Metacognitive and Non-Metacognitive Processes in Arithmetic Performance: Can There Be More than One Meta-Level?
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Theoretical Advancements and Empirical Results from the Last Decades
2.1. Classical Taxonomies of Metacognition and the Nelson-Narens-Model
2.2. System 1 and System 2
2.3. More than Two Systems in Human Cognition
What, reduced to their simplest reciprocal form, were Bloom’s
thoughts about Stephen’s thoughts about Bloom and about Stephen’s
thoughts about Bloom’s thoughts about Stephen?—James Joyce, Ulysses
3. Two Educationally Relevant Dilemmas Concerning the Metacognitive and Non-Metacognitive Processes of Arithmetic Performance
3.1. The Balance between Metacognitive and Non-Metacognitive Processes of Arithmetic Performance
3.2. Difference between Actual and Potential Metacognitive Processes of Arithmetic Performance
4. Educationally Relevant Answers in the Field of Arithmetic Performance
4.1. (At Least) Three Levels of Components in Arithmetic Performance
4.2. Transition between Phases of Arithmetic Performance
5. Corollaries
5.1. Observing and Measuring Metacognition in Arithmetic Performance
5.2. Implications for Teaching and Learning
5.2.1. Curriculum
5.2.2. Teaching and Learning
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
1 | Having written a simple addition like 5 + 2 onto a piece of paper, smart phones with PhotoMath application can immediately tell that the result is 7. Could it cause some kind of loss in terms of quick access to mental number facts? |
References
- Afflerbach, Peter, P. David Pearson, and Scott G. Paris. 2008. Clarifying differences between reading skills and reading strategies. The Reading Teacher 61: 364–73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Alexander, Joyce M., Martha Carr, and Paula J. Schwanenflugel. 1995. Development of metacognition in gifted children: Directions for future research. Developmental Review 15: 1–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Allain, Philippe, Christophe Verny, Ghislaine Aubin, Karine Pinon, Dominique Bonneau, Frédéric Dubas, and Didier Le Gall. 2005. Arithmetic word-problem-solving in Huntington’s disease. Brain and Cognition 57: 1–3. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Almasi, Janice F., and Susan K. Fullerton. 2012. Teaching Strategic Processes in Reading. New York: Guilford Press. [Google Scholar]
- Azevedo, Roger, Daniel C. Moos, Amy M. Johnson, and Amber D. Chauncey. 2010. Measuring Cognitive and Metacognitive Regulatory Processes During Hypermedia Learning: Issues and Challenges. Educational Psychologist 45: 210–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Baker, Linda, and Lorraine C. Cerro. 2000. Assessing metacognition in children and adults. In Issues in the Measurement of Metacognition. Edited by Gregory Schraw and James C. Impara. Lincoln: Buros Institute of Mental Measurements, pp. 99–146. [Google Scholar]
- Ball, Deborah Loewenberg, Mark Hoover Thames, and Geoffrey Phelps. 2008. Content knowledge for teaching: What makes it special? Journal of Teacher Education 59: 389–407. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Baten, Elke, Magda Praet, and Annemie Desoete. 2017. The relevance and efficacy of metacognition for instructional design in the domain of mathematics. ZDM 49: 613–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bellon, Elien, Wim Fias, and Bert De Smedt. 2019. More than number sense: The additional role of executive functions and metacognition in arithmetic. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 182: 38–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Berteletti, Ilaria, Jérôme Prado, and James R. Booth. 2014. Children with mathematical learning disability fail in recruiting verbal and numerical brain regions when solving simple multiplication problems. Cortex 57: 143–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Brown, Ann L. 1978. Knowing when, where and how to remember. A problem of metacognition. In Advances in Instructional Psychology (Vol. I). Edited by Robert Glaser. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, pp. 77–165. [Google Scholar]
- Campbell, Jamie I. D., and Qilin Xue. 2001. Cognitive arithmetic across cultures. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 130: 299–315. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Campione, Joseph C., Ann L. Brown, and Michael L. Connell. 1988. Metacognition: On the importance of understanding what you are doing. In The Teaching and Assessing of Mathematical Problem Solving. Edited by Randall I. Charles and Edward A. Silver. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, vol. 3, pp. 93–114. [Google Scholar]
- Canobi, Katherine H. 2004. Individual differences in children’s addition and subtraction knowledge. Cognitive Development 19: 81–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Carpenter, Thomas P., and James M. Moser. 1984. The acquisition of addition and subtraction concepts in grades one through three. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education 15: 179–202. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cragg, Lucy, Sophie Richardson, Paula J. Hubber, Sarah Keeble, and Camilla Gilmore. 2017. When is working memory important for arithmetic? The impact of strategy and age. PLoS ONE 12: e0188693. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Csíkos, Csaba. 2016. Strategies and performance in elementary students’ three-digit mental addition. Educational Studies in Mathematics 91: 123–39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Csíkos, Csaba, and János Steklács. 2015. Phases of a ten-year old student’s solution process of an insight problem as revealed by eye-tracking methodology. Mathematics Teaching-Research Journal Online 8: 26–48. [Google Scholar]
- Csíkos, Csaba, and Judit Szitányi. 2020. Teachers’ pedagogical content knowledge in teaching word problem solving strategies. ZDM 52: 165–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- D’Amore, Bruno, and Berta Martini. 1999. The didactic contract, mental models and intuitive models in the resolution of standard scholastic problems. In A Multidimensional Approach to Learning in Mathematic and Science. Edited by Athanasios Gagatsis. Nicosia: Intercollege Press, pp. 3–24. [Google Scholar]
- Danthiir, Vanessa, Richard D. Roberts, Ralf Schulze, and Oliver Wilhelm. 2005. Mental speed. In Handbook of Understanding and Measuring Intelligence. Edited by Oliver Wilhelm and Randall W. Engle. Thousand Oaks, London and New Delhi: SAGE, pp. 27–46. [Google Scholar]
- Darling, Stephen, Sergio Della Sala, Colin Gray, and Cristina Trivelli. 1998. Putative functions of the prefrontal cortex: Historical perspectives and new horizons. In Metacognition and Cognitive Neuropsychology: Monitoring and Control Processes. Edited by Giuliana Mazzioni and Thomas O. Nelson. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers, pp. 53–95. [Google Scholar]
- De Corte, Erik. 2007. Learning from instruction: The case of mathematics. Learning Inquiry 1: 19–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- De Corte, Erik, and Lieven Verschaffel. 1987. The effect of semantic structure on first graders’ strategies for solving addition and subtraction word problems. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education 18: 363–81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Desoete, Annemie. 2009. Mathematics and metacognition in adolescents and adults with learning disabili-ties. International Electronic Journal of Elementary Education 2: 82–100. [Google Scholar]
- Duffy, Gerald G. 2005. Developing metacognitive teachers: Visioning and the expert’s changing role in teacher education and professional development. In Metacognition in Literacy Learning: Theory, Assessment, Instruction, and Professional Development. Edited by Susan E. Israel, Cathy C. Block, Kathryn L. Bauserman and Kathryn Kinnucan-Welsch. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, pp. 299–314. [Google Scholar]
- Estes, David. 1998. Young children’s awareness of their mental aactivity: The case of mental rotation. Child Development 69: 1345–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Flavell, John H. 1979. Metacognition and cognitive monitoring: A new area of cognitive–developmental inquiry. American Psychologist 34: 906–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Flavell, John H. 2004. Theory-of-mind development: Retrospect and prospect. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly 50: 274–90. Available online: https://www.jstor.org/stable/23096166 (accessed on 31 May 2022). [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Fleming, Stephen M., and Raymond J. Dolan. 2012. The neural basis of metacognitive ability. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 367: 1338–49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Fleming, Stephen M., Raymond J. Dolan, and Christopher D. Frith. 2012. Metacognition: Computation, biology and function. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 367: 1280–86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Fletcher, Logan, and Peter Carruthers. 2012. Metacognition and reasoning. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 367: 1366–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Fuchs, Lynn S., Douglas Fuchs, and Donald L. Compton. 2010. Rethinking Response to Intervention at Middle and High School. School Psychology Review 39: 22–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gilmore, Camilla K., and Marietta Papadatou-Pastou. 2009. Patterns of individual differences in conceptual understanding and arithmetical skill: A meta-analysis. Mathematical Thinking and Learning 11: 25–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Gilmore, Camilla K., and Peter Bryant. 2008. Can children construct inverse relations in arithmetic? Evidence for individual differences in the development of conceptual understanding and computational skill. British Journal of Developmental Psychology 26: 301–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Ginsburg, Herbert P. 1996. Toby’s math. In The Nature of Mathematical Thinking. Edited by Robert J. Sternberg and Talia Ben-Zeev. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc., pp. 175–202. [Google Scholar]
- Goos, Merrylin, and Peter Galbraith. 1996. Do it this way! Metacognitive strategies in collaborative mathematical problem solving. Educational Studies in Mathematics 30: 229–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gourgey, Annette F. 1998. Metacognition in basic skills instruction. Instructional Science 26: 81–96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Grabner, Roland H., Clemens Brunner, Valerie Lorenz, Stephan E. Vogel, and Bert De Smedt. 2022. Fact retrieval or compacted counting in arithmetic—A Neurophysiological Investigation of Two Hypotheses. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 48: 199–212. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Grabner, Roland, and Bert De Smedt. 2012. Oscillatory EEG correlates of arithmetic strategies: A training study. Frontiers in Psychology 3: 428. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Hatano, Giyoo, and Kayoko Inagaki. 1986. Two courses of expertise. In Child Development and Education in Japan. Edited by Harold W. Stevenson, Azuma Hiroshi and Hakuta Kenji. A Series of Books in Psychology; New York: W H Freeman/Times Books/Henry Holt & Co., pp. 262–72. [Google Scholar]
- He, Tung-hsien. 2008. Reading for different goals: The interplay of EFL college students’ multiple goals, reading strategy use and reading comprehension. Journal of Research in Reading 31: 224–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hegarty, Mary, Richard E. Mayer, and Christopher A. Monk. 1995. Comprehension of arithmetic word problems: A comparison of successful and unsuccessful problem solvers. Journal of Educational Psychology 87: 18–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hikosaka, Okihide, and Masaki Isoda. 2010. Switching from automatic to controlled behavior: Cortico-basal ganglia mechanisms. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 14: 154–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Hudson, Kesha N., Jennifer L. Coffman, and Peter A. Ornstein. 2018. Addition in kindergarten: The role of mothers’ and teachers’ language. Journal of Cognition and Development 19: 65–86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jacobse, Annemieke E., and Egbert G. Harskamp. 2012. Towards efficient measurement of metacognition in mathematical problem solving. Metacognition and Learning 7: 133–49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Jausovec, Norbert. 1994. Metacognition in creative problem solving. In Problem Finding, Problem Solving and Creativity. Edited by Marc A. Runco. Norwood: Ablex Publishing, pp. 77–95. [Google Scholar]
- Kavousi, Shabnam, Patrick A. Miller, and Patricia A. Alexander. 2020. Modeling metacognition in design thinking and design making. International Journal of Technology and Design Education 30: 709–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Keren, Gideon, and Yaacov Schul. 2009. Two is not always better than one: A critical evaluation of two-system theories. Perspectives on Psychological Science 4: 533–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kluwe, Rainer H. 1987. Executive decisions and regulation of problem solving behavior. In Metacognition, Motivation, and Understanding. Edited by Franz E. Weinert and Rainer H. Kluwe. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, pp. 31–64. [Google Scholar]
- Koninklijke Nederlandse Akademie van Wetenschappe. 2009. Koninklijke Nederlandse Akademie van Wetenschappe [Royal Dutch Academia of Sciences]. In Rekenonderwijs Op de Basisschool. Analyse En Sleutels Tot Verbetering [Mathematics Education in the Elementary School. Analysis and Keys to Improvement]. Amsterdam: Koninklijke Nederlandse Akademie van Wetenschappen. [Google Scholar]
- Kuhn, Deanna. 1995. Microgenetic study of change: What has it told us? Psychological Science 6: 133–39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kuhn, Deanna. 2000. Metacognitive development. Current Directions in Psychological Science 9: 178–81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Leahey, Thomas Hardy, and Richard Jackson Harris. 1993. Learning and Cognition, 3rd ed. Englewood Cliffs: Prentice Hall. [Google Scholar]
- Lee, Kerry, Swee Fong Ng, Rebecca Bull, Madeline Lee Pe, and Ringo Ho Moon Ho. 2011. Are patterns important? An investigation of the relationships between proficiencies in patterns, computation, executive functioning, and algebraic word problems. Journal of Educational Psychology 103: 269. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lemaire, Patrick, and Robert S. Siegler. 1995. Four aspects of strategic change: Contributions to children’s learning of multiplication. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 124: 83–97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Levinsson, Henrik. 2008. Autonomy and Metacognition: A Healthcare Perspective. Ph.D. thesis, Lund University (Media-Tryck), Lund, Sweden. [Google Scholar]
- Logan, Gordon D. 1988. Toward an instance theory of automatization. Psychological Review 95: 492–527. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mokhtari, Kouider, and Carla A. Reichard. 2002. Assessing students’ metacognitive awareness of reading strategies. Journal of Educational Psychology 94: 249–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Monaghan, Frank. 2005. ‘Don’t think in your head, think aloud’: Ict and exploratory talk in the Primary School Mathematics Classroom. Research in Mathematics Education 7: 83–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nakakoji, Yoshitaka, and Rachel Wilson. 2020. Interdisciplinary learning in mathematics and science: Transfer of Learning for 21st Century Problem Solving at University. Journal of Intelligence 8: 32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Nelson, Thomas O. 1996. Consciousness and metacognition. American Psychologist 51: 102–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nelson, Thomas O., and Louis Narens. 1994. Why investigate metacognition. In Metacognition: Knowing about Knowing. Edited by Janet Metcalfe and Arthur P. Shimamura. Cambridge: MIT Press, pp. 1–25. [Google Scholar]
- Nunes, Terezinha, Analucia Dias Schliemann, and David W. Carraher. 1993. Street Mathematics and School Mathematics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Ostad, Snorre A. 1997. Developmental differences in addition strategies: A comparison of mathematically disabled and mathematically normal children. British Journal of Educational Psychology 67: 345–57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pappas, Sandra, Herbert P. Ginsburg, and Minyang Jiang. 2003. SES differences in young children’s metacognition in the context of mathematical problem solving. Cognitive Development 18: 431–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Paris, Scott G., and Ellen E. Hamilton. 2009. The development of children’s reading comprehension. In Handbook of Research on Reading Comprehension. Edited by Susan E. Israel and Gerald G. Duffy. London: Routledge, pp. 56–77. [Google Scholar]
- Paris, Scott G., and Peter Winograd. 1990. How metacognition can promote academic learning and instruction. In Dimensions of Thinking and Cognitive Instruction. Edited by Beau Fly Jones and Lorna Idol. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc., pp. 15–51. [Google Scholar]
- Park, Joonkoo, and Elizabeth M. Brannon. 2014. Improving arithmetic performance with number sense training: An investigation of underlying mechanism. Cognition 133: 188–200. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Parsons, Seth A. 2012. Adaptive teaching in literacy instruction: Case studies of two teachers. Journal of Literacy Research 44: 149–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Paulus, Markus, Joelle Proust, and Beate Sodian. 2013. Examining implicit metacognition in 3.5-year-old children: An eye-tracking and pupillometric study. Frontiers in Psychology 4: 145. Available online: https://www.frontiersin.org/article/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00145 (accessed on 31 May 2022). [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Peters, Lien, and Bert De Smedt. 2018. Arithmetic in the developing brain: A review of brain imaging studies. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience 30: 265–79. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Petitmengin, Claire. 2006. Describing one’s subjective experience in the second person: An interview method for the science of consciousness. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences 5: 229–69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pintrich, Paul R., and Elisabeth V. De Groot. 1990. Motivational and self-regulated learning components of classroom academic performance. Journal of Educational Psychology 82: 33–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pintrich, Paul R., Christopher A. Wolters, and Gail P. Baxter. 2000. Assessing metacognition and self-regulated learning. In Issues in the Measurement of Metacognition. Edited by Gregory Schraw and G. James C. Impara. Lincoln: Buros Institute of Mental Measurements, pp. 43–97. [Google Scholar]
- Polanyi, Michael. 1958. Personal Knowledge: Towards a Post-Critical Philosophy. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul, Available online: https://ebookcentral.proquest.com/lib/uqac-ebooks/detail.action?docID=179903 (accessed on 31 May 2022).
- Prediger, Susanne, and Nadine Krägeloh. 2015. Low achieving eighth graders learn to crack word problems: A design research project for aligning a strategic scaffolding tool to students’ mental processes. ZDM 47: 947–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Reusser, Kurt. 2000. Success and failure in school mathematics: Effects of instruction and school environment. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry 9: S17–S26. [Google Scholar]
- Rittle-Johnson, Bethany, and Martha Wagner Alibali. 1999. Conceptual and procedural knowledge of mathematics: Does one lead to the other? Journal of Educational Psychology 91: 175–89. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rosenthal, David M. 1998. A theory of consciousness. In The Nature of Consciousness. Philosophical Debates. Edited by Ned Block, Owen Flanagan and Güven Güzeldere. Cambridge: The MIT Press, pp. 729–53. [Google Scholar]
- Rosenthal, David. 2012. Higher-order awareness, misrepresentation and function. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 367: 1424–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Schatteles, Tibor. 2014. The Mirror of Socrates: Twelve Essays of a Reader on World Literature. Archway Publishing. Available online: http://www.archwaypublishing.com/en/bookstore/bookdetails/496086-The-Mirror-of-Socrates (accessed on 31 May 2022).
- Schneider, Wolfgang. 2008. The development of metacognitive knowledge in children and adolescents: Major trends and implications for education. Mind, Brain, and Education 2: 114–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schooler, Jonathan W. 2002. Re-representing consciousness: Dissociations between experience and meta-consciousness. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 6: 339–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schraw, Gergory, Lisa D. Bendixen, and Michael E. Dunkle. 2002. Development and validation of the Epistemic Belief Inventory (EBI). In Personal Epistemology: The Psychology of Beliefs About Knowledge and Knowing. Edited by Barbara K. Hofer and Paul R. Pintrich. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers, pp. 261–75. [Google Scholar]
- Schraw, Gregory, and Rayne Sperling Dennison. 1994. Assessing Metacognitive Awareness. Contemporary Educational Psychology 19: 460–75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Selter, Christoph. 2009. Creativity, flexibility, adaptivity, and strategy use in mathematics. ZDM 41: 619–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Siegler, Robert S. 1987. The perils of averaging data over strategies: An example from children’s addition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 116: 250–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Spanoudis, George, and Andreas Demetriou. 2020. Mapping mind-brain development: Towards a comprehensive theory. Journal of Intelligence 8: 19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Sperling, Rayne A., Bruce C. Howard, Lee Ann Miller, and Cheryl Murphy. 2002. Measures of children’s knowledge and regulation of cognition. Contemporary Educational Psychology 27: 51–79. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Stanovich, Keith E., and Richard F. West. 2000. Individual differences in reasoning: Implications for the rationality debate? Behavioral and Brain Sciences 23: 645–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Steffler, Dorothy J., Connie K. Varnhagen, Christine K. Friesen, and Rebecca Treiman. 1998. There’s more to children’s spelling than the errors they make: Strategic and automatic processes for one-syllable words. Journal of Educational Psychology 90: 492–505. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sternberg, Robert J. 1985. Beyond IQ: A Triarchic Theory of Human Intelligence. New York: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Sternberg, Robert J. 1998. Metacognition, abilities, and developing expertise: What makes an expert stu-dent? Instructional Science 26: 127–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sternberg, Robert J. 2021. Adaptiveintelligence: Intelligence is not a personal trait but rather a person × task × situation interaction. Journal of Intelligence 9: 58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sternberg, Robert J., Vlad Glaveanu, Sareh Karami, James C. Kaufman, Shane N. Phillipson, and David D. Preiss. 2021. Meta-intelligence: Understanding, control, and interactivity between creative, analytical, practical, and wisdom-based approaches in problem solving. Journal of Intelligence 9: 19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sun, Qiyu, Lawrence Jun Zhang, and Susan Carter. 2021. Investigating students’ metacognitive experiences: Insights from the English as a Foreign Language Learners’ Writing Metacognitive Experiences Questionnaire (EFLLWMEQ). Frontiers in Psychology 12: 744842. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tánczikné Varga, Szilvia, Attila Pásztor, and János Steklács. 2020. An online instrument assessing the relationship between morphological structure awareness and reading comprehension in Hungarian 2–4 graders. Elementary Education Online 19: 2322–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tanner, Kimberly D. 2012. Promoting student metacognition. CBE—Life Sciences Education 11: 113–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Thompson, Valerie A. 2009. Dual-process theories: A metacognitive perspective. In Two Minds: Dual Processes and Beyond. Edited by Jonathan Evans and Keith Frankish. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 171–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Threlfall, John. 1998. Are mental calculation strategies really strategies? Proceedings of the British Society for Research into Learning Mathematics 18: 71–76. [Google Scholar]
- Timmermans, Bert, Leonhard Schilbach, Antoine Pasquali, and Axel Cleeremans. 2012. Higher order thoughts in action: Consciousness as an unconscious re-description process. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 367: 1412–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Träff, Ulf. 2013. The contribution of general cognitive abilities and number abilities to different aspects of mathematics in children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 116: 139–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Van der Stel, Manita, Marcel V. J. Veenman, Kim Deelen, and Janine Haenen. 2010. The increasing role of metacognitive skills in math: A cross-sectional study from a developmental perspective. ZDM 42: 219–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- van der Stel, Manita, and Marcel V. J. Veenman. 2014. Metacognitive skills and intellectual ability of young adolescents: A longitudinal study from a developmental perspective. European Journal of Psychology of Education 29: 117–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Veenman, Marcel V. J. 2011. Alternative assessment of strategy use with self-report instruments: A discussion. Metacognition and Learning 6: 205–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Veenman, Marcel V. J., Bernadette H. A. M. Van Hout-Wolters, and Peter Afflerbach. 2006. Metacognition and learning: Conceptual and methodological considerations. Metacognition and Learning 1: 3–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Veenman, Marcel V. J., Jan J. Elshout, and Joost Meijer. 1997. The generality vs domain-specificity of metacognitive skills in novice learning across domains. Learning and Instruction 7: 187–209. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Veenman, Marcel V. J., Laura Bavelaar, Levina De Wolf, and Marieke G. P. Van Haaren. 2014. The on-line assessment of metacognitive kills in a computerized learning environment. Learning and Individual Differences 29: 123–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Veenman, Marcel V. J., Pascal Wilhelm, and Jos J. Beishuizen. 2004. The Relation between intellectual and metacognitive skills from a developmental perspective. Learning and Instruction 14: 89–109. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Veenman, Marcel, and Jan J. Elshout. 1999. Changes in the relation between cognitive and metacognitive skills during the acquisition of expertise. European Journal of Psychology of Education 14: 509–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Verschaffel, Lieven, Koen Luwel, Joke Torbeyns, and Wim Van Dooren. 2007. Developing adaptive expertise: A feasible and valuable goal for (elementary) mathematics education? Ciencias Psicológicas 1: 27–35. [Google Scholar]
- Verschaffel, Lieven, Koen Luwel, Joke Torbeyns, and Wim Van Dooren. 2009. Conceptualizing, investigating, and enhancing adaptive expertise in elementary mathematics education. European Journal of Psychology of Education 24: 335–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Verschaffel, Lieven, Wim Van Dooren, Brian Greer, and Swapna Mukhopadhyay. 2010. Reconceptualising word problems as exercises in mathematical modelling. Journal Für Mathematik-Didaktik 31: 9–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wagoner, Shirley A. 1983. Comprehension monitoring: What it is and what we know about it. Reading Research Quarterly 18: 328–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Walczyk, Jeffrey J. 1995. Testing a compensatory-encoding model. Reading Research Quarterly 30: 396–408. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Webel, Corey, and Samuel Otten. 2015. Teaching in a world with PhotoMath. The Mathematics Teacher 109: 368–73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zamarian, Laura, Christoph Scherfler, Christian Kremser, Marie-Theres Pertl, Elke Gizewski, Thomas Benke, and Margarete Delazer. 2018. Arithmetic learning in advanced age. PLoS ONE 13: e0193529. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Zarnhofer, Sabrina, Verena Clemens, Franz Ebner, Karl Koschutnig, Christa Neuper, Manuel Ninaus, Gernot Reishofer, and Anja Ischebeck. 2013. Individual differences in solving arithmetic word problems. Behavioral and Brain Functions 9: 1–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Zhang, Weiwei, Donglan Zhang, and Lawrence Jun Zhang. 2021. Metacognitive instruction for sustainable learning: Learners’ perceptions of task difficulty and use of metacognitive strategies in completing integrated speaking tasks. Sustainability 13: 6275. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zimmerman, Corinne. 2007. The development of scientific thinking skills in elementary and middle school. Developmental Review 27: 172–223. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zohar, Anat, and Adi Ben David. 2009. Paving a clear path in a thick forest: A conceptual analysis of a metacognitive component. Metacognition and Learning 4: 177–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zur, Osnat, and Rochel Gelman. 2004. Young children can add and subtract by predicting and checking. Early Childhood Research Quarterly 19: 121–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. |
© 2022 by the author. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Csíkos, C. Metacognitive and Non-Metacognitive Processes in Arithmetic Performance: Can There Be More than One Meta-Level? J. Intell. 2022, 10, 53. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence10030053
Csíkos C. Metacognitive and Non-Metacognitive Processes in Arithmetic Performance: Can There Be More than One Meta-Level? Journal of Intelligence. 2022; 10(3):53. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence10030053
Chicago/Turabian StyleCsíkos, Csaba. 2022. "Metacognitive and Non-Metacognitive Processes in Arithmetic Performance: Can There Be More than One Meta-Level?" Journal of Intelligence 10, no. 3: 53. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence10030053
APA StyleCsíkos, C. (2022). Metacognitive and Non-Metacognitive Processes in Arithmetic Performance: Can There Be More than One Meta-Level? Journal of Intelligence, 10(3), 53. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence10030053