Increasing IQ Test Scores and Decreasing g: The Flynn Effect and Decreasing Positive Manifold Strengths in Austria (2005–2018)
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Participants
2.2. Intelligence Base Functions (IBF; Blum et al. 2018)
2.2.1. Numerical Intelligence Functions
Numerical Reasoning
Mathematical Problem Solving
2.2.2. Spatial Ability
2.2.3. Long-Term Memory
2.2.4. Verbal Intelligence Functions
Verbal Comprehension
Verbal Analogies
2.3. Procedure
2.4. Statistical Analyses
2.4.1. Measurement Invariance
2.4.2. Positive Manifold Changes
2.4.3. Ability Tilt
3. Results
3.1. Measurement Invariance
3.2. Positive Manifold Changes
3.3. Ability Tilt
4. Discussion
4.1. Potential Causes
4.2. Limitations and Future Directions
5. Conclusions
Supplementary Materials
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Blum, Florian, Hans-Joachim Didi, Elisabeth Fay, Ulrich Maichle, Gerhard Trost, Hans-Joachim Wahlen, and Gerhard Gittler. 2018. Basic Intelligence Functions (IBF). Mödling: Schuhfried. [Google Scholar]
- Bratsberg, Bjørnand, and Ole Rogeberg. 2018. Flynn effect and its reversal are both environmentally caused. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 115: 6674–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cheung, Gordon W., and Randall B. Rensvold. 2002. Evaluating Goodness-of-Fit Indexes for Testing Measurement Invariance. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal 9: 233–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Clark, Christina M., Laura Lawlor-Savage, and Vanessa M. Goghari. 2016. The Flynn Effect: A Quantitative Commentary on Modernity and Human Intelligence. Measurement: Interdisciplinary Research and Perspectives 14: 39–53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Colom, Roberto, Luis F. García, Pei-Chen Shih, and Francisco J. Abad. 2023. Generational intelligence tests score changes in Spain: Are we asking the right question? Intelligence 99: 101772. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Colom, Roberto, Manuel Juanespinosa, and Luis Garcia. 2001. The secular increase in test scores is a “Jensen effect”. Personality and Individual Differences 30: 553–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Coolberth, Nicole L. 2024. Digital Equity Advances in Congress: Digital Skills for Today’s Workforce. National Skills Coalition. Available online: https://nationalskillscoalition.org/blog/future-of-work/digital-equity-advances-in-congress-digital-skills-for-todays-workforce/ (accessed on 22 April 2024).
- Cotton, Stephen M., Peter M. Kiely, David P. Crewther, Benjamin Thomson, Robert Laycock, and Stephen G. Crewther. 2005. A normative and reliability study for the Raven’s Coloured Progressive Matrices for primary school aged children from Victoria, Australia. Personality and Individual Differences 39: 647–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Coyle, Thomas R. 2014. Predictive Validity of Non-g Residuals of Tests: More Than g. Journal of Intelligence 2: 21–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Coyle, Thomas R. 2023. All (tilt) models are wrong, but some are useful: A reply to Sorjonen et al.’s (2023) critique of tilt. Intelligence 98: 101749. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dutton, Edward, and Richard Lynn. 2013. A negative Flynn effect in Finland, 1997–2009. Intelligence 41: 817–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dutton, Edward, and Richard Lynn. 2015. A negative Flynn Effect in France, 1999 to 2008–9. Intelligence 51: 67–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dworak, Emily M., William Revelle, and David M. Condon. 2023. Looking for Flynn effects in a recent online U.S. adult sample: Examining shifts within the SAPA Project. Intelligence 98: 101734. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- European Commission. 2020. Commission Presents European Skills Agenda [Text]. European Commission–European Commission. Available online: https://ec.europa.eu/commission/presscorner/detail/en/ip_20_1196 (accessed on 3 May 2024).
- European Commission. 2022. Population on 1 January by Age Group and Sex (demo_ pjangroup) [Data File] [Dataset]. Available online: https://ec.europa.eu/eurostat/databrowser/view/DEMO_GIND__custom_7680622/bookmark/table?lang=en&bookmarkId=edacacb0-acad-4876-859f-9549d766826b (accessed on 3 May 2024).
- Flynn, James R. 1987. Massive IQ gains in 14 nations: What IQ tests really measure. Psychological Bulletin 101: 171–91. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Flynn, James R. 2010. Problems with IQ Gains: The Huge Vocabulary Gap. Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment 28: 412–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Flynn, James R. 2019. Secular Changes in Intelligence: The “Flynn Effect”. In The Cambridge Handbook of Intelligence, 2nd ed. Edited by R. J. Sternberg. New York: Cambridge University Press, pp. 940–63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Genovese, Joseph E. C. 2002. Cognitive Skills Valued by Educators: Historical Content Analysis of Testing in Ohio. The Journal of Educational Research 96: 101–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gignac, George E. 2015. The magical numbers 7 and 4 are resistant to the Flynn effect: No evidence for increases in forward or backward recall across 85 years of data. Intelligence 48: 85–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gonthier, Caroline, and Jean Grégoire. 2022. Flynn effects are biased by differential item functioning over time: A test using overlapping items in Wechsler scales. Intelligence 95: 101688. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lazaridis, Alexandros, Marco Vetter, and Jakob Pietschnig. 2022. Domain-specificity of Flynn effects in the CHC-model: Stratum II test score changes in Germanophone samples (1996–2018). Intelligence 95: 101707. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lynn, Richard, and Charles Cooper. 1993. A secular decline in Spearman’s g in France. Learning and Individual Differences 5: 43–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lynn, Richard, and Charles Cooper. 1994. A secular decline in the strength of Spearman’s g in Japan. Current Psychology 13: 3–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McDonald, Roderick P. 1999. Test Theory: A Unified Treatment. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum. [Google Scholar]
- Must, Olav, Andres Must, and Vahur Raudik. 2003. The secular rise in IQs: In Estonia, the Flynn effect is not a Jensen effect. Intelligence 31: 461–71. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Neisser, Ulric. 1997. Rising Scores on Intelligence Tests: Test scores are certainly going up all over the world, but whether intelligence itself has risen remains controversial. American Scientist 85: 440–47. Available online: https://www.jstor.org/stable/27856851 (accessed on 3 May 2024).
- Nyborg, Herbert. 2012. The decay of Western civilization: Double relaxed Darwinian Selection. Personality and Individual Differences 53: 118–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Oberleiter, Sandra, Jonathan Fries, Flornce Dejardin, Johanna Heller, Christian Schaible, Marco Vetter, Martin Voracek, and Jakob Pietschnig. 2024a. Inconsistent Flynn effect patterns may be due to a decreasing positive manifold: Cohort-based measurement-invariant IQ test score changes from 2005 to 2024. Intelligence 107: 101867. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Oberleiter, Sandra, Sabine Patzl, Jonathan Fries, Jennifer Diedrich, Martin Voracek, and Jakob Pietschnig. 2024b. Measurement-Invariant Fluid Anti-Flynn Effects in Population—Representative German Student Samples (2012–2022). Journal of Intelligence 12: 9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pietschnig, Jakob. 2016. The Flynn Effect: Technology May Be Part of It, but Is Most Certainly Not All of It. Measurement: Interdisciplinary Research and Perspectives 14: 70–73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pietschnig, Jakob, and Georg Gittler. 2015. A reversal of the Flynn effect for spatial perception in German-speaking countries: Evidence from a cross-temporal IRT-based meta-analysis (1977–2014). Intelligence 53: 145–53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pietschnig, Jakob, and Martin Voracek. 2015. One Century of Global IQ Gains: A Formal Meta-Analysis of the Flynn Effect (1909–2013). Perspectives on Psychological Science 10: 282–306. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Pietschnig, Jakob, Martin Voracek, and Anton K. Formann. 2010. Pervasiveness of the IQ Rise: A Cross-Temporal Meta-Analysis. PLoS ONE 5: e14406. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pietschnig, Jakob, Martin Voracek, and Georg Gittler. 2018. Is the Flynn effect related to migration? Meta-analytic evidence for correlates of stagnation and reversal of generational IQ test score changes. Politische Psychologie 2: 267–83. [Google Scholar]
- Pietschnig, Jakob, Sandra Oberleiter, Enrico Toffalini, and David Giofrè. 2023. Reliability of the g factor over time in Italian INVALSI data (2010–2022): What can achievement-g tell us about the Flynn effect? Personality and Individual Differences 214: 112345. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- R Core Team. 2023. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing (Version 4.3.2) [Computer Software]. R Foundation for Statistical Computing. Available online: https://www.R-project.org/ (accessed on 5 January 2023).
- Revelle, William, and John Wilt. 2013. The general factor of personality: A general critique. Journal of Research in Personality 47: 493–504. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rindermann, Heiner, and James Thompson. 2013. Ability rise in NAEP and narrowing ethnic gaps? Intelligence 41: 821–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rosseel, Yves. 2012. {lavaan}: An {R} Package for Structural Equation Modeling. Journal of Statistical Software 48: 1–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rönnlund, Michael, Berit Carlstedt, Yulia Blomstedt, Lars-Göran Nilsson, and Lars Weinehall. 2013. Secular trends in cognitive test performance: Swedish conscript data 1970–93. Intelligence 41: 19–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schneider, Joel, and Kevin McGrew. 2013. The Cattell-Horn-Carroll (CHC) Model of Intelligence v2.2: A Visual Tour and Summary. Available online: http://www.iapsych.com/chcv2.pdf (accessed on 13 March 2024).
- Schuhfried. 2018. Manual: INTELLIGENZ-BASIS-FUNKTIONEN. Mödling: Schuhfried. [Google Scholar]
- Sorjonen, Kimmo, Michael Ingre, Gustav Nilsonne, and Bo Melin. 2023. Further arguments that ability tilt correlations are spurious: A reply to Coyle (2022). Intelligence 98: 101706. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Spearman, Charles. 1904. “General intelligence”, objectively determined and measured. The American Journal of Psychology 15: 201–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Thurstone, Louis L., and Therese G. Thurstone. 1941. Factorial studies of intelligence. Psychometric Monographs 2: 94. [Google Scholar]
- Wickham, Hadley. 2016. ggplot2: Elegant Graphics for Data Analysis, 2nd ed. New York: Springer International Publishing, Imprint. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wongupparaj, Peera, Rangsirat Wongupparaj, Veena Kumari, and Robin G. Morris. 2017. The Flynn effect for verbal and visuospatial short-term and working memory: A cross-temporal meta-analysis. Intelligence 64: 71–80. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Woodley, Michael A., and Greg Meisenberg. 2013. In the Netherlands the anti-Flynn effect is a Jensen effect. Personality and Individual Differences 54: 871–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Woodley, Michael A., and Guy Madison. 2013. Establishing an association between the Flynn effect and ability differentiation. Personality and Individual Differences 55: 387–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wu, Hongyu, and Roy Estabrook. 2016. Identification of Confirmatory Factor Analysis Models of Different Levels of Invariance for Ordered Categorical Outcomes. Psychometrika 81: 1014–45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
2005 | 2011 | 2018 | |
---|---|---|---|
N | 630 | 476 | 286 |
Age | |||
Range | 14–77 | 16–85 | 14–83 |
Mean | 38.14 | 44.57 | 41.84 |
SD | 13.45 | 17.86 | 16.30 |
Sex | |||
Men | 313 (50%) | 237 (50%) | 129 (45%) |
Women | 317 (50%) | 239 (50%) | 157 (55%) |
Education * | |||
Level 1 | 17 (2.7%) | 8 (1.7%) | 4 (1.4%) |
Level 2 | 47 (7.5%) | 87 (18.3%) | 25 (8.7%) |
Level 3 | 351 (55.7%) | 228 (47.9%) | 92 (32.2%) |
Level 4 | 152 (24.1%) | 106 (22.3%) | 116 (40.6%) |
Level 5 | 63 (10%) | 47 (9.9%) | 49 (17.1%) |
Domain | Cohorts | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
2005 | 2011 | 2018 | ||||
M | SD | M | SD | M | SD | |
Numerical Reasoning (NR) | 0.0 * | 5.08 | 1.71 | 4.68 | 4.47 | 4.59 |
Mathematical Problem solving (MPS) | 0.0 * | 4.59 | 1.11 | 4.64 | 3.41 | 4.53 |
Spatial Ability (SA) | 0.0 * | 4.13 | −0.29 | 4.13 | 2.89 | 4.61 |
Long-term Memory (LTM) | 0.0 * | 4.81 | 1.50 | 4.86 | 3.44 | 4.84 |
Verbal Comprehension (VC) | 0.0 * | 3.19 | 0.68 | 2.82 | 1.92 | 2.18 |
Verbal Fluency (VF)—VIB | 0.0 * | 5.12 | 1.31 | 4.94 | 4.04 | 4.26 |
Domain | Stratum II Domain | Interval | Raw Scores | Latent Scores | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
d | DIQ | d | DIQ | |||
Full-scale IQ | 2005–2011 | .26 *** | 6.44 | - | - | |
2011–2018 | .67 *** | 14.42 | .69 ** | 14.75 | ||
2005–2018 | .35 *** | 8.70 | - | - | ||
Numerical Reasoning (NR) | Fluid reasoning (Gf) | 2005–2011 | .35 *** | 8.69 | - | - |
2011–2018 | .59 *** | 12.71 | .59 *** | 12.65 | ||
2005–2018 | .91 *** | 10.45 | - | - | ||
Mathematical Problem solving (MPS) | Quantitative knowledge (Gq) | 2005–2011 | .26 *** | 6.51 | - | - |
2011–2018 | .58 *** | 12.42 | .50 *** | 10.64 | ||
2005–2018 | .83 *** | 9.57 | - | - | ||
Spatial Ability (SA)—RV | Visual processing (Gv) | 2005–2011 | −.07 | −1.78 | - | - |
2011–2018 | .74 *** | 5.79 | .73 *** | 15.66 | ||
2005–2018 | .67 *** | 7.77 | - | - | ||
Long-term Memory (LTM) | Learning efficiency (Gl) | 2005–2011 | .31 *** | 7.77 | - | - |
2011–2018 | .40 *** | 8.54 | .40 *** | 8.59 | ||
2005–2018 | .71 *** | 8.23 | - | - | ||
Verbal Comprehension (VC) | Comprehension-knowledge (Gc) | 2005–2011 | .23 *** | 5.63 | - | - |
2011–2018 | .47 *** | 10.15 | .49 *** | 10.45 | ||
2005–2018 | .66 *** | 7.60 | - | - | ||
Verbal Analogies (VA) | Learning efficiency (Gl) | 2005–2011 | .26 *** | 6.51 | - | - |
2011–2018 | .58 *** | 12.42 | .59 *** | 12.72 | ||
2005–2018 | .83 *** | 9.57 | - | - |
Model | χ2 | df | p(χ2) | CFI | Model Comp. | ∆χ2 (∆df) | ∆CFI | Decision |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Numerical Reasoning (NR) | ||||||||
M1: Overall | 544.136 | 170 | .001 | .997 | - | |||
M2: Configural | 691.347 | 358 | .157 | .999 | - | |||
M3: Strict | 763.521 | 378 | .002 | .996 | M2 | 72.174 (20) | .003 | Accept |
Mathematical Problem solving (MPS) | ||||||||
M1: Overall | 230.034 | 170 | .001 | .997 | - | |||
M2: Configural | 384.901 | 358 | .157 | .999 | - | |||
M3: Strict | 462.276 | 378 | .002 | .996 | M2 | 77.375 (20) | .003 | Accept |
Spatial Ability (SA) | ||||||||
M1: Overall | 169.555 | 119 | .002 | .996 | - | |||
M2: Configural | 285.831 | 253 | .076 | .997 | - | |||
M3: Strict | 351.354 | 270 | .001 | .993 | M2 | 65.523 (23) | .004 | Accept |
Long-term Memory (LTM) | ||||||||
M1: Overall | 549.884 | 170 | <.001 | .965 | - | |||
M2: Configural | 697.226 | 358 | <.001 | .968 | - | |||
M3: Strict | 759.602 | 378 | <.001 | .963 | M2 | 62.376 (20) | .005 | Accept |
Verbal Comprehension (VC) | ||||||||
M1: Overall | 94.494 | 90 | .352 | .997 | - | |||
M2: Configural | 208.616 | 193 | .210 | .990 | - | |||
M3: Strict | 228.002 | 208 | .163 | .988 | M2 | 19.386 (15) | .002 | Accept |
Verbal Fluency (VF) | ||||||||
M1: Overall | 144.092 | 152 | .664 | 1.000 | - | |||
M2: Configural | 259.385 | 342 | .995 | 1.000 | - | |||
M3: Strict | 297.461 | 340 | .953 | 1.000 | M2 | 38.076 (2) | <.001 | Accept |
Raw Scores | Latent Scores | |||
---|---|---|---|---|
R2 | 95% CI | R2 | 95% CI | |
2005 | .908 | [.894; .922] | - | - |
2011 | .901 | [.884; .918] | .899 | [.882; .916] |
2018 | .892 | [.869; .915] | .888 | [.864; .912] |
Raw Scores | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|
α | 95% CI | ω | 95% CI | |
2005 | - | - | .81 | [.77; .82] |
2011 | .961 | [.956; .966] | .81 | [.70; .85] |
2018 | .958 | [.953; .963] | .79 | [.74; .84] |
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2024 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Andrzejewski, D.; Oberleiter, S.; Vetter, M.; Pietschnig, J. Increasing IQ Test Scores and Decreasing g: The Flynn Effect and Decreasing Positive Manifold Strengths in Austria (2005–2018). J. Intell. 2024, 12, 130. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence12120130
Andrzejewski D, Oberleiter S, Vetter M, Pietschnig J. Increasing IQ Test Scores and Decreasing g: The Flynn Effect and Decreasing Positive Manifold Strengths in Austria (2005–2018). Journal of Intelligence. 2024; 12(12):130. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence12120130
Chicago/Turabian StyleAndrzejewski, Denise, Sandra Oberleiter, Marco Vetter, and Jakob Pietschnig. 2024. "Increasing IQ Test Scores and Decreasing g: The Flynn Effect and Decreasing Positive Manifold Strengths in Austria (2005–2018)" Journal of Intelligence 12, no. 12: 130. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence12120130
APA StyleAndrzejewski, D., Oberleiter, S., Vetter, M., & Pietschnig, J. (2024). Increasing IQ Test Scores and Decreasing g: The Flynn Effect and Decreasing Positive Manifold Strengths in Austria (2005–2018). Journal of Intelligence, 12(12), 130. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence12120130