Decomposing the True Score Variance in Rated Responses to Divergent Thinking-Tasks for Assessing Creativity: A Multitrait–Multimethod Analysis
Abstract
:1. Introduction
- Model-implied ICCs can be computed for the DT-scores within a specific rating procedure (construct) that only consider variability of true scores and separate measurement error;
- DT-object-specific variability can be separated from measurement error;
- The model allows for the computation of additional informative relative true-score variance components such as various forms of consistency and method specificity;
- Using Bayesian methods, credibility intervals (CRIs) for all relative variances (mentioned in 1. and 3.) can be computed;
- Rater-effects (variability across raters) can be separated from interaction-effects (variability across rater–target interactions) which allows one to investigate whether raters consistently maintain their standards across targets;
- Due to the flexibility of SEM, the model can be extended to include attributes of raters in order to predict differences in raters, for example (the same is true for rater–target interactions).
2. Defining an Appropriate Cross-Classified CTC(M − 1) Model for DT-Ratings
3. Variance Decomposition
4. Empirical Application
4.1. The Data
4.2. Analytic Strategy
4.3. Results and Discussion
Thus, when low-quality ideas within a set of ideas should receive less weight in computing the score, the unweighted average across ideas should be avoided and the 0.75 quantile should be used for the distribution over the ideas (not raters).For example, there could be two participants who have the same number of good quality ideas, but one of the two has several more low-quality ideas. On average, these two performances may differ a great deal, but if the upper tails of their distributions are considered, the performances of both persons are much more alike.(p. 261)
5. General Discussion
5.1. Substantive Deliberations
5.2. Modifications, Extensions, Useful Applications, and Limitations of the Model
6. Conclusions
Supplementary Materials
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
Appendix A. Variance Decomposition in the Divergent Thinking Two-Level Model (DTTL)
Appendix B. Prior-Specifications within the DTCC and the DTTL-B of the Presented Application
References
- Asparouhov, Tihomir, and Bengt Muthén. 2021. Bayesian Analysis of Latent Variable Models Using Mplus. Los Angeles: Muthén & Muthén. Available online: http://www.statmodel.com/download/BayesAdvantages18.pdf (accessed on 20 July 2024).
- Bishop, Jacob, Christian Geiser, and David A. Cole. 2015. Modeling latent growth with multiple indicators: A comparison of three approaches. Psychological Methods 20: 43–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bollen, Kenneth A. 1989. Structural Equations with Latent Variables. New York: Wiley. [Google Scholar]
- Browne, Michael W., and Robert Cudeck. 1992. Alternative ways of assessing model fit. Sociological Methods & Research 21: 230–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Campbell, Donald T., and Donald W. Fiske. 1959. Convergent and discriminant validation by the multitrait-multimethod matrix. Psychological Bulletin 56: 81–105. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chen, Feinian, Patrick J. Curran, Kenneth A. Bollen, James Kirby, and Pamela Paxton. 2008. An empirical evaluation of the use of fixed cut-off points in RMSEA test statistic in structural equation models. Sociological Methods & Research 36: 462–94. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Costa, Paul T., and Robert R. McCrae. 1989. The NEO PI/FFI Manual Supplement. Odessa: Psychological Assessment Resources. [Google Scholar]
- Curran, Patrick J. 2003. Have multilevel models been structural equation models all along? Multivariate Behavioral Research 38: 529–69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- DeYoung, Colin G., Lena C. Quilty, and Jordan B. Peterson. 2007. Between facets and domains: 10 aspects of the Big Five. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 93: 880–96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Eid, Michael. 2000. A multitrait-multimethod model with minimal assumptions. Psychometrika 65: 241–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Eid, Michael, and Ed Diener. 2004. Global Judgments of Subjective Well-Being: Situational Variability and Long-Term Stability. Social Indicators Research 65: 245–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Eid, Michael, Fridtjof W. Nussbeck, Christian Geiser, David A. Cole, Mario Gollwitzer, and Tanja Lischetzke. 2008. Structural equation modeling of multitrait-multimethod data: Different models for different types of methods. Psychological Methods 13: 230–53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Eid, Michael, Tanja Lischetzke, Fridtjof W. Nussbeck, and Lisa I. Trierweiler. 2003. Separating trait effects from trait-specific method effects in multitrait-multimethod models: A multiple indicator CT-C(M-1) model. Psychological Methods 8: 38–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Forthmann, Boris, and Philipp Doebler. 2022. Fifty years later and still working: Rediscovering Paulus et al.’s (1970) automated ‘scoring of divergent thinking tests. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts. Advance online publication. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Forthmann, Boris, David Jendryczko, Jana Scharfen, Ruben Kleinkorres, Mathias Benedek, and Heinz Holling. 2019. Creative ideation, broad retrieval ability, and processing speed: A confirmatory study of nested cognitive abilities. Intelligence 75: 59–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Forthmann, Boris, Heinz Holling, Pınar Çelik, Martin Storme, and Todd Lubart. 2017. Typing Speed as a Confounding Variable and the Measurement of Quality in Divergent Thinking. Creativity Research Journal 29: 257–69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Forthmann, Boris, Sue H. Paek, Denis Dumas, Baptiste Barbot, and Heinz Holling. 2020. Scrutinizing the basis of originality in divergent thinking tests: On the measurement precision of response propensity estimates. British Journal of Educational Psychology 90: 683–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- French, John W., Ruth B. Ekstrom, and Leighton A. Price. 1963. Manual for Kit of Reference Tests for Cognitive Factors. Princeton: Educational Testing Service. [Google Scholar]
- Geiser, Christian, and Ginger Lockhart. 2012. A comparison of four approaches to account for method effects in latent state–trait analyses. Psychological Methods 17: 255–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Geiser, Christian, and Trenton G. Simmons. 2021. Do method effects generalize across traits (and what if they don’t)? Journal of Personality 89: 382–401. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Geiser, Christian, Michael Eid, and Fridtjof W. Nussbeck. 2008. On the Meaning of the Variance and Covariance Components in Multitrait-Multimethod Models: What Happens if Items Do Not Measure the Intended Constructs? Psychological Methods 13: 49–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gelman, Andrew, and Donald B. Rubin. 1992. Inference from iterative simulation using multiple sequences. Statistical Science 7: 457–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gelman, Andrew, Xiao-Li Meng, and Hal Stern. 1996. Posterior predictive assessment of model fitness via realized discrepancies. Statistica Sinica 6: 733–60. [Google Scholar]
- Guilford, Joy Paul. 1966. Measurement and creativity. Theory into Practice 5: 185–89. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Guilford, Joy Paul. 1967. The Nature of Human Intelligence. New York: McGraw-Hill. [Google Scholar]
- Hu, Li-tze, and Peter M. Bentler. 1999. Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal 6: 1–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jäger, Adolf O., Heinz Holling, Franzis Preckel, Ralf Schulze, Miriam Vock, Heinz-Martin Süß, and André Beauducel. 2006. BIS-HB: Berliner Intelligenzstrukturtest für Jugendliche: Begabungs- und Hochbegabungsdiagnostik—Manual. Göttingen: Hogrefe. [Google Scholar]
- Jendryczko, David, and Fridtjof W. Nussbeck. 2022. Estimating and investigating multiple constructs multiple indicators social relations models with and without roles within the traditional structural equation modeling framework: A tutorial. Psychological Methods. Advance online publication. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Jendryczko, David, and Fridtjof W. Nussbeck. 2024a. A correlated traits correlated (methods–1) multitrait-multimethod model for augmented round-robin data. British Journal of Mathematical and Statistical Psychology 77: 1–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Jendryczko, David, and Fridtjof W. Nussbeck. 2024b. Latent Reciprocal Engagement and Accuracy Variables in Social Relations Structural Equation Modeling. Multivariate Behavioral Research. Advance online publication. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jeon, Minjeong, and Frank Rijmen. 2014. Recent developments in maximum likelihood estimation of MTMM models for categorical data. Frontiers in Psychology 5: 269. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Johnson, Donald M., George L. Parrott, and R. Paul Stratton. 1968. Production and judgment of solutions to five problems. Journal of Educational Psychology 59: 1–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kim, Kyung Hee. 2008. Meta-analyses of the relationship of creative achievement to both IQ and divergent thinking test scores. Journal of Creative Behavior 42: 106–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Koch, Tobias, Jana Holtmann, Johannes Bohn, and Michael Eid. 2018. Explaining general and specific factors in longitudinal, multi-method, and bifactor models: Some caveats and recommendations. Psychological Methods 23: 505–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Koch, Tobias, Martin Schultze, Minjeong Jeon, Fridtjof W. Nussbeck, Anna-Katharina Praetorius, and Michael Eid. 2016. A Cross-Classified CFA-MTMM Model for Structurally Different and Nonindependent Interchangeable Methods. Multivariate Behavioral Research 51: 67–85. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Long, J. Scott, and Laurie H. Ervin. 2000. Using heteroscedasticity consistent standard errors in the linear regression model. The American Statistician 54: 217–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lubart, Todd, Chantal Pacteau, Anne-Yvonne Jacquet, and Xavier Caroff. 2010. Children’s creative potential: An empirical study of measurement issues. Learning and Individual Differences 20: 388–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mehta, Paras D., and Michael C. Neale. 2005. People are variables too: Multilevel structural equations modeling. Psychological Methods 10: 259–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Mullins, Cecil J. 1963. Prediction of creativity in a sample of research scientists. IEEE Transactions on Engineering Management 10: 52–57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Muthén, Linda K., and Bengt O. Muthén. 1998–2017. Mplus User’s Guide, 8th ed. Los Angeles: Muthén & Muthén. Available online: https://www.statmodel.com/download/usersguide/MplusUserGuideVer_8.pdf (accessed on 20 July 2024).
- Nusbaum, Emily C., Paul J. Silvia, and Roger E. Beaty. 2014. Ready, set, create: What instructing people to “be creative” reveals about the meaning and mechanisms of divergent thinking. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts 8: 423–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nussbeck, Fridtjof W., Michael Eid, and Tanja Lischetzke. 2006. Analysing multitrait–multimethod data with structural equation models for ordinal variables applying the WLSMV estimator: What sample size is needed for valid results? British Journal of Mathematical and Statistical Psychology 59: 195–213. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nussbeck, Fridtjof W., Michael Eid, Christian Geiser, Delphine S. Courvoisier, and Tanja Lischetzke. 2009. A CTC(M−1) model for different types of raters. Methodology 5: 88–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pihko, Elina, Anne Virtanen, Veli-Matti Saarinen, Sebastian Pannasch, Lotta Hirvenkari, Timo Tossavainen, Arto Haapala, and Riitta Hari. 2011. Experiencing art: The influence of expertise and painting abstraction level. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 5: 94. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Preckel, Franzis, Christina Wermer, and Frank M. Spinath. 2011. The interrelationship between speeded and unspeeded divergent thinking and reasoning, and the role of mental speed. Intelligence 39: 378–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- R Core Team. 2013. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. Available online: http://www.R-project.org/ (accessed on 20 July 2024).
- Runco, Mark A., and Garrett J. Jaeger. 2012. The standard definition of creativity. Creativity Research Journal 24: 92–96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Runco, Mark A., and Selcuk Acar. 2010. Do tests of divergent thinking have an experiential bias? Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts 4: 144–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Runco, Mark A., and Selcuk Acar. 2012. Divergent thinking as an indicator of creative potential. Creativity Research Journal 24: 66–75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schmitt, Manfred J., and Rolf Steyer. 1993. A latent state-trait model (not only) for social desirability. Personality and Individual Differences 14: 519–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Silvia, Paul J., Christopher Martin, and Emily C. Nusbaum. 2009. A snapshot of creativity: Evaluating a quick and simple method for assessing divergent thinking. Thinking Skills and Creativity 4: 79–85. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Steyer, Rolf. 1989. Models of classical psychometric test theory as stochastic measurement models: Representation, uniqueness, meaningfulness, identifiability, and testability. Methodika 3: 25–60. [Google Scholar]
- Stratton, R. Paul, and Robert Brown. 1972. Improving creative thinking by training in the production and/or judgment of solutions. Journal of Educational Psychology 63: 390–97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Vogt, Stine, and Svein Magnussen. 2007. Expertise in pictorial perception: Eye-movement patterns and visual memory in artists and laymen. Perception 36: 91–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wallach, Michael A., and Cliff W. Wing, Jr. 1969. The Talented Student: A Validation of the Creativity-Intelligence Distinction. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston. [Google Scholar]
- Wallach, Michael A., and Nathan Kogan. 1965. Modes of Thinking in Young Children: A Study of the Creativity-Intelligence Distinction. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston. [Google Scholar]
- West, Stephen G., Aaron B. Taylor, and Wei Wu. 2012. Model fit and model selection in structural equation modeling. In Handbook of Structural Equation Modeling. Edited by Rick H. Hoyle. New York: Guilford Press, pp. 209–31. [Google Scholar]
- Wilson, Robert C., Joy P. Guilford, and Paul R. Christensen. 1953. The measurement of individual differences in originality. Psychological Bulletin 50: 362–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zimmerman, Donald W. 1975. Probability spaces, Hilbert spaces, and the axioms of test theory. Psychometrika 40: 395–412. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Target | Rater | Y11 | Y21 | Y31 | Y12 | Y22 | Y32 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1 | 1 | 3 | 3 | 2 | NA | NA | NA |
1 | 2 | NA | NA | NA | 3.00 | 2.25 | 3.00 |
1 | 3 | 3 | 2 | 2 | NA | NA | NA |
1 | 4 | 4 | 3 | 3 | NA | NA | NA |
1 | 5 | 4 | 3 | 3 | NA | NA | NA |
1 | 6 | NA | NA | NA | 4.00 | 5.00 | 3.00 |
1 | 7 | NA | NA | NA | 2.00 | 3.00 | 3.00 |
2 | 1 | 2 | 4 | 3 | NA | NA | NA |
2 | 2 | NA | NA | NA | 3.00 | 3.50 | 1.75 |
2 | 3 | 2 | 4 | 2 | NA | NA | NA |
2 | 4 | 3 | 4 | 2 | NA | NA | NA |
2 | 5 | 3 | 4 | 2 | NA | NA | NA |
2 | 6 | NA | NA | NA | 4.00 | 4.50 | 2.75 |
2 | 7 | NA | NA | NA | 2.00 | 3.50 | 2.00 |
DTCC | DTTL-B | DTTL-ML | ||||||||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Parameter | Y11 | Y21 | Y31 | Y12 | Y22 | Y32 | Y11 | Y21 | Y31 | Y12 | Y22 | Y32 | Y11 | Y21 | Y31 | Y12 | Y22 | Y32 |
2.993 | 3.066 | 3.026 | 2.907 | 3.132 | 2.927 | 2.996 | 3.055 | 3.024 | 2.908 | 3.124 | 2.922 | 2.993 | 3.069 | 3.030 | 2.904 | 3.129 | 2.926 | |
1 | 0.489 | 0.498 | 1 | 0.540 | 0.459 | 1 | 0.510 | 0.514 | 1 | 0.531 | 0.461 | 1 | 0.521 | 0.508 | 1 | 0.598 | 0.497 | |
1 | 0.715 | 0.502 | 1 | 1.108 | 0.583 | - | - | - | - | - | - | - | - | - | - | - | - | |
1 | 0.997 | 0.908 | 1 | 0.932 | 1.004 | 1 | 0.917 | 0.762 | 1 | 1.114 | 0.596 | 1 | 0.775 | 0.638 | 1 | 1.163 | 0.613 | |
0.324 | 0.261 | 0.273 | 0.158 | 0.209 | 0.188 | 0.346 | 0.257 | 0.274 | 0.183 | 0.207 | 0.208 | 0.321 | 0.262 | 0.275 | 0.208 | 0.195 | 0.209 | |
0.588 | 0.223 | 0.564 | 0.195 | 0.579 | 0.156 | |||||||||||||
0.468 | 0.429 | 0.210 | 0.182 | 0.469 | 0.424 | 0.207 | 0.180 | 0.456 | 0.426 | 0.170 | 0.137 | |||||||
0.055 | 0.328 | - | - | - | - | |||||||||||||
0.047 | 0.004 | 0.084 | 0.253 | 0.109 | 0.240 | |||||||||||||
0.316 (.876) | 0.310 (.941) | 0.298 (.994) | ||||||||||||||||
0.187 (.420) | 0.182 (.411) | 0.181 (.410) | ||||||||||||||||
0.285 (.912) | 0.282 (.908) | 0.162 (.942) | ||||||||||||||||
0.075 (.260) | 0.079 (.274) | 0.058 (.232) | ||||||||||||||||
0.073 (.245) | 0.066 (.224) | 0.054 (.200) * | ||||||||||||||||
0.251 (.901) | 0.251 (.913) | 0.231 (.958) | ||||||||||||||||
0.051 (.263) | 0.049 (.259) | 0.007 (.045) * | ||||||||||||||||
L2Conij | .231 | .254 | .237 | .205 | .238 | .259 | .210 | .188 | .256 | .259 | .247 | .219 | ||||||
L2OMSij | .769 | .746 | .763 | .795 | .762 | .741 | .790 | .812 | .744 | .741 | .753 | .781 | ||||||
L1Conij | .201 | .228 | .091 | .126 | .213 | .238 | .095 | .133 | .232 | .241 | .101 | .145 | ||||||
L1OMSij | .672 | .674 | .306 | .520 | .681 | .680 | .357 | .575 | .672 | .688 | .310 | .515 | ||||||
MIICCij | .846 | .885 | .911 | .401 | .405 | .663 | .870 | .897 | .921 | .435 | .457 | .714 | .842 | .904 | .928 | .394 | .411 | .660 |
RMSij | .080 | .041 | .022 | .590 | .589 | .321 | - | - | - | - | - | - | - | - | - | - | - | - |
IMSij | .065 | .065 | .060 | .006 | .004 | .009 | - | - | - | - | - | - | - | - | - | - | - | - |
UMSij | .154 | .115 | .089 | .599 | .595 | .337 | .130 | .103 | .079 | .565 | .543 | .286 | .158 | .096 | .072 | .606 | .589 | .340 |
RELij | .688 | .730 | .701 | .780 | .767 | .655 | .654 | .730 | .697 | .710 | .738 | .602 | .680 | .721 | .693 | .655 | .738 | .559 |
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2024 by the author. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Jendryczko, D. Decomposing the True Score Variance in Rated Responses to Divergent Thinking-Tasks for Assessing Creativity: A Multitrait–Multimethod Analysis. J. Intell. 2024, 12, 95. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence12100095
Jendryczko D. Decomposing the True Score Variance in Rated Responses to Divergent Thinking-Tasks for Assessing Creativity: A Multitrait–Multimethod Analysis. Journal of Intelligence. 2024; 12(10):95. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence12100095
Chicago/Turabian StyleJendryczko, David. 2024. "Decomposing the True Score Variance in Rated Responses to Divergent Thinking-Tasks for Assessing Creativity: A Multitrait–Multimethod Analysis" Journal of Intelligence 12, no. 10: 95. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence12100095
APA StyleJendryczko, D. (2024). Decomposing the True Score Variance in Rated Responses to Divergent Thinking-Tasks for Assessing Creativity: A Multitrait–Multimethod Analysis. Journal of Intelligence, 12(10), 95. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence12100095