Next Article in Journal
Assessing Metacognitive Regulation during Problem Solving: A Comparison of Three Measures
Next Article in Special Issue
Emotional Intelligence and Creative Self-Efficacy among Gifted Children: Mediating Effect of Self-Esteem and Moderating Effect of Gender
Previous Article in Journal
Acknowledgment to the Reviewers of Journal of Intelligence in 2022
Previous Article in Special Issue
The Neural Mechanisms of the Effect of Spontaneous Insight on Re-Solution: An ERP Study
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

A Positive Association between Working Memory Capacity and Human Creativity: A Meta-Analytic Evidence

1
School of Psychology, Army Medical University, Chongqing 400038, China
2
Experimental Research Center for Medical and Psychological Science (ERC-MPS), Army Medical University, Chongqing 400038, China
3
College of Basic Medicine, Army Medical University, Chongqing 400038, China
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
These authors contributed equally to this work.
J. Intell. 2023, 11(1), 15; https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence11010015
Submission received: 29 September 2022 / Revised: 6 December 2022 / Accepted: 6 January 2023 / Published: 13 January 2023
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Understanding Creativity and Stimulating Creativity)

Abstract

:
Creativity serves as a fountain for social and scientific development. As one of the most crucial human capabilities, creativity has been believed to be supported by the core component of higher cognitive functions—working memory capacity (WMC). However, the evidence supporting the association between WMC and creativity remains contradictory. Here, we conducted a meta-analysis using random-effects models to investigate the linear association between WMC and creativity by pooling the individual effect size from the previous literature. Further, a subgroup analysis was performed to examine whether such association is specific for different WMC categories (i.e., verbal WMC, visual–spatial WMC and dual-task WMC). The main meta-analytic results showed a significantly positive association between WMC and creativity (r = .083, 95% CI: .050–.115, p < .001, n = 3104, k = 28). The subgroup analysis demonstrated consistent results by showing a significantly positive association between them, irrespective of WMC category. We also found that cultural environments could moderate this association, and we identified a strong correlation in participants from an Asian cultural context. In conclusion, this study provides the evidence to clarify the positive association between WMC and creativity, and implies that the Asian cultural context may boost such an association.

1. Introduction

Creativity refers to the ability to produce novel and suitable ideas in a specific environment (Sternberg and Lubart 1999). Based on Guilford’s divergent thinking test, it is defined as the composite concepts of originality, flexibility, and novelty of thinking (Guilford 1968). Creativity facilitates the generation of ideas in a problem-solving context and drives scientific discoveries and human progress (DeHaan 2009). Creativity was also found to be a phenotype associated with mental health problems, such as anxiety (Reid et al. 1959), schizophrenia (Degmečić 2018), and children’s behavioral problems (Fancourt and Steptoe 2019). As one of the most crucial human-specific capabilities, creativity has been intensively studied to uncover what “cognitive cornerstones” are, with working memory being a research hot spot (Hennessey and Amabile 2010; Ovando-Tellez et al. 2022).
Growing evidence suggests that executive functions (EFs) play an important role in creativity (Zabelina et al. 2019). However, it remains unclear which EF-specific components are involved. EF refers to a series of high-order cognitive functions that are essential to ensuring physical and mental health, as well as academic and career success; EF contains three core components: inhibition, working memory, and cognitive flexibility (Diamond 2013). Among them, working memory (WM) refers to the capability to hold and manipulate information temporarily with “block-wise entities” (Baddeley 2012). To structurally quantify WM ability, working memory capacity (WMC) was broadly adopted for encapsulating information in both storage and processing stages (Wagner et al. 2021). With a fundamental role in cognition, WMC is found to be indispensable for knowledge acquisition, complicated reasoning, problem-solving, and so on (Cowan 2014; Miller et al. 2018; Wiley and Jarosz 2012). Moreover, WM impairment is a hallmark of many mental illnesses, such as anxiety (Cowan 2014), Attention Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) (Alderson et al. 2013), and schizophrenia (Gold et al. 2018).
It has long been acknowledged that creativity is one of the most crucial factors associated with the development of multifarious cognitive components (De Dreu et al. 2012; Dygert and Jarosz 2020). However, the specific role of WMC in creativity remains to be clarified. The dual-pathway to creativity model suggests that WMC can reflect cognitive flexibility and cognitive persistence to positively predict creativity (Baas et al. 2008). A certain number of studies provided empirical evidence to support the positive role of WMC in creativity. For instance, Teng et al. (2018) demonstrated that increasing WMC prominently improved information extraction efficiency in creative activities (Teng et al. 2018). Further, Vally et al. (2019) demonstrated that almost all the domains in creativity ability (i.e., originality, elaboration, and fluency) could be improved by a 13-week WMC training (Vally et al. 2019). In addition, indirect evidence also supports the association between WMC and creativity: individuals with high WMC were found to outperform in creative tasks, insight-problem-solving and creative thinking (De Dreu et al. 2011; Korovkin et al. 2018; Murray and Byrne 2013; Orth et al. 2019). As for brain-behavior association, brain functional or neuroanatomical changes supporting WMC (e.g., increased functional connectivity of frontoparietal network) may be in favor of creativity ability (Chen et al. 2018; Sun et al. 2016; Zhuang et al. 2021). In summary, several lines of evidence support that WMC could be a fundamental factor promoting creativity.
In contrast to the above results, another theory, known as the controlled attention theory, posits that individuals with a higher WMC are more easily confined within a single domain, which is detrimental to the implementation of creativity (Beaty and Silvia 2012). Related research findings have raised questions regarding the association between WMC and creativity. Fugate and colleagues (2013) reported the significantly poorer performance of children with a high WMC in creative tasks, compared to children with a low WMC (Fugate et al. 2013). In addition, Furley et al. have extended a similar conclusion into adults, by showing a negative association between WMC and creativity in adult athletes (Furley and Memmert 2015).
To make things more complicated, a portion of studies argued that WMC does not show any impact on creativity, by finding that WMC is not a robust predictor for creativity ability (Chein and Weisberg 2014; Gilhooly and Fioratou 2009). Theoretical evidence underscored the null association between WMC and creativity also: creative problem solving was theoretically defined as a non-conscious process that does not appear to link WM/concentration with creative activities (Wang et al. 2020; Xinru Zhang et al. 2019). Despite evidence for the positive association between WMC and creativity, these conflicting findings challenge such arguments.
To address these conflicting results, meta-analysis has been widely used as a potent tool by providing evidence, based on the extensive previous literature (Egger and Smith 1997; Gajda et al. 2017; Peng et al. 2018). By synthesizing prior evidence into meta-analytic models, the “true effect” could be examined to confront contradictory independent observation, that is, the current meta-analysis may detect the “true effect” of the association between WMC and creativity (Michael Borenstein 2022; Brockwell and Gordon 2001).
Therefore, the current study aimed to provide evidence to clarify the association between WMC and creativity. We conducted a meta-analysis with a random-effects model to pool the individual effect size from each study concerning the association between WMC and creativity. The systematic retrieval of the literature was conducted by following 2020 PRISMA pipeline in PsycINFO, Web of Science, PubMed, EMBASE, CNKI (Chinese database) and PsycARTICLES datasets on 17 June 2022. Further, to probe into the potential hierarchical factors affecting this association, we conducted an exploratory, subgroup meta-analysis by dividing comprehensive WMC into verbal WMC, visual–spatial WMC and dual-task coordination WMC. Finally, to further probe into the impact of potential confounding factors for the meta-analytic effects, we conducted a moderation analysis to examine whether the association between WMC and creativity is moderated by cultural background and age group.

2. Materials and Methods

To improve reproducibility and transparency as recommended, this study adhered to the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) and CHARMS pipelines (Page et al. 2021) (see Figure 1). Further, all the materials relating to the present study were deposited at the Open Science Framework (OSF) with open access. This meta-analysis mainly followed five steps: (1) developing a searching strategy for retrieval of the literature; (2) defining the inclusion and exclusion criteria; (3) screening the eligibility of the literature by using the inclusion and exclusion criteria; (4) the targeting data were extracted, coded, and assessed for evidence-based quality; and (5) statistics were estimated for pooling the individual effect size by building meta-analytic and moderation models.

2.1. Search Strategy

For completeness and accuracy of the literature search, we used a keyword-based retrieval strategy to search in Boolean logic in PsycINFO, Web of Science, PubMed, EMBASE, CNKI and PsycARTICLES datasets. Specific Boolean expressions were as follows: (“Memory, Short-Term” OR “working memor*” OR “phonological loop” OR “visuospatial sketchpad” OR “central executive” OR “verbal working memory” OR “visuospatial working memory” OR “executive function” OR “updating”) AND (“Creativity” OR “creative activit*” OR “creative thinking” OR “creative achievement*” OR “creative imagination*” OR “creative personalit*”) NOT (review OR meta-analysis). To ensure data pooling completeness, reference lists of included articles, published in the last two years, were hand-reviewed.

2.2. Study Selection

According to the research objectives we predefined, the inclusion criteria were defined was as follows: (1) WMC and creativity should be measured by using standardized scales or board-certified behavioral tasks; (2) fundamental statistics (e.g., Pearson’s correlation coefficient, sample size) for examining the association between WMC and creativity should be presented clearly; (3) peer-reviewed journal articles and dissertations are allowed; (4) analytic data should be self-recruited (i.e., independent dataset); (5) a sample or control group would be qualified; (6) creativity and/or WMC would be assessed without intervention; and (7) studies should be in English/Chinese language only. On the other hand, the exclusion criteria were as follows: (1) systematic reviews (with or without meta-analysis) or preprints were not be accepted; (2) non-standardized measures were used to estimate WMC or creativity; and (3) statistics were reported vaguely.

2.3. Encoding and Statistical Analysis

Meta-information was extracted from these included studies, including the author’s name, publication date, sample size, age, and sample populations (nations and identity). Further, tasks for measuring WMC were extracted and coded into three domains: verbal WMC task, visual–spatial WMC task and dual WMC task. In addition, measures for quantifying creativity were extracted and coded into the following categories: Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking (TTCT), Test of Creative Thinking–Drawing Production(TCT-DP), Abbreviated Torrance Test for Adults (ATTA), Divergent thinking tests (DT), Convergent thinking tests (CT), Williams Prefer Measurement Forms (WPMF), Williams Creativity Assessment Packet (WCAP), Unusual uses task (UUT), Alternative Uses Task (AUT), Consensual Assessment Technique (CAT), Creative Achievement Questionnaire (CAQ), Associative fluency tasks (AF) and the Remote Association Test (RAT). Finally, for pooling the individual effect size into the meta-analytic model, the statistics (r value) and sample size for each included study were extracted.

2.4. Quality Analysis

To ensure the data quality, all the data that were extracted and coded from included studies were cross-validated by two independent researchers (IRs, GZ and MK). Any disagreements of data extraction and coding were solved by the third IR (CZY). Furthermore, two additional assessors (LXR and LMJ) were recruited to evaluate evidence-based quality by using a modified Newcastle–Ottawa Scale (mNOS) (Lo et al. 2014). The mNOS included five items to evaluate the risk of bias (ROB) for evidence (study) quality, with high ROB for total scores of ≤3 for each study. The specific assessment of mNOS included the following: (1) sample representativeness; (2) sample size; (3) comparability between respondents and non-respondents; (4) quantitative study quality; and (5) reporting quality for statistics.

2.5. Statistical Analysis

Comprehensive Meta-Analysis Software version 3.0 (CMAV3.0) was used to implement all the data analysis as we mentioned above (Makinde et al. 2021). To determine which statistical model is suitable in the current analysis, between-study heterogeneity, across the included studies, was estimated by using Higgins and Thompson’s I2 test (Borenstein et al. 2011) and Cochran’s Q test. As recommended, the random-effects meta-analytic model is suitable to pool individual effect size by controlling high between-study heterogeneity (I2 > 50%, p-value < 0.1). In addition to this main analysis, the sub-group meta-analysis was deployed to validate the individual meta-analytic effect for this association by three WMC tasks, including verbal, visual–spatial, and dual-task coordination. Furthermore, to examine whether the meta-analytic effect is biased by confounding factors, we built the moderation-effect models by taking the cultural background and age group into account. Finally, for quality control, publication bias was inspected by producing funnel plots and was calculated by using Egger’s test and Kendall’s test (Sterne and Egger 2005).

3. Results

Here, a total of twenty-eight papers (k = 28, the number of r statistics = 75, n = 3104) were screened and deemed eligible for generating the final data pool in the following meta-analysis. Fundamental information for all the included studies is tabulated in Table 1.

3.1. Main Meta-Analysis

The results of the heterogeneity tests revealed high between-study heterogeneity in this meta-analytic model, by showing a significantly high I2 value (I2 = 55%, p < .001). Thus, the random-effects models were built for the following meta-analysis.
As mentioned above, we estimated the pooled effect size by meta-analysis, concerning the r value and the sample size for the included studies. The results demonstrated a significant correlation between WMC and creativity, by pooling these individual effect sizes (r = .083, 95% confidence interval (CI): .050–.115, SE = .003, p < .001, n = 3104) (see Figure 2).

3.2. Subgroup Meta-Analysis

3.2.1. Verbal WMC Tasks and Creativity

Likewise, the heterogeneity test was conducted beforehand. Results showed a high heterogeneity for this subgroup meta-analytic model (I2 = 39%, p = .069). Thus, the meta-analysis, using the random-effects model for investigating the association between verbal WMC and creativity, revealed that the meta-analytic effect for the positive correlation between verbal WMC and creativity reached statistical significance (r = .119, 95% CI: .072–.166, SE = .006, p < .001, n = 1733) (see Figure 3).

3.2.2. Visual–Spatial WMC Tasks and Creativity

Heterogeneity was found to be acceptable for the included studies in the subgroup meta-analytic analysis (I2 = 0%, p = .58). Thus, the fix-effect model for meta-analysis was built; this demonstrated the statistically significant correlation between WMC and creativity (r = .155, 95% CI: .075–.234, SE = .006, p < .001, n = 592) (see Figure 4).

3.2.3. Dual-Tasks and Creativity

Given the high between-study heterogeneity (I2 = 70%, p < .001), the results of the meta-analysis with the random-effects model showed the statistically significant correlation between dual-task WMC and creativity (r = .153, 95% CI: .067–.237, SE = .013, p = .001, n = 1602) (see Figure 5).

3.3. Moderation-Effect Analysis

Given the high heterogeneity in the meta-analytic model, the moderation-effect analyses were performed to identify factors that may moderate the meta-analytic main effect. Here, the age and cultural contexts of the samples were reported in all the included studies and were modeled as potential moderators separately.

3.3.1. Moderation-Effect of Culture

To examine whether there are confounding factors biasing the meta-analytic effect, we conducted a moderation-effect analysis for cultural contexts (i.e., Western culture and Asian culture) and age groups (i.e., college students (age ≥ 18) and schoolchildren (age < 18)). Results showed the moderating roles of cultural contexts in the meta-analytic effect, with a stronger correlation for participants from Asian cultural backgrounds (r = .126, 95% CI: .091–.160, SE = .004, p = .000, n = 1282) than that from Western culture (r = 0.061, 95% CI: .017–.105, SE = .004, p = 0.006, n = 1411); the Q-value was significant (Q = 4.260, p = 0.039) (Figure S1).

3.3.2. Moderation-Effect of Age

Null significant findings were observed for the moderating role of age groups in the main meta-analytic effect, including college students (r = .078, 95% CI: .048–.109, SE = .005, p = .000, n = 1831) and schoolchildren (r = .037, 95% CI: −.016–.089, SE = .004, p = .168, n = 1080). The Q-value was not significant (Q =1.822, p = 0.177) (Figure S2).

3.3.3. Moderation-Effect of WMC Type

Following the moderation analysis of the WMC tasks group, null significant findings were observed for the main meta-analytic effect (Q = 1.360, p = 0.507) (Figure S3).

3.4. Publication Bias and Quality Assessment

A funnel plot for standard Fisher-Z scores for the included studies can be used to explore the publication bias. The scattered points showed a symmetric distribution (see Figure 6), which indicated no perceived publication bias. To quantify the risk of publication bias, the Egger’s test was conducted. Results showed no prominent publication bias (e = .449, p = .350). Finally, the evidence quality for the included studies was validated to be acceptable (mean scores for mNOS = 4.65, SD = 0.56, Median = 5).

4. Discussion

The main purpose of this study was to clarify the association between WMC and creativity by synthesizing meta-analytic evidence. We found that WMC is significantly positively correlated with creativity by pooling individual effects, indicating that an increased WMC indeed supports human creativity. Furthermore, subgroup meta-analysis was conducted by dividing WMC into three categories, including verbal WMC, visual–spatial WMC and dual-task coordination WMC. The results demonstrated that such associations are robust in different WMC tasks. Lastly, we conducted moderation analysis, which revealed that the correlation between WMC and creativity was moderated by cultural background, with a higher correlation for participants from Asian cultural contexts. On balance, the current study may provide weak evidence to clarify the positive correlation of WMC with creativity. In addition, such associations were found to be robust for the potential impacts of the WMC categories, and the moderating role of cultural background was revealed in this association.
One of the most crucial findings in this study is that there is a statistically significant (but weak) correlation between WMC and creativity. Both theoretical and empirical evidence supports that WMC could positively predict one’s creative ability. As the most important indicator of WM, the WMC is typically described as being the limited capacity for the temporary storage and processing of information (Baddeley 2003). On the basis of controlled attention theory (CAT), creativity is theoretically argued to be a top-down cognitive process that requires considerable cognitive resource control (Beaty and Silvia 2012). As an important component of cognitive control, WM has an imperative ability to upstream regulate complicated creative tasks (Lee and Therriault 2013). In other words, creativity is consistently achieved by extracting relevant knowledge from short-term memory or reconstructing it based on existing knowledge in long-term memory; this needs the substantial support of an adequate WMC. In addition, the positive association between WMC and creativity has been validated in a recent large-sample neuroimaging study (Takeuchi et al. 2020); this revealed the overlapping co-activated areas for the WM task and creativity task. Thus, it may provide robust evidence to clarify the positive association of WMC with creativity; this offers insight into addressing this long-lasting debate.
By using subgroup analysis, this study also found that the positive correlations between WMC and creativity were consistent, irrespective of the WMC category (i.e., verbal WMC, visual–spatial WMC, and dual-task coordination WMC). This finding attempted to answer whether the conflicting results, derived from previous studies, were attributed to a heterogenous WMC category. Supporting this, measuring WMC performance was argued to be comparable across different sensory pathways (e.g., visual and verbal) (Xu et al. 2017). In addition, Lee and colleagues (2011) well documented an increased activation in the related brain regions (medial temporal lobe, MTL) when working memory demand was increased, regardless of the type of stimulus (e.g., visual and auditory) (Lee and Rudebeck 2010). This evidence may lead us to draw the conclusion that the positive association between WMC and creativity is robust, or more boldly, to infer that the existing conflicting results may not be ascribed to sensory processing in WMC tasks.
To clarify the impact of potential confounding factors on the meta-analytic effects, moderation analyses were drawn for the cultural background of participants (i.e., Western culture and Asian culture) and age groups (i.e., adolescents and adults), respectively. Interestingly, the meta-analytic effect for the association between WMC and creativity was significantly moderated by cultural background. Specifically, compared to participants with Western cultural background, participants in the Asian cultural environment reported a stronger correlation between WMC and creativity. This finding could be explained partly by the relativity of the creativity theory (Guilford 1950). This theoretical framework elucidated the fact that creativity could be defined and evaluated specifically in different cultural environments, due to a lack of a practical criterion for creativity, with liberal scopes in western cultures (e.g., arts that required less deliberative cognitive process) (Hempel and Sue-Chan 2010). Conversely, measuring creativity in Asian cultures required strict executive functions (especially in WM) in creative tasks, such as problem-solving, deliberative reasoning and insight inference (Leung and Wang 2015). Rudowicz (2003) argued that the influence of culture on creativity is complex and highly interactive, involving historical, social, and personal cross-cultural factors (Rudowicz 2003). The key to the cross-cultural study of creativity is uncovering whether the definition and operationalizations of creativity from one culture can be validly applied to another one; this includes the eastern–western cultural gap or the conservative/traditional-liberal cultural gap. To provide evidence for this, some studies, comparing creativity between Westerners and Asians, demonstrated that the performance of creative activities was higher in people from cultural environments that highlighted creativity values (Niu and Sternberg 2002, 2003). Furthermore, one interesting finding was that Western cultures valued individualistic, intuitive, and artistic processing in creative activities, while Asian culture stressed collectivistic, cognitive, and deliberative thoughts (Goncalo and Staw 2006). That is to say, the gap between cultures, and their required involvement of cognitive processing (i.e., WM) in creative tasks, may be a crucial factor biasing the association between WM and creativity. This study indicates that the cultural gap between participants may be a source of conflict, caused by the results of existing studies.

5. Limitation

Although this study clarifies the association between WMC and creativity, several limitations should be acknowledged. Due to the strict inclusion and exclusion criteria, the total sample size (n < 3500) and evidence (study, k = 28) seem to be inadequate. Therefore, the nuance of these variations in task types of WM or creativity cannot be examined currently. Thus, future studies are needed to provide neuroimaging evidence to further confirm the association between them. Additionally, extending the main conclusion of the current study is prudent, because the total effect size for such an association is relatively small (though reaching statistical significance).

6. Conclusions

This study provides evidence to clarify the statistically significant positive association between WMC and creativity, though it has a weak strength. Further, the present study revealed that such associations exist across different types of WMC measurement (i.e., verbal WMC task, visual–spatial WMC task and dual WMC task), indicating that the conflicting results for the association between them are not biased by measure heterogeneity. This study also demonstrates that the cultural gap may confound the association between WMC and creativity.

Supplementary Materials

The following supporting information can be downloaded at: https://www.mdpi.com/article/10.3390/jintelligence11010015/s1, Figure S1: The results of the moderation-effect of culture; Figure S2, The results of the moderation-effect of age; Figure S3, The results of the moderation-effect of WMC types group.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, Z.G., K.M. and Z.C.; methodology, Z.G.; software, K.M.; validation, X.L., Y.Y. and M.L.; writing—original draft preparation, and writing—review and editing, Z.G. and K.M.; visualization, and project administration, Z.G.; funding acquisition, Z.C. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This work was supported by the PLA Key Research Foundation (CWS20J007) and PLA Talent Program Foundation (2022160258).

Institutional Review Board Statement

No ethical approval was required as the meta-analysis study does not involve original human or animal data.

Informed Consent Statement

Not applicable.

Data Availability Statement

All the materials regarding this study have been deposited in Open Science Framework (OSF): https://osf.io/3g2j9/ (accessed on 1 January 2023).

Acknowledgments

We appreciated Guo-Yu Yang (Army Medical University) and ZhengZhi Feng (Army Medical University) for scientific advice on drafting and thanked to Xiao-Gang Yang (Army Medical University) and Xiao-Qing Zhan (Army Medical University) for English editing and proofreading.

Conflicts of Interest

All the authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Alderson, R. Matt, Lisa J. Kasper, Kristen L. Hudec, and Connor H. Patros. 2013. Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and working memory in adults: A meta-analytic review. Neuropsychology 27: 287–302. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Baas, Matthijs, Carsten K. De Dreu, and Bernard A. Nijstad. 2008. A meta-analysis of 25 years of mood-creativity research: Hedonic tone, activation, or regulatory focus? Psychological Bulletin 134: 779–806. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Baddeley, Alan. 2003. Working memory: Looking back and looking forward. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 4: 829–39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Baddeley, Alan. 2012. Working memory: Theories, models, and controversies. Annual Review of Psychology 63: 1–29. [Google Scholar] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  5. Beaty, Roger E., and Paul J. Silvia. 2012. Why do ideas get more creative across time? An executive interpretation of the serial order effect in divergent thinking tasks. Psychol Aesthet Creat Arts 6: 309–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  6. Borenstein, Michael. 2022. Comprehensive Meta-Analysis Software Systematic Reviews in Health Research. London: BMJ Books, pp. 535–48. [Google Scholar]
  7. Borenstein, Michael, Larry V. Hedges, Julian P. Higgins, and Hannah R. Rothstein. 2011. Introduction to Meta-Analysis. Hoboken: Wiley & Sons. [Google Scholar]
  8. Brockwell, Sarah, and Ian Gordon. 2001. A comparison of statistical methods for meta-analysis. Statistics in Medicine 20: 825–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Chein, Jason M., and Robert W. Weisberg. 2014. Working memory and insight in verbal problems: Analysis of compound remote associates. Memory and Cognition 42: 67–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Chen, Qunlin, Roger E. Beaty, Dongtao Wei, Junyi Yang, Jiangzhou Sun, Wei Liu, Wenjing Yang, Qinglin Zhang, and Jiang Qiu. 2018. Longitudinal Alterations of Frontoparietal and Frontotemporal Networks Predict Future Creative Cognitive Ability. Cerebral Cortex 28: 103–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  11. Chen, Yanran. 2021. The Activation Process of Semantic Information and the Regulatory Role of Executive Function in Creative Thinking. Wuhan: Central China Normal University. [Google Scholar]
  12. Cowan, Nelson. 2014. Working Memory Underpins Cognitive Development, Learning, and Education. Educational Psychology Review 26: 197–223. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  13. Crenshaw, Katherine, and Stephanie Miller. 2022. Creativity and Executive Function in School-Age Children: Effects of Creative Coloring and Individual Creativity on an Executive Function Sorting Task. Psi Chi Journal of Psychological Research 27: 81–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Cushen, Patrick J., and Jennifer Wiley. 2018. Both attentional control and the ability to make remote associations aid spontaneous analogical transfer. Memory and Cognition 46: 1398–412. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  15. De Dreu, Carsten K. W., Bernard A. Nijstad, and Matthijs Baas. 2011. Behavioral Activation Links to Creativity Because of Increased Cognitive Flexibility. Social Psychological and Personality Science 2: 72–80. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. De Dreu, Carsten K., Bernard A. Nijstad, Matthijs Baas, Inge Wolsink, and Marieke Roskes. 2012. Working memory benefits creative insight, musical improvisation, and original ideation through maintained task-focused attention. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 38: 656–69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  17. Degmečić, Dunja. 2018. Schizophrenia and creativity. Psychiatria Danubina 30: 224–27. [Google Scholar]
  18. DeHaan, Robert L. 2009. Teaching creativity and inventive problem solving in science. CBE Life Sciences Education 8: 172–81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  19. Deshayes, Claire, Véronique Paban, Marie-Hélène Ferrer, Béatrice Alescio-Lautier, and Caroline Chambon. 2021. A comprehensive approach to study the resting-state brain network related to creative potential. Brain Structure and Function 226: 1743–53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Diamond, Adele. 2013. Executive functions. Annual Review of Psychology 64: 135–68. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  21. Dygert, Sarah K. C., and Andrew F. Jarosz. 2020. Individual differences in creative cognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology General 149: 1249–74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Egger, Matthias, and George Davey Smith. 1997. Meta-Analysis. Potentials and promise. BMJ 315: 1371–74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  23. Fancourt, Daisy, and Andrew Steptoe. 2019. Effects of creativity on social and behavioral adjustment in 7- to 11-year-old children. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1438: 30–39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Fugate, C. Matthew, Sydney Zentall, and Marcia Gentry. 2013. Creativity and Working Memory in Gifted Students with and Without Characteristics of Attention Deficit Hyperactive Disorder Lifting the Mask. Gifted Child Quarterly 57: 234–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Furley, Philip, and Daniel Memmert. 2015. Creativity and working memory capacity in sports: Working memory capacity is not a limiting factor in creative decision making amongst skilled performers. Frontiers in Psychology 6: 115. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  26. Gajda, Aleksandra, Maciej Karwowski, and Ronald Beghetto. 2017. Creativity and Academic Achievement: A Meta-Analysis. Journal of Educational Psychology 109: 269–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Gilhooly, Ken, and Evridiki Fioratou. 2009. Executive functions in insight versus non-insight problem solving: An individual differences approach. Thinking & Reasoning 15: 355–76. [Google Scholar]
  28. Gold, James M., Benjamin Robinson, Carly J. Leonard, Britta Hahn, Shuo Chen, Robert P. McMahon, and Steven J. Luck. 2018. Selective Attention, Working Memory, and Executive Function as Potential Independent Sources of Cognitive Dysfunction in Schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin 44: 1227–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  29. Goncalo, Jack, and Barry Staw. 2006. Individualism-Collectivism and Group Creativity. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 100: 96–109. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  30. Guilford, Joy Paul. 1950. Creativity. American Psychologist 5: 444–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Guilford, Joy Paul. 1968. The nature of human intelligence. American Educational Research Journal 5: 249. [Google Scholar]
  32. Hempel, Pal S., and Christina Sue-Chan. 2010. Culture and the Assessment of Creativity. Management and Organization Review 6: 415–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Hennessey, Beth A., and Teresa M. Amabile. 2010. Creativity. Annual Review of Psychology 61: 569–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Korovkin, Segei, Ilya Vladimirov, Alexandra Chistopolskaya, and Anna Savinova. 2018. How Working Memory Provides Representational Change During Insight Problem Solving. Frontiers in Psychology 9: 1864. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Kroesbergen, Evelyn, and Eveline Schoevers. 2017. Creativity as Predictor of Mathematical Abilities in Fourth Graders in Addition to Number Sense and Working Memory. Journal of Numerical Cognition 3: 417–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  36. Krumm, Gabriela, Vanessa Arán Filippetti, and Marisel Gutierrez. 2018. The contribution of executive functions to creativity in children: What is the role of crystallized and fluid intelligence? Thinking Skills and Creativity 29: 185–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Lee, Andy C., and Sarah R. Rudebeck. 2010. Investigating the interaction between spatial perception and working memory in the human medial temporal lobe. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 22: 2823–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  38. Lee, Christine S., and David J. Therriault. 2013. The cognitive underpinnings of creative thought: A latent variable analysis exploring the roles of intelligence and working memory in three creative thinking processes. Intelligence 41: 306–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Leung, Kwok, and Jie Wang. 2015. A Cross-Cultural Analysis of Creativity. New York: Oxford University Press, pp. 261–78. [Google Scholar]
  40. Li, P. 2008. The Relationship between Mind Wandering as a Personal Characteristic and Creativity. Jinhua: Zhejiang Normal University. [Google Scholar]
  41. Lin, Wei-Lun, and Yunn-Wen Lien. 2013. The Different Role of Working Memory in Open-Ended Versus Closed-Ended Creative Problem Solving: A Dual-Process Theory Account. Creativity Research Journal 25: 85–96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Liu, Tongran, and Jiannong Shi. 2007. Relationship Among Working Memory, Intelligence and Creativity of the 9–11 Years Old Children. Chinese Journal of Clinical Psychology 15: 164–67. [Google Scholar]
  43. Lo, Carson Ka, Dominik Mertz, and Mark Loeb. 2014. Newcastle-Ottawa Scale: Comparing reviewers’ to authors’ assessments. BMC Medical Research Methodology 14: 45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  44. Lu, Runhao, Yanna Zhang, Naili Bao, Meng Su, Xingli Zhang, and Jiannong Shi. 2021. Visuospatial, rather than verbal working memory capacity plays a key role in verbal and figural creativity. Thinking and Reasoning 28: 29–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Lunke, Katrin, and Beat Meier. 2016. Disentangling the Impact of Artistic Creativity on Creative Thinking, Working Memory, Attention, and Intelligence: Evidence for Domain-Specific Relationships with a New Self-Report Questionnaire. Frontiers in Psychology 7: 1089. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  46. Makinde, Funmilayo L., Milaine S. S. Tchamga, James Jafali, Segun Fatumo, Emile R. Chimusa, Nicola Mulder, and Gaston K. Mazandu. 2021. Reviewing and assessing existing meta-analysis models and tools. Briefings in Bioinformatics 22: bbab324. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Miller, Earl K., Mikael Lundqvist, and André M. Bastos. 2018. Working Memory 2.0. Neuron 100: 463–75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  48. Murray, M., and Ruth Byrne. 2013. Cognitive change in insight problem solving: Initial model errors and counterexamples. Journal of Cognitive Psychology 25: 210–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Niu, Weihua, and Robert Sternberg. 2002. Contemporary Studies on the Concept of Creativity: The East and the West. The Journal of Creative Behavior 36: 266–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Niu, Weihua, and Robert Sternberg. 2003. Societal and School Influences on Student Creativity: The Case of China. Psychology in the Schools 40: 103–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Orth, Dominic, Luke McDonic, Chris Ashbrook, and John van der Kamp. 2019. Efficient search under constraints and not working memory resources supports creative action emergence in a convergent motor task. Human Movement Science 67: 102505. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  52. Ou, Xiancai. 2008. A Study on the Correlation between Working Memory Span and Creative Thinking. Kirkland: Northwest University. [Google Scholar]
  53. Ovando-Tellez, Marcela, Yoed N. Kenett, Mathias Benedek, Matthieu Bernard, Joan Belo, Benoit Beranger, Theophile Bieth, and Emmanuelle Volle. 2022. Brain connectivity-based prediction of real-life creativity is mediated by semantic memory structure. Science Advances 8: eabl4294. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Page, Matthew J., Joanne E. McKenzie, Patrick M. Bossuyt, Isabelle Boutron, Tammy C. Hoffmann, Cynthia D. Mulrow, Larissa Shamseer, Jennifer M. Tetzlaff, Elie A. Akl, Sue E. Brennan, and et al. 2021. The PRISMA 2020 statement: An updated guideline for reporting systematic reviews. BMJ 372: n71. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  55. Peng, Peng, Marcia Barnes, CuiCui Wang, Wei Wang, Shan Li, H. Lee Swanson, William Dardick, and Sha Tao. 2018. A meta-analysis on the relation between reading and working memory. Psychological Bulletin 144: 48–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Peng, Sijia. 2008. An Experimental Study on the Relationship between Creative Thinking, Processing Speed and Working Memory. Xi’an: Shaanxi Normal University. [Google Scholar]
  57. Reid, Jackson B., F. J. King, and Pat N. Wickwire. 1959. Cognitive and other Personality Characteristics of Creative Children. Psychological Reports 5: 729–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Ren, Fen, Yang Liu, Yanxue Wang, and Gongxiang Chen. 2020. Relationship between Continued—Influence Effect of Misinformation and Working Memory Capacity: A Mediating Role of Creativity. Psychology: Techniques and Applications 8: 604–10. [Google Scholar]
  59. Ritter, Simone M., Jens Abbing, and Hein T. van Schie. 2018. Eye-Closure Enhances Creative Performance on Divergent and Convergent Creativity Tasks. Frontiers in Psychology 9: 1315. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  60. Rudowicz, Elisabeth. 2003. Creativity and Culture: A two way interaction. Scandinavian Journal of Educational Research 47: 273–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Sharma, Shivani, and Nandita Babu. 2017. Interplay between Creativity, Executive Function and Working Memory in Middle-Aged and Older Adults. Creativity Research Journal 29: 71–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Smeekens, Bridget A., and Michael J. Kane. 2016. Working Memory Capacity, Mind Wandering, and Creative Cognition: An Individual-Differences Investigation into the Benefits of Controlled Versus Spontaneous Thought. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts 10: 389–415. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  63. Sternberg, Robert J., and Todd I. Lubart. 1999. The Concept of Creativity: Prospects and Paradigms. New York: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
  64. Sterne, Jonathan A. C., and Matthias Egger. 2005. Regression Methods to Detect Publication and Other Bias in Meta-Analysis Publication Bias in Meta—Analysis. In Publication Bias in Meta-Analysis: Prevention, Assessment and Adjustments. New York: Wiley, pp. 99–110. [Google Scholar]
  65. Stolte, Marije, Trinidad García, Van Johannes E. H. Luit, Bob Oranje, and Evelyn H. Kroesbergen. 2020. The Contribution of Executive Functions in Predicting Mathematical Creativity in Typical Elementary School Classes: A Twofold Role for Updating. Journal of Intelligence 8: 26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  66. Sun, Jiangzhou, Qunlin Chen, Qinglin Zhang, Yadan Li, Haijiang Li, Dongtao Wei, Wenjing Yang, and Jiang Qiu. 2016. Training your brain to be more creative: Brain functional and structural changes induced by divergent thinking training. Human Brain Mapping 37: 3375–87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  67. Takeuchi, Hikaru, Yasuyuki Taki, Rui Nouchi, Ryoichi Yokoyama, Yuka Kotozaki, Seishu Nakagawa, Atsushi Sekiguchi, Kunio Iizuka, Sugiko Hanawa, Tsuyoshi Araki, and et al. 2020. Originality of divergent thinking is associated with working memory–related brain activity: Evidence from a large sample study. Neuroimage 216: 116825. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Teng, Jing. 2021. Research on the Cognitive and Neural Basis of Visual Art Creativity from the Perspective of Expertise. Shanghai: East China Normal University. [Google Scholar]
  69. Teng, Jing, Wangbing Shen, and Ning Hao. 2018. The role of cognitive control in divergent thinking. Advances in Psychological Science 26: 411–22. [Google Scholar]
  70. Tocci, Nicoletta, Patrizia Scibinetti, Emiliano Mazzoli, Myrto Foteini Mavilidi, Ilaria Masci, Mirko Schmidt, and Caterina Pesce. 2022. Giving Ideas Some Legs or Legs Some Ideas? Children’s Motor Creativity Is Enhanced by Physical Activity Enrichment: Direct and Mediated Paths. Frontiers in Psychology 13: 806065. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Vally, Zahir, Leen Salloum, Dina AlQedra, Sara El Shazly, Maryam Albloshi, Safeya Alsheraifi, and Alia Alkaabi. 2019. Examining the effects of creativity training on creative production, creative self-efficacy, and neuro-executive functioning. Thinking Skills and Creativity 31: 70–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Wagner, Barry T., Lauren A. Shaffer, Olivia A. Ivanson, and James A. Jones. 2021. Assessing working memory capacity through picture span and feature binding with visual-graphic symbols during a visual search task with typical children and adults. Augmentative and Alternative Communication 37: 39–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  73. Wang, Yi. 2011. The Influence of Working Memory Span and Central Executive Function on Undergraduates’ Creativity. Kaifeng: Henan University. [Google Scholar]
  74. Wang, Lixia, Haiying Long, Jonathan A. Plucker, Qing Wang, Xiaobo Xu, and Weiguo Pang. 2018. High Schizotypal Individuals Are More Creative? The Mediation Roles of Overinclusive Thinking and Cognitive Inhibition. Frontiers in Psychology 9: 1766. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  75. Wang, Shuoyang, Chongde Lin, and Hanyin Sun. 2020. The Mechanism of Associative Abilities in Creative Problem Solving. Studies of Psychology and Behavior 18: 8–16. [Google Scholar]
  76. Wiley, Jennifer, and Andrew F. Jarosz. 2012. Chapter Six—How Working Memory Capacity Affects Problem Solving. In Psychology of Learning and Motivation. Edited by Brian H. Ross. Cambridge: Academic Press, vol. 56, pp. 185–227. [Google Scholar]
  77. Xu, Zhan, Qiangqiang Li, and Jiang Qiu. 2017. The Prediction of Working Memory Capacity to Creative Thinking. Journal of Psychological Science 40: 64–69. [Google Scholar]
  78. Yamaoka, Akina, and Shinataro Yukawa. 2020. Does Mind Wandering During the Thought Incubation Period Improve Creativity and Worsen Mood? Psychological Reports 123: 1785–800. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  79. Zabelina, Darya L., Naomi P. Friedman, and Jessica Andrews-Hanna. 2019. Unity and diversity of executive functions in creativity. Consciousness and Cognition 68: 47–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  80. Zeng, Ziqiu. 2015. The Relationship between Executive Function and Creativity in 9-year-old Children. Beijing: Beijing Forestry University. [Google Scholar]
  81. Zhang, Xinru, Wei Wang, Haijun Duan, Yangyang Zhao, Yuecui Kan, and Weiping Hu. 2019. Effect of Working Memory on Insight and Analytic Problem Solving. Journal of Psychological Science 42: 777–83. [Google Scholar]
  82. Zhuang, Kaixiang, Wenjing Yang, Yu Li, Jie Zhang, Qunlin Chen, Jie Meng, Dongtao Wei, Jiangzhou Sun, Li He, Yu Mao, and et al. 2021. Connectome-based evidence for creative thinking as an emergent property of ordinary cognitive operations. Neuroimage 227: 117632. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Flow diagram of study selection process based on the 2020 PRISMA protocol.
Figure 1. Flow diagram of study selection process based on the 2020 PRISMA protocol.
Jintelligence 11 00015 g001
Figure 2. Forest plot for main meta-analysis concerning the association between WMC and creativity. Effect size was presented as z-value in this figure.
Figure 2. Forest plot for main meta-analysis concerning the association between WMC and creativity. Effect size was presented as z-value in this figure.
Jintelligence 11 00015 g002
Figure 3. Forest plot with 95% confidence intervals and weights for subgroup meta-analysis, concerning the association between verbal WMC tasks and creativity. Larger positive effect sizes indicate that increased creativity ability is related to verbal WMC.
Figure 3. Forest plot with 95% confidence intervals and weights for subgroup meta-analysis, concerning the association between verbal WMC tasks and creativity. Larger positive effect sizes indicate that increased creativity ability is related to verbal WMC.
Jintelligence 11 00015 g003
Figure 4. Forest plot with 95% confidence intervals and weights for subgroup meta-analysis, showing the association between visual–spatial WMC tasks and creativity.
Figure 4. Forest plot with 95% confidence intervals and weights for subgroup meta-analysis, showing the association between visual–spatial WMC tasks and creativity.
Jintelligence 11 00015 g004
Figure 5. Forest plot with 95% confidence intervals and weights for subgroup meta-analysis, showing the association between dual-tasks and creativity.
Figure 5. Forest plot with 95% confidence intervals and weights for subgroup meta-analysis, showing the association between dual-tasks and creativity.
Jintelligence 11 00015 g005
Figure 6. Funnel plot of this study to assess the publication bias. X--axis indicates the individual study effect estimates, and Y-axis indicates standard errors.
Figure 6. Funnel plot of this study to assess the publication bias. X--axis indicates the individual study effect estimates, and Y-axis indicates standard errors.
Jintelligence 11 00015 g006
Table 1. Summary of characteristics of included studies. N.A. = not applicable.
Table 1. Summary of characteristics of included studies. N.A. = not applicable.
AuthorPublication TimeRegionSample SizeAgeGenderSubject CategoryWorking Memory MeasuresCreativity MeasuresVariable Relation
MaleFemale
(Yamaoka and Yukawa 2020)2019Japan5218.96 ± 1.211933College studentsOSPAN and SSPANUUTDual-tasks and creativity
(Zeng 2015)2015CN3649.4 ± 0.45182182SchoolchildrenDigit Span Backwards/N-BackCAT, TTCTVerbal WMC tasks/Dual-tasks and creativity
(Peng 2008)2008CN10817.66 ± 0.746246StudentsDigital comparison task/space tracking taskCreative Thinking TestVerbal WMC tasks/Visual–spatial and creativity
(Zabelina et al. 2019)1 February 2022USA4729.26 ± 7.932324Neuro-typical adultWMC updating tasksATTA, CAQDual-tasks and creativity
(Tocci et al. 2022)10 March 2022Italy957.8 ± 1.34748SchoolchildrenRandom Number Generation Task.TTCTDual-tasks and creativity
(Chen 2021)2021CN30NANANAStudentsShort-term memory taskAUTDual-tasks and creativity
(Wang 2011)2011CN6018–24NANACollege studentsOperation word breadth task/point matrix taskWCAPVerbal WMC tasks/Visual–spatial WMC tasks/Dual-tasks and creativity
(Stolte et al. 2020)2 June 2020The Netherlands2789.71 ± 0.93139139Schoolchildrenthe Monkey Game/the lion gamethe Mathematical Creativity taskDual-tasks and creativity
(Cushen and Wiley 2018)2 August2018USA12019.39 ± 1.74NANACollege studentsOSPAN and SSPANRATDual-tasks and creativity
(Deshayes et al. 2021)7 May 2021Germany4514.13 ± 3.252223Normal person WMC updating tasksTTCTDual-tasks and creativity
(Krumm et al. 2018)30 July 2018Argentina209NANANASchoolchildrenWISC-IVTTCTDual-tasks and creativity
(Crenshaw and Miller 2022)9 May 2020USA499±0.251930SchoolchildrenWAIS –IIIAUTDual-tasks and creativity
(Lu et al. 2021)14 April 2021CN107,
68,
64
20.45 ± 3.31,
21.49 ± 2.26,
23.23 ± 3.83
45,
31,
27
62,
37,
37
College studentsVerbal tasks and Visual–spatial task/The tapping taskTTCTVerbal WMC tasks/Visual–spatial WMC tasks and Verbal\Figure creativity
(Ritter et al. 2018)31 July 2018 The Netherlands3219.7NANACollege studentsDSRAT Verbal WMC tasks and creativity
(Liu and Shi 2007)2007CN839/10/11 ± 0.255033SchoolchildrenSternberg WM ParadigmWCAP, CATVerbal WMC tasks and creativity
(Li 2008)2014CN11618–243185College studentsOSPANUUTVerbal WMC tasks and creativity
(Xiancai Ou 2008)2008CN10119.626544College studentsOperation—word width task/point matrix space width taskCreative Thinking ScaleVerbal WMC tasks/Visual–spatial WMC tasks and creativity
(Ren et al. 2020)2020CN7019.84 ± 1.462743College studentsVerbal WM tasksWPMFVerbal WMC tasks and creativity
(Furley and Memmert 2015)10 February 2015Germany6123.48 ± 3.6610Soccer athletesOSPANDT testVerbal WMC tasks and creativity
(Sharma and Babu 2017)2 February 2017India5852.05, 57.21, 62.052533Middle-aged and older adultsPGI memory scaleTTCTVerbal WMC tasks and creativity
(Wang et al. 2018)21 September 2018CN7821.54 ± 1.33,
21.73 ± 1.45
969Neuro-typical adultOSPAN, RAPM, number-letter taskAUTVerbal WMC tasks/Dual-tasks and creativity
(Lee and Therriault 2013)5 June 2013USA26520.33 ± 2.5459206College studentsSymmetry Span task/Backward Digit Span taskAF tasks, DT tests, CT testsVerbal WMC tasks/Visual–spatial tasks and creativity
(Fugate et al. 2013)30 August 2013USA6NANANAGifted Students Without ADHD characteristicsThe Woodcock Johnson IIITTCTVerbal WMC tasks and creativity
(Kroesbergen and Schoevers 2017)22 December 2017The Netherlands1669.66 ± 0.587987Schoolchildren Two computerized WM tasksTCT-DP, MCTVerbal WMC tasks/Visual–spatial and creativity/mathematical creativity
(Lunke and Meier 2016)28 July 2016Switzerland27026.19 ± 8.52NANANeuro-typical adultRSTACDCVerbal WMC tasks and artistic Creativity
(Teng 2021)2021CN8921.961970College studentsOSPANAUT, TTCT, Creative tasks Verbal WMC tasks and Artistic Creativity
(Smeekens and Kane 2016)7 May 2013CN5520.3 ± 1.22134College studentsN-back tasksDT tasksDual-tasks and creativity
(Lin and Lien 2013)15 February 2016USA173, 142NANANACollege studentsOSPAN and SSPANAUT, SARTDual-tasks and creativity
Notes: TTCT: Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking, TCT-DP: Test of Creative Thinking-Drawing Production, ATTA: Abbreviated Torrance Test for Adults, DT: Divergent thinking tests, CT: Convergent thinking tests, WPMF: Williams Prefer Measurement Forms, WCAP: Williams Creativity Assessment Packet, UUT: Unusual uses task, AUT: Alternative Uses Task, CAT: Consensual Assessment Technique, CAQ: Creative Achievement Questionnaire, AF: Associative fluency tasks, RAT: Remote Association Test. ACDC: The Artistic Creativity Domains Compendium. Verbal WMC task: test phonological loop (Word recall forwards, Digit Recall, Non-word List Recall, Word List Recall, OSPAN; visual–spatial WMC task: test visual–spatial sketchpad (Corsi block task, Dot matrix, matrix pattern, spatial span, Block Recall, SSPAN; dual WMC task: including both dual verbal and visual–spatial WMC tasks or complex WMC task (N-back task, recall tasks, tasks including both verbal and visual–spatial WMC tasks. WISC: Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children–Fourth Edition, WAIS: Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Gong, Z.; Miao, K.; Liu, X.; Luo, M.; Yu, Y.; Chen, Z. A Positive Association between Working Memory Capacity and Human Creativity: A Meta-Analytic Evidence. J. Intell. 2023, 11, 15. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence11010015

AMA Style

Gong Z, Miao K, Liu X, Luo M, Yu Y, Chen Z. A Positive Association between Working Memory Capacity and Human Creativity: A Meta-Analytic Evidence. Journal of Intelligence. 2023; 11(1):15. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence11010015

Chicago/Turabian Style

Gong, Zheng, Kuan Miao, Xuerong Liu, Mengjie Luo, Yang Yu, and Zhiyi Chen. 2023. "A Positive Association between Working Memory Capacity and Human Creativity: A Meta-Analytic Evidence" Journal of Intelligence 11, no. 1: 15. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence11010015

APA Style

Gong, Z., Miao, K., Liu, X., Luo, M., Yu, Y., & Chen, Z. (2023). A Positive Association between Working Memory Capacity and Human Creativity: A Meta-Analytic Evidence. Journal of Intelligence, 11(1), 15. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence11010015

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop