Judgments of Learning Reactivity on Item-Specific and Relational Processing
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Participants
2.2. Materials
2.3. Procedure
3. Results
3.1. JOL Results
3.2. Recall Results
3.3. Model Results
3.4. Follow-Up Analysis
4. Discussion
5. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Conflicts of Interest
Appendix A
Appendix B
Categorical Label | A Natural Earth Formation | A Vegetable | A Four-Footed Animal | A Part of a Building | A Musical Instrument |
List words | valley | potato | tiger | office | drum |
river | squash | horse | stairs | guitar | |
canyon | pepper | rabbit | lobby | flute | |
volcano | lettuce | giraffe | ceiling | piano | |
ocean | radish | elephant | window | trumpet | |
cliff | carrot | moose | elevator | clarinet | |
island | tomato | squirrel | basement | violin | |
stream | cabbage | raccoon | floor | cello |
List Label | List 1 | List 2 | List 3 | List 4 | List 5 |
List words | lettuce | squash | valley | drum | ocean |
river | rabbit | giraffe | tomato | tiger | |
trumpet | pepper | guitar | basement | radish | |
ceiling | elevator | flute | squirrel | office | |
moose | elephant | carrot | cabbage | cello | |
canyon | piano | volcano | horse | stream | |
raccoon | lobby | island | floor | clarinet | |
stairs | potato | violin | cliff | window |
References
- Ackerman, Brian P. 1986. The use of item-specific and relational episodic information in the recall of children and adults. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 42: 115–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Brainerd, Charles J., and Valerie F. Reyna. 1998. Fuzzy-trace theory and children’s false memories. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 71: 81–129. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Brainerd, Charles J., Valerie F. Reyna, and Mark L. Howe. 2009. Trichotomous processes in early memory development, aging, and neurocognitive impairment: A unified theory. Psychological Review 116: 783–832. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Chang, Minyu, and Charles J. Brainerd. 2023. Changed-goal or cue-strengthening? Examining the reactivity of judgments of learning with the dual-retrieval model. Metacognition and Learning 18: 183–217. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Deese, James. 1959. On the prediction of occurrence of particular verbal intrusions in immediate recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology 58: 17–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Double, Kit S., and Damian P. Birney. 2019. Reactivity to measures of metacognition. Frontiers in Psychology 10: 2755. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Double, Kit S., Damian P. Birney, and Sarah A. Walker. 2018. A meta-analysis and systematic review of reactivity to judgements of learning. Memory 26: 741–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dunlosky, John, and Robert Ariel. 2011. Self-regulated learning and the allocation of study time. In Psychology of Learning and Motivation. Edited by Brian H. Ross. Cambridge: Academic Press, vol. 54, pp. 103–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Einstein, Gilles O., and Reed R. Hunt. 1980. Levels of processing and organization: Additive effects of individual-item and relational processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory 6: 588–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Faul, Franz, Edgar Erdfelder, Albert-Georg Lang, and Axel Buchner. 2007. G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods 39: 175–91. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Halamish, Vared, and Monika Undorf. 2022. Why do judgments of learning modify memory? Evidence from identical pairs and relatedness judgments. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 49: 547–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Höhne, Jan K., and Stephan Schlosser. 2018. Investigating the adequacy of response time outlier definitions in computer-based web surveys using paradata SurveyFocus. Social Science Computer Review 36: 369–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hunt, Reed R., and Gilles O. Einstein. 1981. Relational and item-specific information in memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 20: 497–514. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Janes, Jessica L., Michelle L. Rivers, and John Dunlosky. 2018. The influence of making judgments of learning on memory performance: Positive, negative, or both? Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 25: 2356–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jenkins, James J. 1979. Four points to remember: A tetrahedral model of memory experiments. In Levels of Processing in Human Memory. Edited by Laird S. Cermak and Fergus I. M. Craik. Hillsdale: Erlbaum Associates, pp. 429–46. [Google Scholar]
- Kornell, Nate, and Robert A. Bjork. 2008. Optimising self-regulated study: The benefits—and costs—of dropping flashcards. Memory 16: 125–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Maxwell, Nicholas P., and Mark J. Huff. 2023. Is discriminability a requirement for reactivity? Comparing the effects of mixed vs. pure list presentations on judgment of learning reactivity. Memory & Cognition 51: 1198–213. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Metcalfe, Janet, and Bridgid Finn. 2008. Evidence that judgments of learning are causally related to study choice. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 15: 174–79. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mitchum, Ainsley L., Colleen M. Kelley, and Mark C. Fox. 2016. When asking the question changes the ultimate answer: Metamemory judgments change memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 145: 200–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Murphy, Dillon H., Vered Halamish, Matthew G. Rhodes, and Alan D. Castel. 2023. How evaluating memorability can lead to unintended consequences. Metacognition and Learning 18: 375–403. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Myers, Sarah J., Matthew G. Rhodes, and Hannah E. Hausman. 2020. Judgments of learning (JOLs) selectively improve memory depending on the type of test. Memory & Cognition 48: 745–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nelson, Thomas O., and John Dunlosky. 1992. How shall we explain the delayed-judgment-of-learning effect? Psychological Science 3: 317–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Reyna, Valerie F., and Charles J. Brainerd. 1995. Fuzzy-trace theory: An interim synthesis. Learning and Individual Differences 7: 1–75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rivers, Michelle L., Jessica L. Janes, and John Dunlosky. 2021. Investigating memory reactivity with a within-participant manipulation of judgments of learning: Support for the cue-strengthening hypothesis. Memory 29: 1342–53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Rivers, Michelle L., John Dunlosky, Jessica Janes, Amber Witherby, and Sarah Tauber. 2023. Judgments of learning enhance recall for category-cued but not letter-cued items. Memory & Cognition 51: 1547–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Roediger, Henry L., and Kathleen B. McDermott. 1995. Creating false memories: Remembering words not presented in lists. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 21: 803–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Roenker, Daniel L., Charles P. Thompson, and Sam C. Brown. 1971. Comparison of measures for the estimation of clustering in free recall. Psychological Bulletin 76: 45–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schäfer, Franziska, and Monika Undorf. 2021. Positive and negative reactivity in judgments of learning: Shared or distinct mechanisms? Paper presented at 63rd Conference of Experimental Psychologists, Ulm, Germany, March 14–16. [Google Scholar]
- Senkova, Olesya, and Hajime Otani. 2021. Making judgments of learning enhances memory by inducing item-specific processing. Memory & Cognition 49: 955–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Shi, Aike, Chenyuqi Xu, Wenbo Zhao, David R. Shanks, Xiao Hu, Liang Luo, and Chunlin Yang. 2023. Judgments of learning reactively facilitate visual memory by enhancing learning engagement. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 30: 676–87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Soderstrom, Nicholas C., Colin T. Clark, Vered Halamish, and Elizabeth L. Bjork. 2015. Judgments of learning as memory modifiers. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 41: 553–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Spellman, Barbara A., and Robert A. Bjork. 1992. When predictions create reeality: Judgments of learning may alter what they are Intended to assess. Psychological Science 3: 315–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Stevens, Amanda. 2019. Examining the Effects of Making Judgments of Learning on True and False Memory. Master’s thesis, Texas A & M University, College Station, TX, USA. [Google Scholar]
- Stevens, Amanda S., and Bento H. Pierce. 2019. Do reactive effects of judgments of learning extend to words lists? Paper presented at 2019 Annual Meeting of the Psychonomic Society, Montreal, QC, Canada, November 14–17. [Google Scholar]
- Tauber, Sarah K., and Amber E. Witherby. 2019. Do judgments of learning modify older adults’ actual learning? Psychology and Aging 34: 836–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Tekin, Eylul, and Henry L. Roediger. 2020. Reactivity of judgments of learning in a levels-of-processing paradigm. Zeitschrift für Psychologie 228: 278–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Van Overschelde, James P., Katherine A. Rawson, and John Dunlosky. 2004. Category norms: An updated and expanded version of the Battig and Montague 1969 norms. Journal of Memory and Language 50: 289–335. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Witherby, Amber E., and Sarah K. Tauber. 2017. The influence of judgments of learning on long-term learning and short-term performance. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition 6: 496–503. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zhao, Wen B., Jiaojiao Li, David R. Shanks, Baike Li, Xiao Hu, Chunlin Yang, and Liang Luo. 2022. Metamemory judgments have dissociable reactivity effects on item and inter-item relational memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 49: 557–74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zhao, Wenbo B., Yue Yin, Xiao Hu, David R. Shanks, Chunlin Yang, and Liang Luo. 2023. Memory for inter-item relations is reactively disrupted by metamemory judgments. Metacognition and Learning 18: 549–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Theory | Explanations | Major Studies Supporting the Theories |
---|---|---|
Changed-goal hypothesis | Making JOLs heightens participants’ awareness of item differences in learning difficulty, thus prompting them to emphasize easier items (e.g., related word pairs) at the expense of harder items (e.g., unrelated word pairs). | (Janes et al. 2018; Mitchum et al. 2016; Schäfer and Undorf 2021) |
Cue-strengthening hypothesis | Making JOLs strengthens the cues that inform JOLs (e.g., cue-target relatedness in related word pairs), and thus produces memory benefits when future memory tests are sensitive to those cues. | (Chang and Brainerd 2023; Halamish and Undorf 2022; Maxwell and Huff 2023; Myers et al. 2020; Rivers et al. 2021, 2023; Soderstrom et al. 2015; Tekin and Roediger 2020; Witherby and Tauber 2017) |
Item-specific processing hypothesis | Making JOLs promotes item-specific processing and thus enhances the distinctiveness of individual items. | (Senkova and Otani 2021; Zhao et al. 2022, 2023) |
Attention-reorienting account/enhanced engagement account | Making JOLs enhances the attention to the study items and increases learners’ engagement in processing the items. | (Shi et al. 2023; Tauber and Witherby 2019; Murphy et al. 2023) |
Parameters | Definitions |
---|---|
D | Direct access/recollection: the probability that the verbatim traces of an item’s prior presentation can be retrieved on a recall test. |
F | Forgetting of direct access: the probability that direct access to an item’s verbatim traces is lost after the first or second recall test due to forgetting. |
R | Reconstruction: the probability that when direct access fails, an item can be reconstructed on a recall test using gist traces (e.g., inter-item semantic relation). |
J1, J2, J3 | Familiarity judgment: The probability that a reconstructed item is judged to be familiar enough to output. J1, J2, and J3 represent the familiarity judgments for the first, second, and third recall tests, respectively. |
List Organization | JOL Condition | G2 | D | F | J1 | J2 | J3 | R |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Blocked | ||||||||
Item-JOL | 0.00 | 0.42 | 0.05 | 0.40 | 0.52 | 0.79 | 0.19 | |
List-JOL | 13.91 | 0.42 | 0.10 | 0.50 | 0.69 | 0.78 | 0.20 | |
No-JOL | 2.69 | 0.40 | 0.05 | 0.42 | 0.62 | 0.84 | 0.20 | |
Randomized | ||||||||
Item-JOL | 0.31 | 0.37 | 0.06 | 0.54 | 0.64 | 0.84 | 0.20 | |
List-JOL | 4.34 | 0.25 | 0.09 | 0.71 | 0.74 | 0.91 | 0.10 | |
No-JOL | 0.11 | 0.33 | 0.06 | 0.34 | 0.58 | 0.84 | 0.15 |
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2024 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Chang, M.; Brainerd, C. Judgments of Learning Reactivity on Item-Specific and Relational Processing. J. Intell. 2024, 12, 4. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence12010004
Chang M, Brainerd C. Judgments of Learning Reactivity on Item-Specific and Relational Processing. Journal of Intelligence. 2024; 12(1):4. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence12010004
Chicago/Turabian StyleChang, Minyu, and Charles Brainerd. 2024. "Judgments of Learning Reactivity on Item-Specific and Relational Processing" Journal of Intelligence 12, no. 1: 4. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence12010004
APA StyleChang, M., & Brainerd, C. (2024). Judgments of Learning Reactivity on Item-Specific and Relational Processing. Journal of Intelligence, 12(1), 4. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence12010004