the results of 51 studies with a total of 1711 participants, we found robust evidence that reward enhances WM performance. This effect varied based on the type of reward or reward method employed, and it was also moderated by factors such as the type of WM, paradigms of WM, various outcome measures, and participant groups.
The type of reward. Interestingly, we found that the effect of monetary reward on WM is weaker than non-monetary reward. Fail-safe N reveals robustness in these results. This finding contradicts the conclusion of some studies, which suggest that monetary rewards lead to faster learning and thus a greater facilitation effect (e.g., Bennett et al. 2019). We propose several possible reasons for these results. Firstly, monetary rewards may induce excessive motivation to achieve optimal performance. As Botvinick and Braver (2015) have suggested, strong incentives can sometimes induce decrements in performance, a phenomenon referred to as “choking under pressure”. Several studies have demonstrated that high monetary rewards can result in less than optimal performance across various tasks, including motor learning, intelligence tests, and long-term memory (Kuhbandner et al. 2016; Cheng et al. 2020). It has also been found that individuals pursuing monetary rewards often experience heightened feelings of fear (Castro et al. 2021), whereas non-monetary social rewards have been associated with increased positive emotions (Lee et al. 2011). Neuroimaging studies have shown stronger thalamus activation to monetary rewards than non-monetary social rewards, where the thalamus is suggested to modulate goal-directed behavior through ranking incentive values (Rademacher et al. 2010). In other words, activation in the thalamus may reflect subjective reward value, and monetary rewards can be considered to have a greater value than social rewards. Therefore, we suspect that for WM specifically, the motivation brought by a non-monetary reward is optimal for performance while that brought by a monetary reward might be excessive.
Secondly, it is possible that both types of reward could induce extrinsic motivation, while the two may have different impacts on participants’ intrinsic motivation. Extrinsic motivation is often considered as a catalyst for action that is driven by external rewards, such as money, grades, etc., and intrinsic motivation is the incentive to complete a task to satisfy one’s psychological needs, such as competence, enjoyment, etc. Deci (1971) observed that using money as an external reward decreased intrinsic motivation (i.e., monetary reward reduced intrinsic motivation), whereas verbal reinforcement and positive feedback tended to enhance it (i.e., non-monetary reward enhanced intrinsic motivation). The introduction of money into an activity often prompts individuals to reassess their motivations, shifting from intrinsic to extrinsic, thereby diminishing their inherent drive (Lepper and Greene 1976). Conversely, social approval, which mostly triggers intrinsic motivation, does not typically have the same effect. Previous studies have shown that intrinsic motivation is better at promoting individual performance in self-regulated learning and employee outcomes (Young 2005; Moos 2010; Kuvaas et al. 2017). It is possible that in the field of WM, intrinsic motivation also plays a more pivotal role compared to extrinsic motivation. Consequently, the use of monetary rewards resulted in less improvement when compared to non-monetary rewards.
One might speculate whether the difference in the effects between monetary and non-monetary rewards was due to unequal sample sizes, since there were 39 studies using monetary rewards and only 10 studies using non-monetary rewards. To test this possibility, we performed a Bootstrap analysis for both monetary and non-monetary studies and randomly sampled the effect sizes from 10 studies from each type of study with 10,000 resamples. The Bootstrapped mean effect size was 0.98 for the monetary studies and 1.80 for the non-monetary ones. In other words, the effect size of the non-monetary studies was significantly larger than that of the monetary studies, ruling out the confounding of unequal sample sizes. However, one confounding factor remains unresolved. Most of the studies utilizing non-monetary rewards employed the reward binding method, which was shown to have the largest effect among the three reward methods examined in this meta-analysis. To further clarify the possible confounds, secondary subgroup analyses were conducted and the results are discussed in the following section.
Reward methods. We found a trend that the reward binding method induced a larger effect compared to both reward expectation and subliminal reward methods. This result was consistent with empirical findings showing that studies applying the reward binding method consistently showed a reward enhancement effect on WM (Gong and Li 2014; Klink et al. 2017; Klyszejko et al. 2014; Christie and Schrater 2015; Wallis et al. 2015), while studies using the reward expectation method have yielded mixed results (Gilbert and Fiez 2004; Kawasaki and Yamaguchi 2013; van den Berg et al. 2023; Zhang and Luck 2008). For studies that employed the latter two methods, rewards were assigned to all targets in a single trial, and participants were incentivized to enhance their overall WM performance, which required an increase in the total amount of WM resources to improve performance. Since we observed a significant reward effect in the reward expectation method, this suggests that the total amount of WM resources for completing the task was boosted; therefore, memory performance for each item in a trial can be indiscriminately enhanced and the overall performance can be improved. However, when reward binding was applied, since rewards were only associated with certain targets, memory resources may be prioritized to these stimuli over the others. In other words, not only is the total amount of WM resources increased but the flexible allocation of these resources among items based on the distribution of rewards also leads to enhanced performance for the rewarded target to a greater extent (i.e., more resources are directed to the rewarded target and fewer resources to the unrewarded ones). Because the effect size of the reward binding method is larger than that of the reward expectation method, we speculate that improving the flexibility of resource reallocation contributes to enhancing WM performance. Taken together, our findings suggest that the beneficial effect of reward can be attributed to both increasing the total amount of WM resources and improving the flexibility of resource reallocation, and the effects of these two aspects may be accumulative.
However, we observed no significant difference in the reward effects between the subliminal reward method and the reward expectation method, which partially contradicts the theoretical framework proposed by Bijleveld et al. (2012). According to this model, individuals initially process subliminal reward information in rudimentary brain structures, such as the striatum (Childress et al. 2008), and task performance can be directly boosted by increasing mental efforts without requiring conscious awareness. Conversely, if rewards can be consciously perceived, higher-level cognitive functions are engaged, which allows facilitated processing and enables the development of complex strategic behavioral responses that further enhance performance (Dehaene et al. 1998). Contrary to this framework, our findings suggest that, at least for WM, the conscious adoption of additional strategies may not yield practical benefits for WM performance. Since for these two reward methods, the improvements in WM performance required an increase in the total amount of WM resources, our results indicate that the WM resources available may be primarily determined by neurophysiological constraints (Bays 2015; Lisman and Idiart 1995; Wei et al. 2012), not by conscious strategy adoption. It is worth noting that results for the subliminal reward method may lack reliability, with only four studies included and a fail-safe N equal to 59. Therefore, these findings require further validation.
In addition, secondary subgroup analysis showed that compared to reward binding paradigms, the difference in reward effects between non-monetary and monetary rewards widens under the reward expectation paradigms (see Table 3; the difference in effect size between the non-monetary and monetary reward conditions was 0.40 in the reward binding paradigm and 1.46 in the reward expectation paradigm), and the divergence mainly lies in the monetary condition (d = 1.03 for reward binding and 0.52 for reward expectation). By cross-referencing the other moderators, we think that the type of WM possibly modulates the differential effects observed in the monetary condition between the reward binding and reward expectation methods. Under the monetary condition of reward expectation, 10 out of 22 studies examined verbal WM, which has smaller reward effects than visual WM; and under the monetary condition of reward binding, 16 out of 17 studies examined visual WM, which typically has greater reward effects. This may contribute to the difference in reward effects between the two reward paradigms. Note that the number of studies on non-monetary rewards is relatively small, and the two studies on non-monetary rewards under reward expectation conditions happen to have relatively large effect sizes (1.74 and 2.26); therefore, the current results might not be robust.
The type of WM, paradigms of WM, and outcome measures. Another interesting finding is that improvements in visual WM due to reward are greater than those in verbal WM. The fail-safe N reveals robustness in the observed results. There are two potential interpretations. Firstly, visual WM predominantly relies on cortical processing of visual stimuli, while verbal WM engages verbal information processing that is related to multiple brain regions, including the left occipito-temporal and bilateral intraparietal cortexes (Dehaene and Cohen 2007). Consequently, rewards may have a smaller effect on verbal WM tasks due to the possible involvement and cooperation of multiple brain regions, while their effect on visual WM is more pronounced (Adcock et al. 2006). Secondly, it is due to the influence of WM experimental paradigms. In cases where memory materials are verbal, there are a total of six studies using the n-back paradigm, which has the smallest effect size out of the WM paradigms; whereas for visual WM, there is only one study. Additionally, in cases where memory materials are visual, there are 14 studies utilizing the recall paradigm, which has the largest effect size out of the WM paradigms; whereas for verbal WM, there are only two studies. Hence, the reward effect in visual WM is greater than in verbal WM.
As the experimental paradigms may confound the effect of the type of WM, the type of outcome measure may also modulate the effect since the type of outcome measure is closely contingent on the experimental paradigm used in the study. For visual WM, about half of the studies (21/37) used an outcome measure of accuracy (see secondary subgroup analysis), which has the largest effect size among all three measures examined. Therefore, we suggest that the type of WM, the experimental paradigms, and the outcome measure are three moderators that should be jointly considered when assessing the effect of reward on WM.
Overall, the effects of reward on all outcome measures are significant, with the impact on accuracy being larger than for error and reaction time. On the one hand, the enhancement across all outcome measures suggests that reward may universally improve WM performance regardless of the outcome measure tested. Reward elevates both accuracy and processing speed (reflected by RT), with no apparent speed–accuracy trade-off. On the other hand, the effect on RT is the smallest among these outcome measures, indicating variability among these measures. Notably, in a third of the studies included, reward did not significantly reduce RT. We suspect that the effect of reward on RT may be influenced by the specific experimental instructions provided. If participants were instructed to be both fast and correct/precise, participants may try to be more accurate and respond faster at the same time. Conversely, if they are instructed to prioritize accuracy or precision to receive the reward, they may be more cautious and respond more slowly. In the current study, since most of the studies did not provide detailed descriptions of the instructions applied, it was not examined as a moderator and thus heterogeneity may result from this confound.
Participant group. Consistent with previous studies (Rhodes and Katz 2017; Juckel et al. 2006), we found that special groups such as elderly people and schizophrenia patients experience fewer benefits from rewards. This result indicates that the reward effect may vary across different populations. Individuals in these special groups may lack flexibility in WM, rendering them unable to optimize resource allocation or adjust mnemonic strategies according to different reward conditions. On a neural level, both older adults and individuals with schizophrenia may be affected by dopaminergic abnormalities, resulting in reduced reactivity to reward (Li and Rieckmann 2014; Barch 2005; Nieoullon and Coquerel 2003). However, the number of studies on special groups is limited. There are only six studies with special groups in our meta-analysis and different types of participants (i.e., the elderly and schizophrenia patients) were mixed in one subgroup, which increased within-group heterogeneity, meaning that the results may not be reliable (see more discussion in Section 6).

6. Limitations

There are several constraints of the current analyses. First, the number of available studies is limited. Due to our stringent inclusion and exclusion criteria, fewer studies were eligible. Future studies may consider less stringent criteria, and more diverse moderator variables can be evaluated. Second, in the moderator analyses, there are large error variances for some subgroups that contained only a few eligible studies with a small sample size. Due to potentially low statistical power, we did not perform secondary subgroup analyses for these subgroups. Finally, due to the limitation of the number of available studies, we have combined elderly individuals and schizophrenia patients into a collective category labeled “others” as a comparative subgroup against the “healthy adults” subgroup. This is for practical reasons, and inherent variations may be embedded within the “others” population, thereby rendering the results not universally representative of all subgroups. More attention should be paid to special groups in the future.
Moreover, the method of meta-analysis by pooling data from many studies inevitably leaves out some research questions related to detailed experimental procedures available only in individual studies, for example, how the reward effects may be influenced by the order of items appearing in a memory sequence, e.g., the primacy effect and recency effect. We speculate that rewards can enhance the primacy and recency effects as items at these temporal locations are more sensitive indicators for diagnosis (Melin et al. 2022). In addition, a neurological study has shown that activities in reward-sensitive brain regions, e.g., the fusiform gyrus and orbital-frontal cortex, were largest 4–8 s after receiving a reward cue (May et al. 2004). It is possible that performance for memory items presented during this period could be better improved by rewards. These research questions require further exploration.
It is worth noting that the WM capacity, as reflected in memory performance, may be considered a personal trait. This is because memory capacity can be influenced by genetic factors, exhibiting relatively stable individual differences (Ilkowska and Engle 2010). Conversely, memory capacity can also display state-dependent variations, since it may be influenced by situational factors such as mood, fatigue, and sleep deprivation (Engle and Kane 2004; Schmeichel 2007). Therefore, the reward effects reported in this study might be influenced by such factors.
Finally, although it was not applied in our meta-analysis, modern measurement theories, such as the Rasch psychometric model and entropy-based Construct specification equations (Melin et al. 2022), can provide a more accurate explanation of the relationship between task performance and memory ability to take task difficulty into account. The included studies had response accuracies ranging between 70–90%, indicating that the task difficulty was appropriately set and could effectively measure participants’ WM performance. However, for individual studies concerning special groups, e.g., the most capable or least capable individuals, the modern measurement theory can be a valuable approach for improving the accuracy and validity of measurements.

7. Conclusions

In conclusion, in this meta-analytic study, we systematically investigated the effect of reward on WM performance and the possible moderators that can affect its effect. The results showed that reward enhanced WM performance robustly even when exceptionally large effect sizes were deleted. The type of reward, reward methods, type of WM, paradigm of WM, outcome measures, and participant group moderated the effect of reward on WM performance. This result provides solid evidence that reward improves WM performance and reveals possible mechanisms of the processing underlying these improvements.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, W.W. and J.Q.; data curation, W.W., X.Y. and X.H.; formal analysis, W.W., X.Y. and X.H.; validation, W.W., X.Y. and X.H.; writing—original draft preparation, W.W., X.Y. and X.H.; writing—review and editing, J.Q.; supervision, J.Q.; project administration, J.Q.; funding acquisition, J.Q. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by National Natural Science Foundation of China [32271100] and the Basic and Applied Basic Research Foundation of Guangdong Province [2021A1515010840]. The APC was funded by National Natural Science Foundation of China [32271100] and Graduate Education Innovation Program [2024_02300_B24743].

Institutional Review Board Statement

Not applicable.

Informed Consent Statement

Not applicable.

Data Availability Statement

Data is available upon request.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors have no competing financial interests that might be perceived to influence the results and/or discussion reported in this paper.

References

  1. Adcock, Alison, Arul Thangavel, Susan Whitfield-Gabrieli, Brian Knutson, and John D. E. Gabrieli. 2006. Reward-motivated learning: Mesolimbic activation precedes memory formation. Neuron 50: 507–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Allen, Richard, and Taiji Ueno. 2018. Multiple high-reward items can be prioritized in working memory but with greater vulnerability to interference. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics 80: 1731–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Baddeley, Alan, and Graham Hitch. 1974. Working memory. In Recent Advances in Learning and Motivation; New York: GA Bower, vol. 8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Bae, Gi-Yeul, Maria Olkkonen, Sarah R. Allred, and Jonathan I. Flombaum. 2015. Why some colors appear more memorable than others: A model combining categories and particulars in color working memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 144: 744–63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Barch, Deanna. 2005. The relationships among cognition, motivation, and emotion in schizophrenia: How much and how little we know. Schizophrenia Bulletin 31: 875–81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Bays, Paul. 2015. Spikes not slots: Noise in neural populations limits working memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 19: 431–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Bays, Paul, and Masud Husain. 2008. Supplementary: Dynamic shifts of limited working memory resources in human vision. Science 321: 851–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  8. Bays, Paul, Raquel Catalao, and Masud Husain. 2009. The precision of visual working memory is set by allocation of a shared resource. Journal of Vision 9: 7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Beck, Stefanie, Hannah S. Locke, Adam C. Savine, Koji Jimura, and Todd S. Braver. 2010. Primary and secondary rewards differentially modulate neural activity dynamics during working memory. PLoS ONE 5: e9251. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Belayachi, Sanaâ, Steve Majerus, Guido Gendolla, Eric Salmon, Frédéric Peters, and Martial Van der Linden. 2015. Are the carrot and the stick the two sides of same coin? A neural examination of approach/avoidance motivation during cognitive performance. Behavioural Brain Research 293: 217–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Bennett, Daniel, Karen Sasmita, Ryan T. Maloney, Carsten Murawski, and Stefan Bode. 2019. Monetary feedback modulates performance and electrophysiological indices of belief updating in reward learning. Psychophysiology 56: e13431. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Berridge, Kent, and Morten L. Kringelbach. 2015. Pleasure systems in the brain. Neuron 86: 646–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Bijleveld, Erik, Ruud Custers, and Henk Aarts. 2012. Human Reward Pursuit: From Rudimentary to Higher-Level Functions. Current Directions in Psychological Science 21: 194–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Botvinick, Matthew, and Todd Braver. 2015. Motivation and cognitive control: From behavior to neural mechanism. Annual Review of Psychology 66: 83–113. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Brady, Timothy F., Viola S. Störmer, and George A. Alvarez. 2016. Working memory is not fixed-capacity: More active storage capacity for real-world objects than for simple stimuli. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 113: 7459–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Capa, Rémi L., and Cédric A. Bouquet. 2018. Individual differences in reward sensitivity modulate the distinctive effects of conscious and unconscious rewards on executive performance. Frontiers in Psychology 9: 148. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Castro, Juan F., Gustavo Yamada, Hans Contreras, Freddy Linares, and Herwig Watson. 2021. Tracking the Pressure: The Effect of Monetary Incentives on Effort, Emotions, and Performance. Journal of Neuroscience, Psychology, and Economics 14: 234–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Cheng, Si, Ting Jiang, Jingming Xue, Songxue Wang, Chuansheng Chen, and Mingxia Zhang. 2020. The influence of rewards on incidental memory: More does not mean better. Learning & Memory 27: 462–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Chen, Yung-Nien, Suvobrata Mitra, and Friederike Schlaghecken. 2008. Sub-processes of working memory in the N-back task: An investigation using ERPs. Clinical Neurophysiology 119: 1546–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Childress, Anna Rose, Ronald N. Ehrman, Ze Wang, Yin Li, Nathan Sciortino, Jonathan Hakun, William Jens, Jesse Suh, John Listerud, Kathleen Marquez, and et al. 2008. Prelude to Passion: Limbic Activation by “Unseen” Drug and Sexual Cues. PLoS ONE 3: e1506. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Choi, Jong Moon, Srikanth Padmala, and Luiz Pessoa. 2015. Counteracting effect of threat on reward enhancements during working memory. Cognition and Emotion 29: 1517–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  22. Cho, Youngsun T., Flora Moujaes, Charles H. Schleifer, Martina Starc, Jie Lisa Ji, Nicole Santamauro, Brendan Adkinson, Antonija Kolobaric, Morgan Flynn, John H. Krystal, and et al. 2022. Reward and loss incentives improve spatial working memory by shaping trial-by-trial posterior frontoparietal signals. NeuroImage 254: 119139. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Cho, Youngsun T., Norman H. Lam, Martina Starc, Nicole Santamauro, Aleksandar Savic, Caroline K. Diehl, Charles H. Schleifer, Flora Moujaes, Vinod H. Srihari, Grega Repovs, and et al. 2018. Effects of reward on spatial working memory in schizophrenia. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 127: 695. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. Christie, Thomas, and Paul Schrater. 2015. Cognitive cost as dynamic allocation of energetic resources. Frontiers in Neuroscience 9: 289. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Codol, Olivier, Peter J. Holland, Sanjay G. Manohar, and Joseph M. Galea. 2020. Reward-based improvements in motor control are driven by multiple error-reducing mechanisms. Journal of Neuroscience 40: 3604–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Cohen, Jacob. 1988. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences, rev. ed. Cambridge: Academic Press. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Cohen, Michael S., Larry Y. Cheng, Ken A. Paller, and Paul J. Reber. 2019. Separate memory-enhancing effects of reward and strategic encoding. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 31: 1658–73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Cowan, Nelson. 2001. The magical number 4 in short-term memory: A reconsideration of mental storage capacity. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 24: 87–114. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Deci, Edward. 1971. Effects of externally mediated rewards on intrinsic motivation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 18: 105–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Dehaene, Stanislas, and Laurent Cohen. 2007. Cultural recycling of cortical maps. Neuron 56: 384–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Dehaene, Stanislas, Michel Kerszberg, and Jean-Pierre Changeux. 1998. A Neuronal Model of a Global Workspace in Effortful Cognitive Tasks. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 95: 14529–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Di Rosa, Elisa, Sabrina Brigadoi, Simone Cutini, Vincenza Tarantino, Roberto Dell’Acqua, Daniela Mapelli, Todd S. Braver, and Antonino Vallesi. 2019. Reward motivation and neurostimulation interact to improve working memory performance in healthy older adults: A simultaneous tDCS-fNIRS study. Neuroimage 202: 116062. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Engelmann, Jan B., Eswar Damaraju, Srikanth Padmala, and Luiz Pessoa. 2009. Combined effects of attention and motivation on visual task performance: Transient and sustained motivational effects. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 3: 4. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  34. Engle, Randall W., and Michael J. Kane. 2004. Executive attention, working memory capacity, and a two-factor theory of cognitive control. Psychology of Learning and Motivation 44: 145–200. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Fairclough, Stephen H., Christopher Burns, and Ute Kreplin. 2018. FNIRS activity in the prefrontal cortex and motivational intensity: Impact of working memory load, financial reward, and correlation-based signal improvement. Neurophotonics 5: 035001. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Fang, Wei, Kaiyue Wang, Ke Zhang, and Jiehui Qian. 2023. Spatial attention based on 2-D location and relative depth order modulates visual working memory in a 3-D environment. British Journal of Psychology 114: 112–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  37. Fitts, Paul. 1954. The information capacity of the human motor system in controlling the amplitude of movement. Journal of Experimental Psychology 47: 381. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Gaillard, Claudie, Matthias Guillod, Monique Ernst, Andrea Federspiel, Dominik Schoebi, Romina Evelyn Recabarren, Xinyi Ouyang, Christoph Mueller-Pfeiffer, Antje Horsch, Philipp Homan, and et al. 2020. Striatal reactivity to reward under threat-of-shock and working memory load in adults at increased familial risk for major depression: A preliminary study. NeuroImage: Clinical 26: 102193. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Gaillard, Claudie, Matthias Guillod, Monique Ernst, Salvatore Torrisi, Andrea Federspiel, Dominik Schoebi, Romina E. Recabarren, Xinyi Ouyang, Christoph Mueller-Pfeiffer, Antje Horsch, and et al. 2019. Striatal responsiveness to reward under threat-of-shock and working memory load: A preliminary study. Brain and Behavior 9: e01397. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Gilbert, Alison M., and Julie A. Fiez. 2004. Integrating rewards and cognition in the frontal cortex. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience 4: 540–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Gong, Mengyuan, and Sheng Li. 2014. Learned reward association improves visual working memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 40: 841–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Grogan, John P., Govind Randhawa, Minho Kim, and Sanjay G. Manohar. 2022. Motivation improves working memory by two processes: Prioritisation and retrieval thresholds. Cognitive Psychology 135: 101472. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Hager, Oliver M., Matthias Kirschner, Martin Bischof, Matthias N. Hartmann-Riemer, Agne Kluge, Erich Seifritz, Philippe N. Tobler, and Stefan Kaiser. 2015. Reward-dependent modulation of working memory is associated with negative symptoms in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research 168: 238–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  44. Hammer, Rubi, Michael Tennekoon, Gillian E. Cooke, Jessica Gayda, Mark A. Stein, and James R. Booth. 2015. Feedback associated with expectation for larger-reward improves visuospatial working memory performances in children with ADHD. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience 14: 38–49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Heitz, Richard P., Josef C. Schrock, Tabitha W. Payne, and Randall W. Engle. 2008. Effects of incentive on working memory capacity: Behavioral and pupillometric data. Psychophysiology 45: 119–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Heuer, Anna, and Anna Schubö. 2018. Separate and combined effects of action relevance and motivational value on visual working memory. Journal of Vision 18: 14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  47. Hinault, Thomas, Kara J. Blacker, Matt Gormley, Brian A. Anderson, and Susan M. Courtney. 2019. Value-driven attentional capture is modulated by the contents of working memory: An EEG study. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience 19: 253–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Hitch, Graham J., Yanmei Hu, Richard J. Allen, and Alan D. Baddeley. 2018. Competition for the focus of attention in visual working memory: Perceptual recency versus executive control. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1424: 64–75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Ilkowska, Małgorzata, and Randall W. Engle. 2010. Trait and state differences in working memory capacity. In Handbook of Individual Differences in Cognition: Attention, Memory, and Executive Control. New York: Springer, pp. 295–320. [Google Scholar]
  50. Infanti, Elisa, Clayton Hickey, and Massimo Turatto. 2015. Reward associations impact both iconic and visual working memory. Vision Research 107: 22–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Jiang, Zanzan. 2013. Reward Effect in Spatial Working Memory. Master’s thesis, Capital Normal University, Beijing, China. Available online: https://kns-cnki-net-443.webvpn.sysu.edu.cn/KCMS/detail/detail.aspx?dbname=CMFD201402&filename=1013289518.nh (accessed on 15 December 2022). (In Chinese).
  52. Jimura, Koji, Hannah S. Locke, and Todd S. Braver. 2010. Prefrontal cortex mediation of cognitive enhancement in rewarding motivational contexts. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 107: 8871–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Juckel, Georg, Florian Schlagenhauf, Michael Koslowski, Dimitri Filonov, Torsten Wüstenberg, Arno Villringer, Brian Knutson, Thorsten Kienast, Jürgen Gallinat, Jana Wrase, and et al. 2006. Dysfunction of ventral striatal reward prediction in schizophrenic patients treated with typical, not atypical, neuroleptics. Psychopharmacology 187: 222–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Kalma, Akko. 1991. Hierarchisation and dominance assessment at first glance. European Journal of Social Psychology 21: 165–81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Kawasaki, Masahiro, and Yoko Yamaguchi. 2013. Frontal theta and beta synchronizations for monetary reward increase visual working memory capacity. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 8: 523–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  56. Klink, Christiaan, Danique Jeurissen, Jan Theeuwes, Damiaan Denys, and Pieter R. Roelfsema. 2017. Working memory accuracy for multiple targets is driven by reward expectation and stimulus contrast with different time-courses. Scientific Reports 7: 9082. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  57. Klyszejko, Zuzanna, Masih Rahmati, and Clayton E. Curtis. 2014. Attentional priority determines working memory precision. Vision Research 105: 70–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  58. Kotyusov, Alexander I., Dauren Kasanov, Alexandra I. Kosachenko, Anastasia S. Gashkova, Yuri G. Pavlov, and Sergey Malykh. 2023. Working Memory Capacity Depends on Attention Control, but Not Selective Attention. Behavioral Sciences 13: 92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Krawczyk, Daniel C., Adam Gazzaley, and Mark D’Esposito. 2007. Reward modulation of prefrontal and visual association cortex during an incentive working memory task. Brain Research 1141: 168–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Kuhbandner, Christof, Alp Aslan, Kathrin Emmerdinger, and Kou Murayama. 2016. Providing extrinsic reward for test performance undermines long-term memory acquisition. Frontiers in Psychology 7: 79. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Kuvaas, Bård, Robert Buch, Antoinette Weibel, Anders Dysvik, and Christina G.L. Nerstad. 2017. Do intrinsic and extrinsic motivation relate differently to employee outcomes? Journal of Economic Psychology 61: 244–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Lee, Chong Ho, Myung Sook An, Jae Wook Um, and Dong Myong Lee. 2011. Effects of Organizational and Social Rewards on Positive Emotional Displays among Flight Attendants. Journal of the Aviation Management Society of Korea 9: 105. [Google Scholar]
  63. Lennie, Peter. 2003. The cost of cortical computation. Current Biology 13: 493–97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Lepper, Mark, and David Greene. 1976. On Understanding “Overjustification”: A Reply to Reiss and Sushinsky. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 33: 25–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Li, Jiaofeng, Jiehui Qian, and Fang Liang. 2018. Evidence for the beneficial effect of perceptual grouping on visual working memory: An empirical study on illusory contour and a meta-analytic study. Scientific Reports 8: 13864. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  66. Lin, Yin-Ting, and Daryl Fougnie. 2022. No evidence that the retro-cue benefit requires reallocation of memory resources. Cognition 229: 105230. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  67. Lipsey, Mark W., and David B. Wilson. 2001. Practical Meta-Analysis. Thousand Oaks: Sage Publications. [Google Scholar]
  68. Li, Shu-Chen, and Anna Rieckmann. 2014. Neuromodulation and aging: Implications of aging neuronal gain control on cognition. Current Opinion in Neurobiology 29: 148–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Lisman, John, and Marco Idiart. 1995. Storage of 7 + 2 short-term memories in oscillatory subcycles. Science 267: 1512–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Li, Zhuolun, Meixuan Tong, Shiting Chen, and Jiehui Qian. 2021. Effect of attentional selection on working memory for depth in a retro-cueing paradigm. Memory & Cognition 49: 747–57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Luck, Steven J., and Edward K. Vogel. 1997. The capacity of visual working memory for features and conjunctions. Nature 390: 279–81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Magis-Weinberg, Lucia, Ruud Custers, and Iroise Dumontheil. 2019. Rewards enhance proactive and reactive control in adolescence and adulthood. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 14: 1219–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  73. Manga, Annamária, Pál Vakli, and Zoltán Vidnyánszky. 2020. The influence of anticipated monetary incentives on visual working memory performance in healthy younger and older adults. Scientific Reports 10: 8817. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  74. Manohar, Sanjay G., Kinan Muhammed, Sean J. Fallon, and Masud Husain. 2019. Motivation dynamically increases noise resistance by internal feedback during movement. Neuropsychologia 123: 19–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  75. Marquand, Andre F., Sara De Simoni, Owen G. O’Daly, Steven C. R. Williams, Janaina Mouro-Miranda, and Mitul A. Mehta. 2011. Pattern classification of working memory networks reveals differential effects of methylphenidate, atomoxetine, and placebo in healthy volunteers. Neuropsychopharmacology 36: 1237–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  76. Martin, Joanne, and Joseph W. Harder. 1994. Bread and roses: Justice and the distribution of financial and socioemotional rewards in organizations. Social Justice Research 7: 241–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  77. May, Christopher, Mauricio R. Delgado, Ronald E. Dahl, V. Andrew Stenger, Neal D. Ryan, Julie A. Fiez, and Cameron S. Carter. 2004. Event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging of reward-related brain circuitry in children and adolescents. Biological Psychiatry 55: 359–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  78. Melin, Jeanette, Stefan Cano, Agnes Flöel, Laura Göschel, and Leslie Pendrill. 2022. The role of entropy in construct specification equations (CSE) to improve the validity of memory tests: Extension to word lists. Entropy 24: 934. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  79. Miller, George. 1956. The magical number seven, plus or minus two: Some limits on our capacity for processing information. Psychological Review 101: 343–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  80. Moos, Daniel C. 2010. Nonlinear technology: Changing the conception of extrinsic motivation? Computers & Education 55: 1640–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  81. Morey, Candice Coker, Nelson Cowan, Richard D. Morey, and Jeffery N. Rouder. 2011. Flexible attention allocation to visual and auditory working memory tasks: Manipulating reward induces a trade-off. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics 73: 458–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  82. Mosteller, Frederick, and Robert R. Bush. 1954. Selected Quantitative Techniques. Boston: Addison-Wesley. [Google Scholar]
  83. Ng, Tommy, Cathy Kao, Y. C. Chan, Effie Chew, Kai Hsiang Chuang, and Shen-Hsing A. Chen. 2016. Modality specificity in the cerebro-cerebellar neurocircuitry during working memory. Behavioural Brain Research 305: 164–73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  84. Nieoullon, André, and Antoine Coquerel. 2003. Dopamine: A key regulator to adapt action, emotion, motivation and cognition. Current Opinion in Neurology 16: S3–S9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  85. Pashler, Harold. 1988. Familiarity and visual change detection. Perception & Psychophysics 44: 369–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  86. Pessoa, Luiz. 2009. How do emotion and motivation direct executive control? Trends in Cognitive Sciences 13: 160–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  87. Pessoa, Luiz, and Jan B. Engelmann. 2010. Embedding reward signals into perception and cognition. Frontiers in Neuroscience 4: 17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  88. Peternel, Luka, Oliver Sigaud, and Jan Babič. 2017. Unifying speed-accuracy trade-off and cost-benefit trade-off in human reaching movements. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 11: 615. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  89. Qian, Jiehui, Ke Zhang, Quan Lei, Yifei Han, and Wenwen Li. 2020. Task-dependent effects of voluntary space-based and involuntary feature-based attention on visual working memory. Psychological Research 84: 1304–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  90. Qian, Jiehui, Ke Zhang, Shengxi Liu, and Quan Lei. 2019. The transition from feature to object: Storage unit in visual working memory depends on task difficulty. Memory & Cognition 47: 1498–514. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  91. Rademacher, Lena, Sören Krach, Gregor Kohls, Arda Irmak, Gerhard Gründer, and Katja N. Spreckelmeyer. 2010. Dissociation of neural networks for anticipation and consumption of monetary and social rewards. NeuroImage 49: 3276–85. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  92. Redick, Thomas S., and Dakota R. Lindsey. 2013. Complex span and n-back measures of working memory: A meta-analysis. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 20: 1102–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  93. Rhodes, Rebecca E., and Benjamin Katz. 2017. Working memory plasticity and aging. Psychology and Aging 32: 51–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  94. Ricker, Timothy J., Alessandra S. Souza, and Evie Vergauwe. 2023. Feature identity determines representation structure in working memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 152: 2925–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  95. Rosenthal, Robert. 1979. The file drawer problem and tolerance for null results. Psychological Bulletin 86: 638. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  96. Rowe, James B., Doris Eckstein, Todd Braver, and Adrian M. Owen. 2008. How Does Reward Expectation Influence Cognition in the Human Brain? Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 20: 1980–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  97. Sanada, Motoyuki, Koki Ikeda, Kenta Kimura, and Toshikazu Hasegawa. 2013. Motivation enhances visual working memory capacity through the modulation of central cognitive processes. Psychophysiology 50: 864–71. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  98. Sandry, Joshua, and Timothy J. Ricker. 2020. Prioritization within visual working memory reflects a flexible focus of attention. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics 82: 2985–3004. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  99. Savine, Adam C., Stefanie M. Beck, Bethany G. Edwards, Kimberly S. Chiew, and Todd S. Braver. 2010. Enhancement of cognitive control by approach and avoidance motivational states. Cognition and Emotion 24: 338–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  100. Schmeichel, Brandon J. 2007. Attention control, memory updating, and emotion regulation temporarily reduce the capacity for executive control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 136: 241. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  101. Schneegans, Sebastian, and Paul M. Bays. 2016. No fixed item limit in visuospatial working memory. Cortex 83: 181–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  102. Shadmehr, Reza, de Jean Jacques Orban Xivry, Minnan Xu-Wilson, and Ting-Yu Shih. 2010. Temporal discounting of reward and the cost of time in motor control. The Journal of Neuroscience 30: 10507–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  103. Shaw, Rhonda M., Edward Helmes, and David Mitchell. 2006. Age-related change in visual, spatial and verbal memory. Australasian Journal on Ageing 25: 14–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  104. Smith, Edward E., John Jonides, and Robert A. Koeppe. 1996. Dissociating verbal and spatial working memory using PET. Cerebral Cortex 6: 11–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  105. Stebbins, Glenn T., John DE Gabrieli, Francesca Masciari, Laura Monti, and Christopher G. Goetz. 1999. Delayed recognition memory in Parkinsons disease: A role for working memory? Neuropsychologia 37: 503–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  106. Sterling, Peter. 2017. Principles of Neural Design. Cambridge: The MIT Press. [Google Scholar]
  107. Szatkowska, Iwona, Piotr Bogorodzki, Tomasz Wolak, Artur Marchewka, and Wojciech Szeszkowski. 2008. The effect of motivation on working memory: An fMRI and SEM study. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory 90: 475–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  108. Taylor, Stephan F., Robert C. Welsh, Tor D. Wager, K. Luan Phan, Kate D. Fitzgerald, and William J. Gehring. 2004. A functional neuroimaging study of motivation and executive function. Neuroimage 21: 1045–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  109. Thornton, Allen E., Vanessa G. Boudreau, Stephanie Y. Griffiths, Todd S. Woodward, Tanya Fawkes-Kirby, and William G. Honer. 2007. The impact of monetary reward on memory in schizophrenia spectrum disorder. Neuropsychology 21: 631. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  110. Thurm, Franka, Nicolas Zink, and Shu-Chen Li. 2018. Comparing effects of reward anticipation on working memory in younger and older adults. Frontiers in Psychology 9: 2318. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  111. Tian, Shuang. 2020. Effects of Reward Directing Attention and Space-Based Attention on VWM Representation. Master’s thesis, Shangdong Normal University, Jinan, China. Available online: https://kns-cnki-net-443.webvpn.sysu.edu.cn/KCMS/detail/detail.aspx?dbname=CMFD202002&filename=1020028310.nh (accessed on 15 December 2022). (In Chinese).
  112. Turner, Marilyn L., and Randall W. Engle. 1989. Is working memory capacity task dependent? Journal of Memory and Language 28: 127–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  113. Unsworth, Nash, and Randall W. Engle. 2007. The nature of individual differences in working memory capacity: Active maintenance in primary memory and controlled search from secondary memory. Psychological Review 114: 104. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  114. Unsworth, Nash, Keisuke Fukuda, Edward Awh, and Edward K. Vogel. 2014. Working memory and fluid intelligence: Capacity, attention control, and secondary memory retrieval. Cognitive Psychology 71: 1–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  115. van den Berg, Ronald, and Wei Ji Ma. 2018. A resource-rational theory of set size effects in human visual working memory. eLife 7: e34963. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  116. van den Berg, Ronald, Hongsup Shin, Wen-Chuang Chou, Ryan George, and Wei Ji Ma. 2012. Variability in encoding precision accounts for visual short-term memory limitations. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 109: 8780–85. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  117. van den Berg, Ronald, Qijia Zou, Yuhang Li, and Wei Ji Ma. 2023. No effect of monetary reward in a visual working memory task. PLoS ONE 18: e0280257. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  118. Wallis, George, Mark G. Stokes, Craig Arnold, and Anna C. Nobre. 2015. Reward boosts working memory encoding over a brief temporal window. Visual Cognition 23: 291–312. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  119. Watanabe, Masataka. 2007. Role of anticipated reward in cognitive behavioral control. Current Opinion in Neurobiology 17: 213–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  120. Wei, Ziqiang, Xiao-Jing Wang, and Da-Hui Wang. 2012. From distributed resources to limited slots in multiple-item working memory: A spiking network model with normalization. The Journal of Neuroscience 32: 11228–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  121. Wilken, Patrick, and Wei Ji Ma. 2004. A detection theory account of change detection. Journal of Vision 4: 1120–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  122. Xu, Shiyang, Senqing Qi, Haijun Duan, Juan Zhang, Miriam Akioma, Fei Gao, Anise M. S. Wu, and Zhen Yuan. 2022. Task Difficulty Regulates How Conscious and Unconscious Monetary Rewards Boost the Performance of Working Memory: An Event-Related Potential Study. Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience 139: 716961. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  123. Young, Mark R. 2005. The Motivational Effects of the Classroom Environment in Facilitating Self-Regulated Learning. Journal of Marketing Education 27: 25–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  124. Zedelius, Claire M., Harm Veling, and Henk Aarts. 2012. When unconscious rewards boost cognitive task performance inefficiently: The role of consciousness in integrating value and attainability information. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 6: 219. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  125. Zhang, Ke, and Jiehui Qian. 2022. The role of ensemble average differs in working memory for depth and planar information. Journal of Vision 22: 4. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  126. Zhang, Ke, Dingguo Gao, and Jiehui Qian. 2021. Overestimation and contraction biases of depth information stored in working memory depend on spatial configuration. British Journal of Psychology 112: 230–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  127. Zhang, Sheng. 2021. Adaptive Invocation of Visual Working Memory Resources. Master’s thesis, Southwest University, Chongqing, China. Available online: https://kns-cnki-net-443.webvpn.sysu.edu.cn/KCMS/detail/detail.aspx?dbname=CMFD202201&filename=1021768929.nh (accessed on 15 December 2022). (In Chinese).
  128. Zhang, Weiwei, and Steven J. Luck. 2008. Discrete fixed-resolution representations in visual working memory. Nature 453: 233–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  129. Zheng, Weixi, Jiayang Geng, Dexiang Zhang, Jie Zhang, and Jingpeng Qiao. 2022. Task difficulty rather than reward method modulates the reward boosts in visual working memory. Journal of Vision 22: 1. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  130. Zhou, Qingjie, Zanzan Jiang, and Jinhong Ding. 2021. Reward Expectation Differentially Modulates Global and Local Spatial Working Memory Accuracy. Frontiers in Psychology 12: 744400. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Effect sizes for all studies.
Figure 1. Effect sizes for all studies.
Jintelligence 12 00088 g001
Figure 2. Funnel plot. (A) Funnel plot (with outliers). (B) Funnel plot (without outliers).
Figure 2. Funnel plot. (A) Funnel plot (with outliers). (B) Funnel plot (without outliers).
Jintelligence 12 00088 g002
Figure 3. Forest plots for all moderators.
Figure 3. Forest plots for all moderators.
Jintelligence 12 00088 g003
Table 1. Characteristics of Individual Studies.
Table 1. Characteristics of Individual Studies.
Study (Author/Year)Sample SizeType of RewardReward MethodsType of WMParadigm of WMOutcome MeasuresParticipant GroupEffect Size (d)
Allen and Ueno (2018) Experiment 120Non-monetaryReward bindingVisualRecallACCHealthy adults1.12
Allen and Ueno (2018) Experiment 220Non-monetaryReward bindingVisualRecallACCHealthy adults1.34
Allen and Ueno (2018) Experiment 320Non-monetaryReward bindingVisualRecallACCHealthy adults2.85
Allen and Ueno (2018) Experiment 420Non-monetaryReward bindingVisualRecallACCHealthy adults2.56
Beck et al. (2010)31MonetaryReward expectationVerbalRecognitionRTHealthy adults3.89
Belayachi et al. (2015)18MonetaryReward expectationVerbalN-backACC, RTHealthy adults0.18
Capa and Bouquet (2018)60MonetaryReward expectation,
Subliminal reward
VerbalOthersACCHealthy adults1.46
Cho et al. (2018)65MonetaryReward bindingVisualDelay estimationErrorHealthy adults,
Others
0.79
Cho et al. (2022)33MonetaryReward bindingVisualDelay estimationErrorHealthy adults0.68
Choi et al. (2015)26MonetaryReward expectationVisualRecognitionACCHealthy adults1.38
Cohen et al. (2019)20MonetaryReward expectationVerbalRecognitionACCHealthy adults0.99
Di Rosa et al. (2019)21MonetaryReward expectationVisualRecognitionACC, RTOthers0.24
Gaillard et al. (2019)23MonetaryReward expectationVisualChange detectionACC, RTHealthy adults0.45
Gaillard et al. (2020)32MonetaryReward expectationVisualChange detectionACC, RTHealthy adults0.30
Grogan et al. (2022) Experiment 130MonetaryReward expectationVisualDelay estimationACC, RTHealthy adults0.86
Grogan et al. (2022) Experiment 430MonetaryReward bindingVisualDelay estimationACC, RTHealthy adults0.98
Hager et al. (2015)56MonetaryReward expectationVerbalN-backACC, RTHealthy adults,
Others
−0.01
Hammer et al. (2015)34MonetaryReward expectationVerbalN-backACCHealthy adults0.84
Heitz et al. (2008)30MonetaryReward expectationVerbalOthersACC, RTHealthy adults0.52
Heuer and Schubö (2018) Experiment 1B14MonetaryReward bindingVisualRecognitionACC, RTHealthy adults1.17
Heuer and Schubö (2018) Experiment 224MonetaryReward bindingVisualChange detectionACCHealthy adults1.15
Hinault et al. (2019)19MonetaryReward bindingVisualOthersACC, RTHealthy adults1.88
Infanti et al. (2015)30MonetaryReward bindingVisualRecallACCHealthy adults2.85
Jiang (2013) Experiment 120Non-monetaryReward bindingVisualRecallACCHealthy adults1.31
Jiang (2013) Experiment 220Non-monetaryReward bindingVisualRecallACCHealthy adults1.13
Jimura et al. (2010)31MonetaryReward expectationVerbalRecognitionRTHealthy adults1.58
Kawasaki and Yamaguchi (2013)14MonetaryReward expectationVisualChange detectionACCHealthy adults0.39
Klink et al. (2017) Experiment 110MonetaryReward bindingVisualDelay estimationErrorHealthy adults3.23
Klink et al. (2017) Experiment 28MonetaryReward expectationVisualDelay estimationErrorHealthy adults−0.02
Lin and Fougnie (2022) Experiment 156MonetaryReward bindingVisualDelay estimationErrorHealthy adults1.04
Lin and Fougnie (2022) Experiment 256MonetaryReward bindingVisualDelay estimationErrorHealthy adults1.52
Magis-Weinberg et al. (2019)50MonetaryReward expectationVerbalRecognitionACC, RTHealthy adults0.50
Manga et al. (2020)146MonetaryReward expectationVisualDelay estimationError, RTHealthy adults,
Others
0.02
Morey et al. (2011) Experiment 132MonetaryReward bindingVisualChange detectionACCHealthy adults0.82
Morey et al. (2011) Experiment 230MonetaryReward expectationVisualChange detectionACCHealthy adults0.67
Morey et al. (2011) Experiment 324MonetaryReward bindingVisualChange detectionACCHealthy adults1.08
Sanada et al. (2013)12MonetaryReward expectationVisualChange detectionACCHealthy adults2.16
Sandry and Ricker (2020) Experiment 129Non-monetaryReward bindingVisualRecognition ACC, RTHealthy adults1.06
Sandry and Ricker (2020) Experiment 231Non-monetaryReward bindingVisualRecognitionACC, RTHealthy adults0.95
Sandry and Ricker (2020) Experiment 330Non-monetaryReward bindingVisualRecognitionACC, RTHealthy adults1.43
Savine et al. (2010) Experiment 230MonetaryReward expectationVerbalRecognitionACC, RTHealthy adults0.24
Szatkowska et al. (2008)16MonetaryReward expectationVerbalN-backACC, RTHealthy adults0.00
Taylor et al. (2004)12MonetaryReward bindingVisualRecognitionACC, RTHealthy adults0.47
Thornton et al. (2007)102MonetaryReward expectationVisualN-backACCHealthy adults,
Others
0.00
Thurm et al. (2018)44MonetaryReward bindingVerbalN-backACC, RTHealthy adults,
Others
0.97
Tian (2020) Experiment 124Non-monetaryReward bindingVisualRecallACCHealthy adults2.12
Tian (2020) Experiment 224Non-monetaryReward bindingVisualRecallACCHealthy adults2.35
Xu et al. (2022)34MonetarySubliminal rewardVerbalN-backACC, RTHealthy adults1.04
Zedelius et al. (2012) Experiment 141MonetarySubliminal rewardVerbalRecallACCHealthy adults0.50
Zedelius et al. (2012) Experiment 233MonetarySubliminal rewardVerbalRecallACCHealthy adults0.94
Zhang (2021) Experiment 126Non-monetaryReward expectationVisualRecallACCHealthy adults1.74
Zhang (2021) Experiment 230Non-monetaryReward expectationVisualRecallACCHealthy adults2.26
Zheng et al. (2022) Experiment 156MonetaryReward bindingVisualRecallACCHealthy adults0.55
Zheng et al. (2022) Experiment 256MonetaryReward bindingVisualRecallACCHealthy adults0.74
Zhou et al. (2021)20MonetaryReward bindingVisualRecallACCHealthy adults1.22
Table 2. Main results.
Table 2. Main results.
No. of
Studies
Sample
Size
Cohen’s dStandard ErrorZp95% CIHeterogeneityFail-Safe N
QpI2
Overall5117110.930.0811.71<0.001[0.77, 1.08]314.88<0.00184.129362
Moderator analysis
Type of reward
Monetary3913610.780.089.61<0.001[0.62, 0.94]210.23<0.00181.924440
Non-monetary102541.620.18 9.20<0.001[1.28, 1.97]30.66<0.00170.65713
Reward methods
Reward binding237031.100.1011.30 <0.001[0.91, 1.29]79.75<0.00172.412581
Reward expectation2810080.82 0.12 7.00<0.001[0.59, 1.05]226.72 <0.00188.092307
Subliminal reward41680.72 0.23 3.16 0.002[0.27, 1.17]18.150.00683.4759
Type of WM
Visual3711501.050.1011.15<0.001[0.87, 1.24]209.82<0.00182.845712
Verbal145610.620.14 4.58<0.001[0.36, 0.89]84.28<0.00184.58433
Paradigm of WM
N-back73000.430.192.25 0.025[0.05, 0.80]37.20<0.00183.8746
Change detection61360.94 0.18 5.40 <0.001[0.60, 1.29]13.34 0.00262.51118
Recognition133490.79 0.13 5.97 <0.001[0.53, 1.05]56.10 <0.00178.61477
Recall103061.23 0.19 6.40 <0.001[0.85, 1.60]50.51 <0.00182.18508
Delay estimation83520.78 0.18 4.41 <0.001[0.43, 1.13]41.82 <0.00183.26240
Others31091.24 0.40 3.07 0.002[0.45, 2.03]15.82<0.00187.3652
Outcome measures
Error84230.750.164.60<0.001[0.43, 1.08]43.06 <0.00183.75250
ACC4212570.95 0.09 10.88 <0.001[0.78, 1.13]241.85 <0.00183.056265
RT206900.69 0.14 5.08<0.001[0.42, 0.96]161.75 <0.00188.25806
Participant group
Healthy adults4914520.95 0.0811.53<0.001[0.79, 1.11]290.38<0.00183.478503
Others62250.33 0.16 1.99 0.047[0.01, 0.65]30.26<0.00183.4823
Table 3. Secondary subgroup analysis.
Table 3. Secondary subgroup analysis.
No. of
Studies
Sample
Size
Cohen’s dStandard ErrorZp95% CIHeterogeneityFail-Safe N
QpI2
Monetary reward
Outcome measures
Error84180.860.204.25<0.001[0.46, 1.26]54.02<0.00187.04247
ACC196280.880.127.51<0.001[0.66, 1.12]90.71<0.00180.161230
RT155410.480.143.54<0.001[0.21, 0.74]95.86<0.00185.40227
Visual WM
Outcome measures
Error74150.790.174.71<0.001[0.46, 1.12]41.77<0.00185.64252
ACC215241.240.129.96<0.001[1.00, 1.49]81.13<0.00175.352006
RT103550.750.223.410.001[0.32, 1.18]97.27<0.00190.76170
Reward binding
Type of reward
Monetary154691.030.0813.40<0.001[0.88, 1.19]22.850.06338.731155
Non-monetary81981.530.197.88<0.001[1.15, 1.91]22.940.00269.48400
Reward expectation
Type of reward
Monetary207240.520.105.08<0.001[0.32, 0.72]91.78<0.00179.30512
Non-monetary2561.980.267.71<0.001[1.48, 2.49]1.430.23229.98--
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Wang, W.; Yan, X.; He, X.; Qian, J. Evidence for the Beneficial Effect of Reward on Working Memory: A Meta-Analytic Study. J. Intell. 2024, 12, 88. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence12090088

AMA Style

Wang W, Yan X, He X, Qian J. Evidence for the Beneficial Effect of Reward on Working Memory: A Meta-Analytic Study. Journal of Intelligence. 2024; 12(9):88. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence12090088

Chicago/Turabian Style

Wang, Weiyu, Xin Yan, Xinyu He, and Jiehui Qian. 2024. "Evidence for the Beneficial Effect of Reward on Working Memory: A Meta-Analytic Study" Journal of Intelligence 12, no. 9: 88. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence12090088

APA Style

Wang, W., Yan, X., He, X., & Qian, J. (2024). Evidence for the Beneficial Effect of Reward on Working Memory: A Meta-Analytic Study. Journal of Intelligence, 12(9), 88. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence12090088

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop