Next Article in Journal
The Role of Semantic Associations as a Metacognitive Cue in Creative Idea Generation
Previous Article in Journal
Measurement Efficiency of a Teacher Rating Scale to Screen for Students at Risk for Social, Emotional, and Behavioral Problems
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Childhood Maltreatment and Creativity among Chinese College Students: A Serial Mediation Model

1
School of Mental Health, Jining Medical University, Jining 272067, China
2
College of Chinese and Western Medicine, Jining Medical University, Jining 272067, China
3
Labor Union, Jining Medical University, Jining 272067, China
*
Authors to whom correspondence should be addressed.
These authors contributed equally to this work.
J. Intell. 2023, 11(4), 58; https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence11040058
Submission received: 18 December 2022 / Revised: 7 March 2023 / Accepted: 21 March 2023 / Published: 24 March 2023
(This article belongs to the Topic Understanding Scientific Creativity)

Abstract

:
Creativity plays a very crucial impact on our cultural life and has also been important to the improvement of human civilization. Numerous studies have indicated that family circumstance plays an important role in the development of individual creativity. However, little is known about the mediating mechanisms underlying the association between childhood maltreatment and creativity. This study intended to explore the serial multiple mediation model in which undergraduates’ cognitive flexibility and self-efficacy were proposed to mediate the potential influence of childhood maltreatment on their creativity. Participants were 1069 undergraduates (573 males and 496 females, mean age was 20.57 ± 1.24 years ranging from 17 to 24) from a university in Shandong Province, China. Participants were required to complete an internet survey including the Short Form of Childhood Trauma Questionnaire (CTQ-SF), General Self-Efficacy Scale (GSES), Cognitive Flexibility Inventory (CFI), and Williams Creativity Aptitude Test (WCAT). Serial multiple mediation analysis and the bootstrap method were used to investigate the mediation effects of cognitive flexibility and self-efficacy. The results showed that childhood maltreatment indirectly influenced undergraduates’ creativity through three indirect paths: childhood maltreatment→cognitive flexibility→creativity, childhood maltreatment→self-efficacy→creativity, and childhood maltreatment→cognitive flexibility→self-efficacy→creativity. The ratios of the total indirect effects and branch-indirect effects to the total effects were 92.73%, 34.61%, 35.68%, and 22.44%, respectively. These results indicated that cognitive flexibility and self-efficacy could completely mediate the potential impact of childhood maltreatment on individuals creativity.

1. Introduction

Creativity plays a very critical impact on our cultural life and has also been crucial to the improvement of human civilization (Takeuchi et al. 2010). Furnham and Bachtiar (2008) proposed the idea of creativity, which was the ability that was designed to generate a product or idea that was characteristic of novel and useful. Given the crucial role of creativity, a great number of studies have been conducted to investigate the possible mechanisms that are conductive to creativity. Numerous studies have found that environment factors are one of the most important variables to influence creativity (Ma 2009; Onarheim and Friis-Olivarius 2013). As one of the early exposed environments, family circumstances played an important role in the development of individual creativity. Various cognitive functions such as creativity were influenced by the family environment and parenting style (Chan 2005; Si et al. 2020). If there is an association between family cultivating and creativity, one can speculate that childhood maltreatment has an important effect on an individual’s capacity to achieve excellent creative accomplishment. However, the relationship between childhood maltreatment and individual creativity has not been clarified so far. This study aimed to explore the relationship between childhood maltreatment and creativity and further investigate the mediation mechanism of cognitive flexibility and self-efficacy.

1.1. Childhood Maltreatment and Creativity

Childhood maltreatment, defined as adverse events including physical abuse, emotional abuse, sexual abuse, physical neglect, and emotional neglect against children occurring prior to the age of 18 years, was one of the risk factors for potential or actual hurt to mental and physical health (Bernstein et al. 2003). Childhood abuse and neglect were serious problems that related to public health, legal issues, and society harmony, and caused considerable adverse influences on the people’s physiological and psychological growth (Capusan et al. 2021; Liu et al. 2022). They were also common and severe issues among Chinese university students (Fu et al. 2018). Numerous studies have documented a significant relationship between childhood maltreatment and mental problems, as well as behaviors problems (Kadivari et al. 2022; Lamela and Figueiredo 2018; McRae et al. 2022).
Childhood maltreatment not only predicted crime, domestic violence, and antisocial personality (Ducci et al. 2008; Fox et al. 2015; Pournaghash-Tehrani and Feizabadi 2009), but also influenced executive function, cognitive flexibility, working memory, and openness to experience (Bernardes et al. 2020; Chiasson et al. 2021; Fletcher and Schurer 2017; Spann et al. 2012), which were related with creativity (Benedek et al. 2014; Sun et al. 2018; Wu and Koutstaal 2020; Xing et al. 2019). Recent research indicated that childhood adverse experience, such as low family socioeconomic status and poor parent–child relationships, was negatively related with social creativity (Zhang et al. 2018). Other studies also found that creative individuals often had a warmer childhood environment and harmonious family (Chan 2005; Zaeske et al. 2022). A recent study indicated that parental warmth and parental rejection were positively related with creativity (Guo et al. 2021). Additionally, two recent studies have found a positive relationship between childhood maltreatment and malevolent creativity (Jia et al. 2020; Li et al. 2022), which is defined as the ability to generate creative ideas to harm others intentionally (Cropley et al. 2014; Gill et al. 2013; Hao et al. 2016). For example, Jia et al. (2020) demonstrated that childhood neglect and Dark Triad personality traits were positively associated with malevolent creativity. Another study further found that childhood maltreatment and aggression were positively related with malevolent creativity (Li et al. 2022). However, it was also indicated that maltreated individuals developed intact, or stress-adapted skills for solving problems in adverse situations (Ellis et al. 2022). Therefore, based on these close relationships between adverse experience in early life and social creativity, as well as malevolent creativity, it is meaningful to investigate whether and how childhood maltreatment influences individual creativity.

1.2. Cognitive Flexibility as a Mediator

Cognitive flexibility, one of many crucial factors influencing individual creativity (Guilford 1967), can be defined as the capacity to switch thinking sets to adapt to the changing environmental stimulus (Dennis and Vander Wal 2010). Cognitive flexibility theory proposed by Spiro and Jehng (1990) defined cognitive flexibility as “the ability to spontaneously restructure one’s knowledge, in many ways, in adaptive response to radically changing situational demands…” (p. 165). From a general view, the findings of studies focusing on cognitive control, attention, associative process, executive function, prefrontal cortex function, and Rapid Eye Movement (REM) sleep (Beaty et al. 2014; Dietrich 2004; Dorfman et al. 2008; Stickgold and Walker 2004; Zabelina and Robinson 2010) collectively suggested that creativity was associated with cognitive flexibility. Additionally, high creative participants, compared with less creative participants, performed well in the levels of flexible cognitive control (Zabelina and Robinson 2010). Recent researches also found that cognitive flexibility was positively correlated with creative problem-solving (Wu and Koutstaal 2020; Zuo et al. 2019). Therefore, we hypothesize that cognitive flexibility positively predict individuals creativity.
A burgeoning study about exposure during early life to childhood maltreatment showed that, in addition to mental or behaviors problems (Kadivari et al. 2022; Lamela and Figueiredo 2018; McRae et al. 2022), childhood maltreatment was related to a range of cognitive function impairments and psychological disorders (Su et al. 2019; Xiao et al. 2021). Associations between maltreatment experience and executive function have been certified consistently in adolescents and adults (Letkiewicz et al. 2021; Mothes et al. 2015). For example, Spann et al. (2012) found that both physical abuse and neglect were related to a reduction in cognitive flexibility in 12–17-years old adolescents. Similarly, Kalia and Knauft (2020) further found childhood adversity was correlated with decreased cognitive flexibility in adulthood. Thus, it is reasonable to hypothesize that childhood maltreatment negatively predicts cognitive flexibility.
Given the close relationship between childhood maltreatment and cognitive flexibility (Kalia and Knauft 2020; Spann et al. 2012), and cognitive flexibility and creativity (Wu and Koutstaal 2020; Zuo et al. 2019), cognitive flexibility might be considered to be an important variable in the hypothesized connection between childhood maltreatment and individual creativity. The Dual Pathway to Creativity Model (DPCM) proposed that dispositional or situational factors might affect creativity through cognitive flexibility or cognitive persistence pathway, and any characteristics or state that reduced either cognitive flexibility or cognitive persistence would hinder individuals creativity (Baas et al. 2013). Indeed, empirical research indicated that cognitive flexibility mediated the link between early life maltreatment and psychological characteristic (Kalia et al. 2020). Therefore, it is reasonable that the relationship between childhood maltreatment and creativity might be mediated by the effect of cognitive flexibility.

1.3. Self-Efficacy and Creativity

Self-efficacy was the ability to judge about whether or not one has the capability to complete certain types of performance that were related to the individual (Puente-Díaz 2016). The social cognitive theory putted forward that self-efficacy played an important motivational role in creative thinking (Bandura 1997). Previous empirical studies indicated that self-efficacy influenced individual creativity positively (Daher et al. 2021). For instance, Tamannaeifar and Motaghedifard (2014) found that self-efficacy had a significant positive influence on the creativity of university students. Other research also emphasized that self-efficacy was a crucial predictor of creativity (Waheed and Dastgeer 2020). In a recent meta-analysis research, Haase et al. (2018) synthesized 60 effect sizes from 41 articles, which explored the association between self-efficacy and creativity, and revealed that self-efficacy was positively correlated with multifaceted creativity measures, such as self-rated creativity and divergent thinking. Thus, the present research proposes that self-efficacy positively correlates with self-rated creativity.
Other research also found a similar relationship between childhood maltreatment and self-efficacy. For instance, Lu et al. (2017) investigated the association among childhood maltreatment, self-efficacy, and abstinence motivation, and found that childhood maltreatment was negatively correlated with self-efficacy. Additionally, Soffer et al. (2008) found that childhood emotional neglect was associated with the lower level of self-efficacy in undergraduates. These findings indicated that childhood maltreatment was an important negative predictor of both cognitive flexibility and self-efficacy.
Meanwhile, social cognitive theory (Bandura 1997) suggested that the successful experience was a major source of self-efficacy. However, individuals who experienced childhood abuse or neglect had more denial from caregiver and failure experience, and this may have led to low levels of self-efficacy (Adjorlolo et al. 2017). Therefore, based on the association between childhood maltreatment and self-efficacy (Lu et al. 2017; Soffer et al. 2008), and self-efficacy and creativity (Daher et al. 2021; Waheed and Dastgeer 2020), we hypothesize that self-efficacy might mediate the influence of childhood maltreatment on creativity.

1.4. A Serial Multiple Mediator Model

Furthermore, people who have a high level of cognitive flexibility confront difficulties and are more flexible with what they could do with these troubles and more confident in themselves; people who have a low level of cognitive flexibility become overwhelmed by not knowing what to do and are less confident about their capability. It is essential for individuals to be flexible and to handle changes and difficulties quickly. An empirical study revealed that cognitive flexibility was positively related to self-efficacy (Esen et al. 2017). Bandura, who proposed the self-efficacy was the portion of cognitive flexibility, pointed out that the more cognitive flexibility people have, the higher their level of self-efficacy (Esen et al. 2017). Other studies also revealed that there was a meaningful association between cognitive flexibility and self-efficacy (Demirtaş 2020). Accordingly, the previous findings indicated that the more cognitive flexibility, the more self-efficacy (Esen et al. 2017). Therefore, we hypothesize that childhood maltreatment might influence self-efficacy through the mediated role of cognitive flexibility.
Additionally, given the potential mediation effect of cognitive flexibility and self-efficacy between childhood maltreatment and creativity, we assume that childhood maltreatment might influence creativity through the serial multiple mediation effects of cognitive flexibility and self-efficacy.

1.5. The Current Study

The above discussions indicated obviously that childhood maltreatment, cognitive flexibility, self-efficacy, and individuals creativity were correlated with each other. In other words, the current study intended to investigate the serial multiple mediation roles of cognitive flexibility and self-efficacy between the influence of childhood maltreatment on creativity. Based on the research reviewed above, the present study was conducted to investigate the potential patterns in (1) the influence of childhood maltreatment on creativity; (2) the mediating role of cognitive flexibility and self-efficacy in the association between childhood maltreatment and creativity respectively; (3) the serial multiple mediation effects of cognitive flexibility and self-efficacy in the relationship between childhood maltreatment and individual creativity.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants

This study enrolled 1120 undergraduates from universities in Shandong Province, China. All participants volunteered for the online questionnaire survey on the website www.wjx.cn. All the questionnaires used in the present research were revised or established using a standard procedure. All the items were in a clear and easily understandable Chinese version. It takes about five minutes to complete the survey. The data of 19 participants who completed the questionnaires with simply repeated answers or fixed patterns (e.g., participants completed a four-point Likert scale with 1, 2, 3, and 4 in turn), and 32 participants whose reaction time was beyond three standard deviations, were omitted from further analysis. Finally, the present study included 1069 participants (573 males and 496 females, mean age was 20.57 ± 1.24 years ranging from 17 to 24), including birthplace in cities (n = 699, 65.4%) and rural areas (n = 370, 34.6%), and only child (n = 452, 42.3%) and not an only child (n = 617, 57.7%). The research program was approved by the local Ethics Committee.

2.2. Measures

2.2.1. Short Form of Childhood Trauma Questionnaire (CTQ-SF)

The Chinese version (Zhao et al. 2005) of CTQ-SF (Bernstein et al. 2003) was used, which consisted of 28 items (included 25 clinical items and 3 validity items) and assessed childhood abuse and neglect histories that happened before 18 years of age. Each item asked about maltreatment experience in childhood and adolescence and was graded on a five-point Likert scale with five answer options ranging from Never to Always. The CTQ-RS has five clinical factors: physical, emotional, and sexual abuse, and physical and emotional neglect. Each type of factor was represented by five items. The sample items and the definitions of abuse and neglect can be found in previous researches (Bernstein et al. 1994; Bernstein et al. 2003). The score of CTQ-SF was equal to the sum of 25 clinical items. The higher the score of CTQ-SF, the more serious the childhood maltreatment was. This Chinese version has satisfactory reliability and validity which have been empirically derived (Zhao et al. 2005). The Cronbach’s alpha coefficient in the present study was 0.920.

2.2.2. General Self-Efficacy Scale (GSES)

The Chinese version of the GSES was used in the present research, which was adapted by Zhang and Schwarzer (1995). The GSES consisted of 10 items and measured the participants’ beliefs or expectation about the capacity to finish tasks on their own. Each item was rated on a four-point Likert-type scale with four reaction options ranging from Never True to Very Often True. The score of GSES was equivalent to the sum of all 10 items. The higher the score of GSES, the stronger the ability of self-efficacy was. This scale has high reliability and validity (Zhang and Schwarzer 1995). The Cronbach’s alpha coefficient was 0.835.

2.2.3. Cognitive Flexibility Inventory (CFI)

The Chinese version (Wang et al. 2016) of the CFI (Dennis and Vander Wal 2010) was used to evaluate the ability of cognitive flexibility to successfully overcome or displace inappropriate thoughts with more appropriate and concordant thinking. This scale consisted of 20 self-report items that were rated on a five-point Likert-type scale with five forced-choice options ranging from Never to Very Often. The score of CFI was calculated with the sum of numerical response values. Higher scores implied the greater cognitive flexibility, while lower scores represented the greater cognitive rigidity (Dennis and Vander Wal 2010). The reliability and validity with the Chinese participants of this questionnaire were satisfactory. The Cronbach’s alpha coefficient of CFI was 0.885.

2.2.4. Williams Creativity Aptitude Test (WCAT)

The Chinese version developed by Lin and Wang (1994) was utilized to measure creativity. This test included 50 self-assessment items. Participants were required to rate on a three-point Likert scale ranging from totally disagree to totally agree. The score of WCAT was the sum of numerical response values of 50 items (eight items were required reverse coding firstly). The higher score of WCAT represented the higher creative potential. The split half reliability and test–retest reliability of this Chinese version were satisfactory (Hwang et al. 2007). The Cronbach’s alpha coefficient of WCAT in the present research was 0.893.

2.3. Statistical Analysis

The SPSS and PROCESS V3.3 developed by Hayes (2013), a free-to-use macros for SPSS used to calculate the mediation and moderation effects, were utilized to analyze the data. Pearson correlation test was used to calculate the relationship between variables. Template model six of PROCESS V3.3, which tested the serial multiple mediators model, was used to compute the serial mediating effects of cognitive flexibility and self-efficacy. PROCESS V3.3 calculated the total effects, direct effects, and indirect effects with a 95% bootstrap confidence interval based on 5000 resamples. The p = 0.05 was used as statistically significant.

3. Results

3.1. Common Method Bias Test

We used Harman’s single factor test to examine the common method bias and imput all the items of CTQ-SF, GSES, CFI, and WCAT into the un-rotated exploratory factor analysis. The results revealed that 18 components with initial eigenvalues greater than 1 were extracted from the factorial analysis. The explanation rate of the component with maximum initial eigenvalue was 18.47%, which did not exceed the critical value of 40%. These results indicated that there was no obvious common method bias in the present research.

3.2. Descriptive Statistical Analysis

Descriptive statistical analysis results are shown in Table 1. Characterization of the Skewness and Kurtosis indicated that the score of childhood maltreatment, cognitive flexibility, self-efficacy, and creativity basically fitted the normal distribution (Hancock et al. 2010). Given the large sample size, the raw data were used in the following statistical analysis followed (Tabachnick and Fidell 2007).

3.3. Correlation Analysis

The results of the Pearson correlation analysis of variables are shown in Table 2. The results indicated that the score of CTQ-SF was negatively related to GSES, CFI, and WCAT respectively, while the scores of GSES, CFI, and WCAT were positively correlated with each other.

3.4. Test of the Serial Multiple Mediation Effects of Cognitive Flexibility and Self-Efficacy

The serial multiple mediation effects of cognitive flexibility and self-efficacy were analyzed using PROCESS V3.3 followed Hayes (2013). In the multiple regression analysis, the score of WCAT was the dependent variable and the score of CTQ-SF was the independent variable. The scores of CFI and GSES were the mediating variables. Gender, age, urban or rural areas, and singletons or non-singletons were the control variables to reduce the possible effects of the demographic variables on the mediation analysis. The unstandardized regression coefficient was calculated to reduce the type I errors caused by the data distribution. The significant level of path coefficients and 95% confidence interval were computed using multiple regression analysis and bootstrap method. The mediating effects were statistically significant, if the 95% bias-corrected bootstrap confidence interval did not contain zero (Hayes 2013). The standardized regression coefficients were calculated in the above model using the standardized data.
The results of serial multiple mediation analysis are shown in Table 3 and Figure 1. The results revealed that the total effect of childhood maltreatment on creativity, and the total indirect effects of cognitive flexibility and self-efficacy, was statistically significant, while the direct effect was not significant. Therefore, cognitive flexibility and self-efficacy played a complete mediating role between childhood maltreatment and creativity.
Furthermore, all three indirect effects paths were statistically significant based on the 95% bootstrap confidence interval. That is to say, the childhood maltreatment influenced creativity through three indirect paths: childhood maltreatment→cognitive flexibility→creativity, childhood maltreatment→self-efficacy→creativity, and childhood maltreatment→cognitive flexibility→self-efficacy→creativity. The total effect was −0.26 (t = −13.42, p < 0.001), while the direct effect was −0.02(t = −0.79, p = 0.43) and the total indirect effect was −0.24. The three branch-indirect effects were −0.09 (a1 × b1), −0.09 (a2 × b2), and −0.06 (a1 × a3 × b2), respectively. The ratios of total indirect effect and branch-indirect effects to total effect were 92.73%, 34.61%, 35.68%, and 22.44%, respectively.

4. Discussion

The present study intended to explore the potential mediating mechanisms underlying the relationship between childhood maltreatment and creativity. The principal finding was that childhood maltreatment, at least as reported retrospectively, was associated with self-reported creativity indirectly through the mediation effects of cognitive flexibility and self-efficacy, and their serial multiple mediating role. This result provided new psychological perspectives to minimize the negative influence of childhood maltreatment, strengthen cognitive flexibility and self-efficacy, and cultivate the creative thinking of college students.

4.1. The Relation between Childhood Maltreatment and Creativity

Consistent with that hypothesis, childhood maltreatment was found to be negatively correlated with self-report creativity among Chinese college students. In accordance with our result, other studies also indicated that early adverse experience was associated with different measures of creativity. For example, Zhang et al. (2018) investigated the influence of family socioeconomic conditions and parent–child relations on social creativity, and found that both socioeconomic conditions and parent–child relations were positively associated with social creativity. Gralewski and Jankowska (2020) further found that both parental child acceptance and autonomy were positively correlated with high school students’ creative ability and self-belief. The children who had experienced maltreatment usually have impaired executive function, lower cognitive flexibility, poor working memory, and low level of openness to experience (Bernardes et al. 2020; Chiasson et al. 2021; Fletcher and Schurer 2017; Spann et al. 2012). These cognitive functions and personality traits facilitated the development of individual creativity (Beaty et al. 2014; Miller and Tal 2007; Zabelina and Robinson 2010). The present result further indicated that childhood maltreatment has a strong negative influence on latter creativity.
Furthermore, the childhood maltreatment experience not only hindered the development of benevolent creativity, but also promoted the dark side of creativity, commonly called malevolent creativity (Cropley et al. 2014). The influence of malevolent creativity was mainly shown in the domain of terrorism and law-breaking conduct (Cropley and Cropley 2011; Gill et al. 2013). Interestingly, a recent study found that childhood neglect positively predicted the malevolent creativity of undergraduate students (Jia et al. 2020). Another study also found that childhood maltreatment and aggression were positively related with malevolent creativity (Li et al. 2022). Thus, individuals who have experienced more neglect or abuse in child’s period were likely to pursue more malevolent creativity and less benevolent creativity. Parents should be more attentive to their children’s needs and healthy growth to prevent malevolent creativity from happening. Notably, burgeoning studies on the exposure during childhood to harsh, unpredictable environments indicated that, in addition to a consequential lower level on a range of cognitive performance, such as intelligence, language, and executive functions (Duncan et al. 2017; Ursache and Noble 2016), childhood adversity was associated with some potential stress-adapted skills that can be leveraged for positive outcomes (Ellis et al. 2022). Thus, further scrutiny was needed to clarify the complex association between childhood maltreatment and creativity.

4.2. The Mediation Role of Cognitive Flexibility

Consistent with our hypothesis, childhood maltreatment predicted individual creativity indirectly through cognitive flexibility. Previous research indicated that childhood maltreatment was associated with a range of cognitive function impairments and psychological disorders (Su et al. 2019; Xiao et al. 2021). Poor executive function was always found in adults with a history of childhood maltreatment or trauma (Letkiewicz et al. 2021). Recent surveys found that physical abuse and neglect usually led to a reduction in cognitive flexibility measured by CFI (Kalia et al. 2020), and early life stress negatively related to cognitive flexibility (Zhou et al. 2020). Another empirical study also indicated that both physical abuse and neglect were related with diminished cognitive flexibility as measured by the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST) (Spann et al. 2012). Childhood adversity was associated with lower cognitive flexibility, as measured by the Dimensional Change Card Sort (Zelazo 2006), relative to controls in children who lived in foster care (Lewis-Morrarty et al. 2012). Other studies further found that individuals who were exposed to early stress were impaired in cognitive flexibility measured by task-switching (Harms et al. 2018).
Cognitive flexibility is an important factor to influence creativity (Guilford 1967). Many studies focusing on cognitive control, attention, associative process, executive function, prefrontal function and REM sleep (Beaty et al. 2014; Dietrich 2004; Dorfman et al. 2008; Stickgold and Walker 2004; Zabelina and Robinson 2010) consistently indicated that cognitive flexibility was related closely to creativity. Furthermore, other studies also found cognitive flexibility was positively correlated with the performance of creativity measured by multiple dimensions of creativity, such as divergent thinking, convergent thinking, Chinese idiom riddle tests, and poster design tasks (Wu and Koutstaal 2020; Zuo et al. 2019). The present result further revealed that the cognitive flexibility measured by CFI positively predicted the self-report creativity as measured by WCAT.
In line with the viewpoints of DPCM (Baas et al. 2013), childhood environmental factors influenced individual creativity through cognitive flexibility. Therefore, childhood maltreatment might cause lower cognitive flexibility. Consequently, poor cognitive flexibility further brings a low ability to shift thinking to adapt to the changing environmental stimulus, which might eventually lead to a reduction in creative ideas or innovations. That is, the effect of childhood maltreatment on creativity in college students might pertain through the mediation effect of cognitive flexibility.

4.3. The Mediation Role of Self-Efficacy

Additionally, childhood maltreatment also predicted individual creativity indirectly through self-efficacy. With higher self-efficacy, one has the belief that he/she has the ability to obtain a desired goal. Individuals who have experienced maltreatment or trauma are at increased risk for lack of the self-confidence that they can achieve remarkable success. Previous study indicated that undergraduates who experienced childhood emotional neglect generally formed the lower level of self-efficacy (Soffer et al. 2008). Similar results were also found in subsequent research (Lu et al. 2017). Social cognitive theory indicated that self-efficacy played an vital motivational effect in creativity performance (Bandura 1997). In addition, much empirical research showed that self-efficacy was associated positively with creativity. For example, a study on Grade 8-10 students found that self-efficacy was the first variable to explain the variance in creative emotion (Daher et al. 2021). Another paper enrolled 355 university students and revealed that there was a positive association between creativity and self-efficacy (Tamannaeifar and Motaghedifard 2014). The results of a meta-analysis research indicated that self-efficacy was positively related to self-rated creativity and divergent thinking (Haase et al. 2018). Thus, the earlier childhood maltreatment might lead to a low level of self-efficacy. Therewith, the lack of self-efficacy might contribute to doubt in one’s own abilities, which might finally lead to depressed creativity. It is probably fair to say that childhood maltreatment influenced creativity in college students through the mediation effect of self-efficacy.

4.4. The Serial Multiple Mediation Model

The present results showed that the association between childhood maltreatment and undergraduates’ creativity was a serial multiple mediation model. That is to say, earlier maltreatment experience indirectly influenced individual creativity through the effects of cognitive flexibility and self-efficacy. The above discussions indicated that childhood maltreatment was correlated with cognitive flexibility (Kalia and Knauft 2020; Spann et al. 2012) and self-efficacy was linked to creativity (Daher et al. 2021; Waheed and Dastgeer 2020). Bandura noted that the higher cognitive flexibility often brought the gradual accumulation of self-efficacy (Esen et al. 2017). Other empirical researches also indicated that there was a close relationship between cognitive flexibility and self-efficacy (Demirtaş 2020; Esen et al. 2017). Therefore, the earlier experience of maltreatment or trauma might induce the low level of cognitive flexibility. Consequently, the low level of cognitive flexibility might cause self-efficacy deficiency. Lastly, the insufficient self-efficacy might further lead to lack of creativity.

4.5. Limitations and Future Directions

There were some limitations of the present research that should be addressed. First, although the bootstrap process was used, the cross-sectional design and correlation analysis method could not clarify the causal relationship between childhood maltreatment and creativity and led to possible biased estimates of the parameters (Maxwell and Cole 2007). The serial multiple mediation model might need to be verified in further longitudinal research. Second, this research used a retrospective self-reported questionnaire to measure childhood maltreatment. There have been worries over whether participants could accurately report distal events occurring in childhood. Future research could take multiple forms of measures to assess earlier experience. Third, the present research only explored the relationship between childhood maltreatment and self-reported creativity and the mediation effects of cognitive flexibility and self-efficacy. Further research could clarify other potential factors that affect creativity, such as openness to experience (Takeuchi et al. 2012), intuition (Thomson and Jaque 2021) and schizotypal personality (Gibson et al. 2009), and other forms of creativity, for example, creative thinking and creative achievement.

5. Conclusions

In summary, although further validation and extension studies were needed, the present study was an important step forward in understanding how earlier maltreatment experience correlated with latter creativity. It indicated that cognitive flexibility and self-efficacy served as one potential mechanism by which childhood maltreatment was related to creativity. To better cultivate creative capacity and innovative talents, parents should give their children more love and care instead of neglect or abuse. Additionally, this research showed that the improvement of cognitive flexibility and self-efficacy would be more important in the future development of creativity performance.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, W.L., H.L. and C.L.; data curation, S.Z., H.L., K.Z., X.Z. and F.X.; formal analysis, W.L.; funding acquisition, W.L. and J.C.; investigation, S.Z., K.Z., X.Z. and F.X.; methodology, W.L. and S.Z.; project administration, W.L. and C.L.; resources, J.C.; supervision, H.L. and J.C.; writing—original draft, W.L.; writing—review and editing, W.L. and J.C. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This work was supported by the Natural Science Foundation of Shandong Province (ZR2022MC113); the Project of Shandong Province Higher Educational Youth Innovation Science and Technology Program (2019RWF003); the Supporting Fund for Teachers’ research of Jining Medical University (JYFC2019KJ011; JYFC2019RW007); the Special Project of Innovation Quality of Educational Sciences Planning of Shandong Province (2022CYB207); and the College Students Innovation and Entrepreneurship Training Program (cx2021142).

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study procedure was approved by the Institutional Review Board of Jining Medical University (JNMC-2021-YX-015).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all participants involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The data are available on request to the corresponding author.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Adjorlolo, Samuel, Sarah Adu-Poku, Johnny Andoh-Arthur, Irene Botchway, and Budeba Petro Mlyakado. 2017. Demographic factors, childhood maltreatment and psychological functioning among university students’ in Ghana: A retrospective study. International Journal of Psychology 52: 9–17. [Google Scholar]
  2. Baas, Matthijs, Marieke Roskes, Daniel Sligte, Bernard A. Nijstad, and Carsten K. W. De Dreu. 2013. Personality and Creativity: The Dual Pathway to Creativity Model and a Research Agenda. Social and Personality Psychology Compass 7: 732–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Bandura, Albert. 1997. Self-Efficacy: The Exercise of Control. New York: Freeman. [Google Scholar]
  4. Beaty, Roger E., Paul J. Silvia, Emily C. Nusbaum, Emanuel Jauk, and Mathias Benedek. 2014. The roles of associative and executive processes in creative cognition. Memory and Cognition 42: 1186–97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Benedek, Mathias, Emanuel Jauk, Markus Sommer, Martin Arendasy, and Aljoscha C. Neubauer. 2014. Intelligence, creativity, and cognitive control: The common and differential involvement of executive functions in intelligence and creativity. Intelligence 46: 73–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  6. Bernardes, Elisa Teixeira, Alicia Matijasevich Manitto, Eurípedes Constantino Miguel, Pedro Mario Pan, Marcelo Camargo Batistuzzo, Luis Augusto Rohde, and Guilherme V. Polanczyk. 2020. Relationships between childhood maltreatment, impairment in executive functions and disruptive behavior disorders in a community sample of children. European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 29: 969–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Bernstein, David P., Judith A. Stein, Michael D. Newcomb, Edward Walker, David Pogge, Taruna Ahluvalia, John Stokes, Leonard Handelsman, Martha Medrano, David Desmond, and et al. 2003. Development and validation of a brief screening version of the Childhood Trauma Questionnaire. Child Abuse and Neglect 27: 169–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Bernstein, David P., Laura Fink, Leonard Handelsman, Jeffrey Foote, Meg Lovejoy, Katherine Wenzel, Elizabeth Sapareto, and Ruggiero E. Joseph. 1994. Initial reliability and validity of a new retrospective measure of child abuse and neglect. The American Journal of Psychiatry 151: 1132–36. [Google Scholar]
  9. Capusan, Andrea J., Per A. Gustafsson, Ralf Kuja-Halkola, Kajsa Igelström, Leah M. Mayo, and Markus Heilig. 2021. Re-examining the link between childhood maltreatment and substance use disorder: A prospective, genetically informative study. Molecular Psychiatry 26: 3201–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Chan, David W. 2005. Self-Perceived Creativity, Family Hardiness, and Emotional Intelligence of Chinese Gifted Students in Hong Kong. Journal of Secondary Gifted Education 16: 47–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  11. Chiasson, Carley, Jessie Moorman, Elisa Romano, Michel Vezarov, Andrew Cameron, and Andra Smith. 2021. The influence of emotion on working memory: Exploratory fMRI findings among men with histories of childhood sexual abuse. Child Abuse & Neglect 122: 105340. [Google Scholar]
  12. Cropley, Arthur, and David Cropley. 2011. Creativity and Lawbreaking. Creativity Research Journal 23: 313–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Cropley, David H., James C. Kaufman, Arielle E. White, and Belinda A. Chiera. 2014. Layperson perceptions of malevolent creativity: The good, the bad, and the ambiguous. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts 8: 400–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Daher, Wajeeh, Faaiz Gierdien, and Ahlam Anabousy. 2021. Self-efficacy in creativity and curiosity as predicting creative emotions. Journal of Research and Advances in Mathematics Education 6: 86–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Demirtaş, Ayşe Sibel. 2020. Cognitive Flexibility and Mental Well-Being in Turkish Adolescents: The Mediating Role of Academic, Social and Emotional Self-Efficacy. Anales de Psicologia 36: 111–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  16. Dennis, John P., and Jillon S. Vander Wal. 2010. The Cognitive Flexibility Inventory: Instrument Development and Estimates of Reliability and Validity. Cognitive Therapy and Research 34: 241–53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Dietrich, Arne. 2004. The cognitive neuroscience of creativity. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 11: 1011–26. [Google Scholar]
  18. Dorfman, Leonid, Colin Martindale, Vera Gassimova, and Oshin Vartanian. 2008. Creativity and speed of information processing: A double dissociation involving elementary versus inhibitory cognitive tasks. Personality and Individual Differences 44: 1382–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Ducci, Francesca, M. A. Enoch, Chloe Hodgkinson, K. Xu, M. Catena, R. W. Robin, and David Goldman. 2008. Interaction between a functional MAOA locus and childhood sexual abuse predicts alcoholism and antisocial personality disorder in adult women. Molecular Psychiatry 13: 334–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  20. Duncan, Greg J., Katherine Magnuson, and Elizabeth Votruba-Drzal. 2017. Moving Beyond Correlations in Assessing the Consequences of Poverty. Annual Review of Psychology 68: 413–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  21. Ellis, Bruce J., Laura S. Abrams, Ann S. Masten, Robert J. Sternberg, Nim Tottenham, and Willem E. Frankenhuis. 2022. Hidden talents in harsh environments. Development and Psychopathology 34: 95–113. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Esen, Binnaz Kiran, H. Duygu Özcan, and Mehtap Sezgin. 2017. High School Students’ Cognitive Flexibility Is Predicted by Self-Efficacy and Achievement. European Journal of Education Studies 3: 143–51. [Google Scholar]
  23. Fletcher, Jason M., and Stefanie Schurer. 2017. Origins of Adulthood Personality: The Role of Adverse Childhood Experiences. The B.E. Journal of Economic Analysis & Policy 17: 20150212. [Google Scholar]
  24. Fox, Bryanna Hahn, Nicholas Perez, Elizabeth Cass, Michael T. Baglivio, and Nathan Epps. 2015. Trauma changes everything: Examining the relationship between adverse childhood experiences and serious, violent and chronic juvenile offenders. Child Abuse & Neglect 46: 163–73. [Google Scholar]
  25. Fu, Hanlin, Tiejian Feng, Jiabi Qin, Tingting Wang, Xiaobing Wu, Yumao Cai, Lina Lan, and Tubao Yang. 2018. Reported prevalence of childhood maltreatment among Chinese college students: A systematic review and meta-analysis. PLoS ONE 13: e0205808. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  26. Furnham, Adrian, and Velicia Bachtiar. 2008. Personality and intelligence as predictors of creativity. Personality and Individual Differences 45: 613–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Gibson, Crystal, Bradley S. Folley, and Sohee Park. 2009. Enhanced divergent thinking and creativity in musicians: A behavioral and near-infrared spectroscopy study. Brain and Cognition 69: 162–69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Gill, Paul, John Horgan, Samuel T. Hunter, and Lily D. Cushenbery. 2013. Malevolent Creativity in Terrorist Organizations. The Journal of Creative Behavior 47: 125–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  29. Gralewski, Jacek, and Dorota Maria Jankowska. 2020. Do parenting styles matter? Perceived dimensions of parenting styles, creative abilities and creative self-beliefs in adolescents. Thinking Skills and Creativity 38: 100709. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Guilford, Joy Paul. 1967. The Nature of Human Intelligence. New York: McGraw-Hill. [Google Scholar]
  31. Guo, Jiajun, Jing Zhang, and Weiguo Pang. 2021. Parental warmth, rejection, and creativity: The mediating roles of openness and dark personality traits. Personality and Individual Differences 168: 110369. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Haase, Jennifer, Eva V. Hoff, Paul H. P. Hanel, and Åse Innes-Ker. 2018. A Meta-Analysis of the Relation between Creative Self-Efficacy and Different Creativity Measurements. Creativity Research Journal 30: 1–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  33. Hancock, Gregory R., Ralph O. Mueller, and Laura M. Stapleton. 2010. The Reviewer’s Guide to Quantitative Methods in the Social Sciences, 1st ed. New York: Routledge. [Google Scholar]
  34. Hao, Ning, Mengying Tang, Jing Yang, Qifei Wang, and Mark A. Runco. 2016. A New Tool to Measure Malevolent Creativity: The Malevolent Creativity Behavior Scale. Frontiers in Psychology 7: 682. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  35. Harms, Madeline B., Katherine E. Shannon Bowen, Jamie L. Hanson, and Seth D. Pollak. 2018. Instrumental learning and cognitive flexibility processes are impaired in children exposed to early life stress. Developmental Science 21: e12596. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  36. Hayes, Andrew F. 2013. Introduction to Mediation, Moderation, and Conditional Process Analysis: A Regression-Based Approach. New York: The Guilford Press. [Google Scholar]
  37. Hwang, Wu-Yuin, Nian-Shing Chen, Jian-Jie Dung, and Yi-Lun Yang. 2007. Multiple representation skills and creativity effects on mathematical problem solving using a multimedia whiteboard system. Educational Technology and Society 10: 191. [Google Scholar]
  38. Jia, Xuji, Qingjin Wang, and Lin Lin. 2020. The Relationship Between Childhood Neglect and Malevolent Creativity: The Mediating Effect of the Dark Triad Personality. Frontiers in Psychology 11: 613695. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  39. Kadivari, Faranak, Mahmoud Najafi, and Vahid Khosravani. 2022. Childhood adversity affects symptomatology via behavioral inhibition in patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder. Current Psychology, in press. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Kalia, Vrinda, and Katherine Knauft. 2020. Emotion regulation strategies modulate the effect of adverse childhood experiences on perceived chronic stress with implications for cognitive flexibility. PLoS ONE 15: e0235412. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Kalia, Vrinda, Katherine Knauft, and Niki Hayatbini. 2020. Cognitive flexibility and perceived threat from COVID-19 mediate the relationship between childhood maltreatment and state anxiety. PLoS ONE 15: e0243881. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Lamela, Diogo, and Bárbara Figueiredo. 2018. Childhood physical maltreatment with physical injuries is associated with higher adult psychopathology symptoms. European Psychiatry: The Journal of the Association of European Psychiatrists 53: 1–6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Letkiewicz, Allison M., Carter J. Funkhouser, and Stewart A. Shankman. 2021. Childhood maltreatment predicts poorer executive functioning in adulthood beyond symptoms of internalizing psychopathology. Child Abuse & Neglect 118: 105140. [Google Scholar]
  44. Lewis-Morrarty, Erin, Mary Dozier, Kristin Bernard, Stephanie M. Terracciano, and Shannon V. Moore. 2012. Cognitive flexibility and theory of mind outcomes among foster children: Preschool follow-up results of a randomized clinical trial. Journal of Adolescent Health 51: S17–S22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  45. Li, Wenfu, Linghui Zhang, Zhilei Qin, Jingting Chen, and Chuanxin Liu. 2022. Childhood Trauma and Malevolent Creativity in Chinese College Students: Moderated Mediation by Psychological Resilience and Aggression. Journal of Intelligence 10: 97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  46. Lin, XingTai, and Murong Wang. 1994. The Creativity Assessment Packet. Taipei: Psychological Publishing. [Google Scholar]
  47. Liu, Jie, Bingbing Li, Mengsi Xu, Junlong Luo, and Xu Li. 2022. Effects of childhood maltreatment on prosocial behaviors among Chinese emerging adults: A mediated moderation model of psychological suzhi and gratitude. Children and Youth Services Review 132: 106334. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Lu, Feng-Ying, Si Wen, Gang Deng, and Yung-Lung Tang. 2017. Self-concept mediate the relationship between childhood maltreatment and abstinence motivation as well as self-efficacy among drug addicts. Addictive Behaviors 68: 52–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  49. Ma, Hsen-Hsing. 2009. The effect size of variables associated with creativity: A meta-analysis. Creativity Research Journal 21: 30–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Maxwell, Scott E., and David A. Cole. 2007. Bias in cross-sectional analyses of longitudinal mediation. Psychological Methods 12: 23–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  51. McRae, Elizabeth, Laura Stoppelbein, Sarah O’kelley, Shana Smith, and Paula Fite. 2022. Pathways to Suicidal Behavior in Children and Adolescents: Examination of Child Maltreatment and Post-Traumatic Symptoms. Journal of Child & Adolescent Trauma 15: 715–725. [Google Scholar]
  52. Miller, Geoffrey F., and Ilanit R. Tal. 2007. Schizotypy versus openness and intelligence as predictors of creativity. Schizophrenia Research 93: 317–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Mothes, Luiza, Christian Haag Kristensen, Rodrigo Grassi-Oliveira, Rochele Paz Fonseca, Irani Iracema De Lima Argimon, and Tatiana Quarti Irigaray. 2015. Childhood maltreatment and executive functions in adolescents. Child and Adolescent Mental Health 20: 56–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  54. Onarheim, Balder, and Morten Friis-Olivarius. 2013. Applying the neuroscience of creativity to creativity training. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 7: 656. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  55. Pournaghash-Tehrani, Said, and Zahra Feizabadi. 2009. Predictability of Physical and Psychological Violence by Early Adverse Childhood Experiences. Journal of Family Violence 24: 417–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Puente-Díaz, Rogelio. 2016. Creative Self-Efficacy: An Exploration of Its Antecedents, Consequences, and Applied Implications. The Journal of Psychology 150: 175–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Si, Si, Yukang Su, Shun Zhang, and Jinghuan Zhang. 2020. Genetic susceptibility to parenting style: DRD2 and COMT influence creativity. NeuroImage 213: 116681. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  58. Soffer, Nirit, Eva Gilboa-Schechtman, and Golan Shahar. 2008. The relationship of childhood emotional abuse and neglect to depressive vulnerability and low self-efficacy. International Journal of Cognitive Therapy 1: 151–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Spann, Marisa N., Linda C. Mayes, Jessica H. Kalmar, Joanne Guiney, Fay Y. Womer, Brian Pittman, Carolyn M. Mazure, Rajita Sinha, and Hilary P. Blumberg. 2012. Childhood abuse and neglect and cognitive flexibility in adolescents. Child Neuropsychology 18: 182–89. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Spiro, Rand J., and Jihn-Chang Jehng. 1990. Cognitive flexibility and hypertext: Theory and technology for the nonlinear and multidimensional traversal of complex subject matter. In Cognition, Education, and Multimedia: Exploring Ideas in High Technology. Edited by Don Nix and Rand J. Spiro. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, pp. 163–204. [Google Scholar]
  61. Stickgold, Robert, and Matthew Walker. 2004. To sleep, perchance to gain creative insight? Trends in Cognitive Sciences 8: 191–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Su, Yingying, Carl D’Arcy, Shuai Yuan, and Xiangfei Meng. 2019. How does childhood maltreatment influence ensuing cognitive functioning among people with the exposure of childhood maltreatment? A systematic review of prospective cohort studies. Journal of Affective Disorders 252: 278–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Sun, Jiangzhou, Liang Shi, Qunlin Chen, Wenjing Yang, Dongtao Wei, Jinfu Zhang, Qinglin Zhang, and Jiang Qiu. 2018. Openness to experience and psychophysiological interaction patterns during divergent thinking. Brain Imaging and Behavior 13: 1580–89. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  64. Tabachnick, Barbara G., and Linda S. Fidell. 2007. Using Multivariate Statistics, 5th ed. Boston: Allyn and Bacon. [Google Scholar]
  65. Takeuchi, H., Yasuyuki Taki, Yuko Sassa, Hiroshi Hashizume, Atsushi Sekiguchi, Ai Fukushima, and Ryuta Kawashima. 2010. White matter structures associated with creativity: Evidence from diffusion tensor imaging. NeuroImage 51: 11–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  66. Takeuchi, Hikaru, Yasuyuki Taki, Hiroshi Hashizume, Yuko Sassa, Tomomi Nagase, Rui Nouchi, and Ryuta Kawashima. 2012. The association between resting functional connectivity and creativity. Cerebral Cortex 22: 2921–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  67. Tamannaeifar, Mohammad Reza, and Mahshad Motaghedifard. 2014. Subjective well-being and its sub-scales among students: The study of role of creativity and self-efficacy. Thinking Skills and Creativity 12: 37–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Thomson, Paula, and S. Victoria Jaque. 2021. Centrality of the creative process: Intuition, autotelic flow, and childhood adversity. Journal of Creativity 31: 100011. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Ursache, Alexandra, and Kimberly G. Noble. 2016. Neurocognitive development in socioeconomic context: Multiple mechanisms and implications for measuring socioeconomic status. Psychophysiology 53: 71–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  70. Waheed, Hina, and Ghulam Dastgeer. 2020. Impact of Self-Efficacy on Radical and Incremental Creativity: Mediating Role of Work Engagement. International Journal of Humanities and Social Sciences 12: 36–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Wang, Yang, Yan Yang, Wanting Xiao, and Qin Su. 2016. Validity and reliability of the Chinese version of the Cognitive Flexibility Inventory in college students. Chinese Mental Health Journal 30: 58–63. [Google Scholar]
  72. Wu, Yihan, and Wilma Koutstaal. 2020. Charting the contributions of cognitive flexibility to creativity: Self-guided transitions as a process-based index of creativity-related adaptivity. PLoS ONE 15: e0234473. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  73. Xiao, Zhuoni, Mina Murat Baldwin, Franziska Meinck, Ingrid Obsuth, and Aja Louise Murray. 2021. The impact of childhood psychological maltreatment on mental health outcomes in adulthood: A protocol for a systematic review and meta-analysis. Systematic Reviews 10: 224. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  74. Xing, Qiang, Zheyi Lu, and Jing Hu. 2019. The Effect of Working Memory Updating Ability on Spatial Insight Problem Solving: Evidence From Behavior and Eye Movement Studies. Frontiers in Psychology 10: 927. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  75. Zabelina, Darya L., and Michael D. Robinson. 2010. Creativity as flexible cognitive control. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts 4: 136–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  76. Zaeske, Lauren M., Haiying Long, Susanna A. Kahnke, and Barbara A. Kerr. 2022. How personality moderates the relationships between childhood environment, flow metacognitions, and creative ideational behavior: A preliminary study. Thinking Skills and Creativity 44: 101040. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  77. Zelazo, Philip David. 2006. The Dimensional Change Card Sort (DCCS): A method of assessing executive function in children. Nature Protocols 1: 297–301. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  78. Zhang, Dongjing, Zongkui Zhou, Chuanhua Gu, Yuju Lei, and Cuiying Fan. 2018. Family Socio-Economic Status and Parent-Child Relationships Are Associated with the Social Creativity of Elementary School Children: The Mediating Role of Personality Traits. Journal of Child and Family Studies 27: 2999–3007. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  79. Zhang, Jian Xin, and Ralf Schwarzer. 1995. Measuring optimistic self-beliefs: A Chinese adaptation of the General Self-Efficacy Scale. Psychologia: An International Journal of Psychology in the Orient 38: 174–81. [Google Scholar]
  80. Zhao, Xingfu, Yalin Zhang, Longfei Li, Yunfei Zhou, Hezhan Li, and Shichang Yang. 2005. Reliability and validity of the Chinese version of childhood trauma questionnaire. Chinese Journal of Clinical Rehabilitation 9: 105–7. [Google Scholar]
  81. Zhou, Xinqi, Yayun Meng, Helena S. Schmitt, Christian Montag, Keith M. Kendrick, and Benjamin Becker. 2020. Cognitive flexibility mediates the association between early life stress and habitual behavior. Personality and Individual Differences 167: 110231. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  82. Zuo, Bin, Fangfang Wen, Miao Wang, and Yang Wang. 2019. The Mediating Role of Cognitive Flexibility in the Influence of Counter-Stereotypes on Creativity. Frontiers in Psychology 10: 105. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
Figure 1. The serial multiple mediation of attachment anxiety and interpersonal relationship between adverse childhood experience and mobile phone use. Note: path coefficients were shown in unstandardized regression coefficient. *** p < 0.001.
Figure 1. The serial multiple mediation of attachment anxiety and interpersonal relationship between adverse childhood experience and mobile phone use. Note: path coefficients were shown in unstandardized regression coefficient. *** p < 0.001.
Jintelligence 11 00058 g001
Table 1. Descriptive statistics results of study variables.
Table 1. Descriptive statistics results of study variables.
VariablesMSDSkewnessKurtosis
Childhood maltreatment42.3813.731.310.54
Cognitive flexibility66.828.52−0.881.26
Self-efficacy27.585.01−0.120.73
Creativity111.869.340.060.30
Table 2. Correlations among study variables.
Table 2. Correlations among study variables.
1234
1. Childhood maltreatment
2. Cognitive flexibility−0.643 ***
3. Self-efficacy−0.547 ***0.555 ***
4. Creativity−0.384 ***0.447 ***0.518 ***
Note: *** p < 0.001.
Table 3. The serial multiple mediation effects of cognitive flexibility and self-efficacy between childhood maltreatment and creativity.
Table 3. The serial multiple mediation effects of cognitive flexibility and self-efficacy between childhood maltreatment and creativity.
PathEffectSEBootLLCIBootULCI
Total effect (c)−0.39 ***0.03−0.4459−0.3322
Direct effect (c’)−0.030.02−0.09830.0417
a1−0.64 ***0.02−0.6896−0.5953
a2−0.36 ***0.03−0.4186−0.2938
a30.35 ***0.030.28710.4102
b10.21 ***0.040.14050.2786
b20.39 ***0.030.32580.4536
Indirect effects
Total indirect effects−0.360.03−0.4156−0.3065
 Indirect 1−0.130.03−0.1863−0.0834
 Indirect 2−0.140.02−0.1866−0.1011
 Indirect 3−0.090.02−0.1224−0.0574
Note: N = 1069, k = 5000, *** p < 0.001. Indirect 1 = childhood maltreatment→cognitive flexibility→creativity; Indirect 2 = childhood maltreatment→self-efficacy→creativity; Indirect 3 = childhood maltreatment→cognitive flexibility→self-efficacy→creativity. BootLLCI = bootstrapping lower-limit confidence interval; BootULCI = bootstrapping upper-limit confidence interval; SE = standard error; Effect: standardized regression coefficient.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Li, W.; Zhang, S.; Lin, H.; Zhang, K.; Zhang, X.; Chen, J.; Xu, F.; Liu, C. Childhood Maltreatment and Creativity among Chinese College Students: A Serial Mediation Model. J. Intell. 2023, 11, 58. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence11040058

AMA Style

Li W, Zhang S, Lin H, Zhang K, Zhang X, Chen J, Xu F, Liu C. Childhood Maltreatment and Creativity among Chinese College Students: A Serial Mediation Model. Journal of Intelligence. 2023; 11(4):58. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence11040058

Chicago/Turabian Style

Li, Wenfu, Shuai Zhang, Hao Lin, Keke Zhang, Xiaolong Zhang, Jingting Chen, Fangfang Xu, and Chuanxin Liu. 2023. "Childhood Maltreatment and Creativity among Chinese College Students: A Serial Mediation Model" Journal of Intelligence 11, no. 4: 58. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence11040058

APA Style

Li, W., Zhang, S., Lin, H., Zhang, K., Zhang, X., Chen, J., Xu, F., & Liu, C. (2023). Childhood Maltreatment and Creativity among Chinese College Students: A Serial Mediation Model. Journal of Intelligence, 11(4), 58. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence11040058

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop