Fluid Intelligence Emerges from Representing Relations
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Psychometric Studies on Fluid Intelligence
3. Theoretical Limits of Psychometric Studies
4. What Is Needed for a Task to Become a Fluid Intelligence Test?
5. Computational Models Which Process Structures and Relations
6. Fluid Intelligence and Relational Representations
7. Conclusions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Alexander, William H., and Joshua W. Brown. 2011. Medial prefrontal cortex as an action-outcome predictor. Nature Neuroscience 14: 1338–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Andrews, Glenda, and Graeme S. Halford. 2002. A cognitive complexity metric applied to cognitive development. Cognitive Psychology 45: 153–219. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Arden, Rosalind, and Mark J. Adams. 2016. A general intelligence factor in dogs. Intelligence 55: 79–85. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Arendasy, Martin, Andreas Hergovich, and Markus Sommer. 2008. Investigating the ‘g’ saturation of various stratum-two factors using automatic item generation. Intelligence 36: 574–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Arendasy, Martin E., and Markus Sommer. 2013. Reducing response elimination strategies enhances the construct validity of figural matrices. Intelligence 41: 234–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bahramisharif, Ali, Ole Jensen, Joshua Jacobs, and John Lisman. 2018. Serial representation of items during working memory maintenance at letter-selective cortical sites. PLoS Biology 16: e2003805. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Barbey, Aron K. 2018. Network neuroscience theory of human intelligence. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 22: 8–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Bateman, Joel E., Kate A. Thompson, and Damian P. Birney. 2019. Validating the relation-monitoring task as a measure of relational integration and predictor of fluid intelligence. Memory & Cognition 47: 1457–68. [Google Scholar]
- Bays, Paul M. 2015. Spikes not slots: Noise in neural populations limits working memory. Trends in Cognitive Science 19: 431–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Becker, Nicolas, Florian Schmitz, Anke M. Falk, Jasmin Feldbrugge, Daniel R. Recktenwald, Oliver Wilhelm, Franzis Preckel, and Frank M. Spinath. 2016. Preventing response elimination strategies improves the convergent validity of figural matrices. Journal of Intelligence 4: 2. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Bethell-Fox, Charles E., David F. Lohman, and Richard E. Snow. 1984. Adaptive reasoning: Componential and eye movement analysis of geometric analogy performance. Intelligence 8: 205–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Botvinick, Matthew M., Todd S. Braver, Deanna M. Barch, Cameron S. Carter, and Jonathan D. Cohen. 2001. Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. Psychological Review 108: 624–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Brady, Timothy F., Talia Konkle, and George A. Alvarez. 2011. A review of visual memory capacity: Beyond individual items and toward structured representations. Journal of Vision 11: 4. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Buzsaki, Gyorgy. 2006. Rhythms of the Brain. Oxford: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Carlstedt, Berit, Jan-Eric Gustafsson, and Eva Ullstadius. 2000. Item sequencing effects on the measurement of fluid intelligence. Intelligence 28: 145–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Carpenter, Patricia A., Marcel A. Just, and Peter Shell. 1990. What one intelligence test measures: A theoretical account of the processing in the Raven progressive matrices test. Psychological Review 97: 404–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Carroll, John B. 1993. Human Cognitive Abilities: A Survey of Factor-Analytic Studies. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Cattell, Raymond B. 1943. The measurement of adult intelligence. Psychological Bulletin 40: 153–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cattell, Raymond B. 1949. Culture Free Intelligence Test, Scale 1, Handbook. Champaign: IPAT. [Google Scholar]
- Cattell, Raymond B. 1963. Theory of fluid and crystallized intelligence: A critical experiment. Journal of Educational Psychology 54: 1–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chuderski, Adam. 2013. When are fluid intelligence and working memory isomorphic and when are they not? Intelligence 41: 244–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chuderski, Adam. 2014. The relational integration task explains fluid reasoning above and beyond other working memory tasks. Memory & Cognition 42: 448–63. [Google Scholar]
- Chuderski, Adam. 2015a. The broad factor of working memory is virtually isomorphic to fluid intelligence tested under time pressure. Personality and Individual Differences 85: 98–104. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chuderski, Adam. 2015b. Why people fail on the fluid intelligence tests. Journal of Individual Differences 36: 138–49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chuderski, Adam. 2019. Even a single trivial binding of information is critical for fluid intelligence. Intelligence 77: 101396. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chuderski, Adam, and Krzysztof Andrelczyk. 2015. From neural oscillations to complex cognition: Simulating the effect of the theta-to-gamma cycle length ratio on analogical reasoning. Cognitive Psychology 76: 78–102. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Chuderski, Adam, Krzysztof Andrelczyk, and Tomasz Smoleń. 2013. An oscillatory model of individual differences in working memory capacity and relational integration. Cognitive Systems Research 24: 87–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chuderski, Adam, and Jan Jastrzębski. 2018. Much ado about Aha! Insight problem solving is strongly related to working memory capacity and reasoning ability. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 147: 257–81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chuderski, Adam, and Tomasz Smoleń. 2016. An integrated model of utility-based evaluation and resolution of conflicts in the Stroop task. Psychological Review 123: 255–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chuderski, Adam, Maciej Taraday, Edward Nęcka, and Tomasz Smoleń. 2012. Storage capacity explains fluid intelligence but executive control does not. Intelligence 40: 278–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Clevenger, Pamela E., and John E. Hummel. 2014. Working memory for relations among objects. Attention, Perception & Psychophysics 76: 1933–53. [Google Scholar]
- Cohen, Michael X. 2011. It’s about time. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 5: 2. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Colom, Roberto, Francisco J. Abad, M. Angeles Quiroga, Pei C. Shih, and Carmen Flores-Mendoza. 2008. Working memory and intelligence are highly related constructs but why? Intelligence 36: 584–606. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Conway, Andrew R., Nelson Cowan, Michael F. Bunting, David J. Therriault, and Scott R. Minkoff. 2002. A latent variable analysis of working memory capacity, short-term memory capacity, processing speed, and general fluid intelligence. Intelligence 30: 163–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cowan, Nelson. 2022. Working memory development: A 50-year assessment of research and underlying theories. Cognition 224: 105075. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Deary, Ian J. 1994. Sensory Discrimination and Intelligence: Postmortem or Resurrection? American Journal of Psychology 107: 95–115. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Deary, Ian J., P. Joseph Bell, Andrew J. Bell, Mary L. Campbell, and Nicola D. Fazal. 2004. Sensory discrimination and intelligence: Testing Spearman’s other hypothesis. American Journal of Psychology 117: 1–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Deary, Ian J., Lars Penke, and Wendy Johnson. 2010. The neuroscience of human intelligence differences. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 11: 201–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Diamond, Adele. 2013. Executive functions. Annual Review of Psychology 64: 135–68. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Doebler, Phillip, and Barbara Scheffler. 2016. The relationship of choice reaction time variability and intelligence: A meta-analysis. Learning and Individual Differences 52: 157–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Doumas, Leonidas A. A., and John E. Hummel. 2005. Approaches to modeling human mental representations: What works, what doesn’t and why. In The Cambridge Handbook of Thinking and Reasoning. Edited by Keith J. Holyoak and Robert Morrison. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 73–91. [Google Scholar]
- Doumas, Leonidas A. A., John E. Hummel, and Catherine M. Sandhofer. 2008. A theory of the discovery and predication of relational concepts. Psychological Review 115: 1–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Draheim, Christopher, Jason S. Tsukahara, Jessie D. Martin, Cody A. Mashburn, and Randall W. Engle. 2021. A toolbox approach to improving the measurement of attention control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 150: 242–75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Duncan, John, Daphne Chylinski, Daniel J. Mitchell, and Apoorva Bhandari. 2017. Complexity and compositionality in fluid intelligence. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 114: 5295–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Engle, Randall W., Stephen W. Tuholski, James E. Laughlin, and Andrew R. A. Conway. 1999. Working memory, short-term memory, and general fluid intelligence: A latent-variable approach. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 128: 309–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Estrada, Eduardo, Francisco J. Román, Francisco J. Abad, and Roberto Colom. 2017. Separating power and speed components of standardized intelligence measures. Intelligence 61: 159–68. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Forbus, Kenneth D., Ronald W. Fergusson, and Dedre Gentner. 1994. Incremental structure mapping. In Proceedings of the Sixteenth Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. Edited by Ashwin Ram and Kurt Eislet. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum, pp. 313–18. [Google Scholar]
- Fougnie, Daryl, and George A. Alvarez. 2011. Object features fail independently in visual working memory: Evidence for a probabilistic feature-store model. Journal of Vision 11: 3. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Frischkorn, Gideon. T., and Claudia C. von Bastian. 2021. In search of the executive cognitive processes proposed by Process-Overlap Theory. Journal of Intelligence 9: 43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Galton, Francis. 1883. Inquiries into Human Faculty. London: Dent. [Google Scholar]
- Gągol, Adam, Mikołaj Magnuski, Bartłomiej Kroczek, Patrycja Kałamała, Michał Ociepka, Emiliano Santarnecchi, and Adam Chuderski. 2018. Delta-gamma coupling as a potential neurophysiological mechanism of fluid intelligence. Intelligence 66: 54–63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gentner, Dedre. 1983. Structure mapping: A theoretical framework for analogy. Cognitive Science 7: 155–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gilbert, J. Allen. 1894. Researches on the mental and physical development of schoolchildren. Studies of Yale Psychological Laboratory 2: 40–100. [Google Scholar]
- Goodwin, Geoffrey P., and Phillip N. Johnson-Laird. 2008. Transitive and pseudotransitive inferences. Cognition 108: 320–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gustafsson, Jan-Eric. 1984. A unifying model for the structure of intellectual abilities. Intelligence 8: 179–203. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Haier, Richard J. 2016. The Neuroscience of Intelligence. New York: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Halford, Graeme S., Glenda Andrews, and William H. Wilson. 2015. Relational processing in reasoning: The role of working memory. In Reasoning as Memory. Edited by Aidan Feeney and Valerie A. Thompson. Hove: Psychology Press, pp. 34–52. [Google Scholar]
- Halford, Graeme S., William H. Wilson, and Steven Phillips. 1998. Processing capacity defined by relational complexity: Implications for comparative, developmental, and cognitive psychology. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 21: 803–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Halford, Graeme S., William H. Wilson, and Steven Phillips. 2010. Relational knowledge: The foundation of higher cognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 14: 497–505. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hanslmayr, Simon, Nikolai Axmacher, and Cory S. Imman. 2019. Modulating human memory via entrainment of brain oscillations. Trends in Neurosciences 42: 485–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Harrison, Tyler L., Zach Shipstead, and Randall W. Engle. 2015. Why is working memory capacity related to matrix reasoning tasks? Memory & Cognition 43: 389–96. [Google Scholar]
- Hayes, Steven C., Dermot Barnes-Holmes, and Bryan Roche. 2001. Relational Frame Theory: A Post-Skinnerian Account of Human Language and Cognition. New York: Plenum Press. [Google Scholar]
- Hayes, Taylor R., Alexander D. Petrov, and Per B. Sederberg. 2015. Do we really become smarter when our fluid-intelligence scores improve? Intelligence 48: 1–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Hedge, Craig, Georgina Powell, and Petroc Sumner. 2018. The reliability paradox: Why robust cognitive tasks do not produce reliable individual differences. Behavior Research Methods 50: 1166–86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hofstadter, Douglas R. 2001. Analogy as the Core of Cognition. In The Analogical Mind: Perspectives from Cognitive Science. Edited by Dedre Gentner, Keith J. Holyoak and Boicho N. Kokinov. Cambridge: The MIT Press/Bradford Book, pp. 499–538. [Google Scholar]
- Holroyd, Clay B., Nick Yeung, Michael G. H. Coles, and Jonathan D. Cohen. 2005. A mechanism for error detection in speeded response time tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 134: 163–91. [Google Scholar]
- Holyoak, Keith J. 2012. Analogy and relational reasoning. In The Oxford Handbook of Thinking and Reasoning. Edited by Keith J. Holyoak and Robert G. Morrison. New York: Oxford University Press, pp. 234–59. [Google Scholar]
- Horn, John L., and Raymond B. Cattell. 1966. Refinement and test of the theory of fluid and crystallized general intelligences. Journal of Educational Psychology 57: 253–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Horn, David, and Marius Usher. 1991. Parallel activation of memories in an oscillatory neural network. Neural Computation 3: 31–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hummel, John E., and Keith J. Holyoak. 1997. Distributed representations of structure: A theory of analogical access and mapping. Psychological Review 104: 427–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hummel, John E., and Keith J. Holyoak. 2003. A symbolic-connectionist theory of relational inference and generalization. Psychological Review 110: 220–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Hunt, Earl B., Clifford Lunneborg, and Joe Lewis. 1975. What does it mean to be high verbal? Cognitive Psychology 1: 194–227. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jastrzębski, Jan, Bartłomiej Kroczek, and Adam Chuderski. 2021. Galton and Spearman revisited: Can single general discrimination ability drive performance on diverse sensorimotor tasks and explain intelligence? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 150: 1279–302. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jastrzębski, Jan, Michał Ociepka, and Adam Chuderski. 2020. Fluid intelligence is equivalent to relation processing. Intelligence 82: 101–489. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jastrzębski, Jan, Michał Ociepka, and Adam Chuderski. 2022. Graph Mapping: A novel and simple test to validly assess fluid reasoning. Behavior Research Methods, 1–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jensen, Arthur R. 2006. Clocking the Mind: Mental Chronometer Individual Differences. Amsterdam: Elsevier. [Google Scholar]
- Jensen, Arthur R., and Ella Munro. 1979. Reaction time, movement time, and intelligence. Intelligence 3: 121–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jensen, Ole, Yali Pan, Steven Frisson, and Lin Wang. 2021. An oscillatory pipelining mechanism supporting previewing during visual exploration and reading. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 25: 1033–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Johnson-Laird, Phillip N. 2006. How We Reason? Oxford: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Kan, Kes-Jan, Rogier A. Kievit, Conor Dolan, and Han van der Maas. 2011. On the interpretation of the CHC factor Gc. Intelligence 39: 292–302. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Keane, Mark T., Tim Ledgeway, and Stuart Duff. 1994. Constraints on analogical mapping: A comparison of three models. Cognitive Science 18: 387–438. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Knowlton, Barbara J., Robert G. Morrison, John E. Hummel, and Keith J. Holyoak. 2012. A neurocomputational system for relational reasoning. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 16: 373–81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Koene, Randal, and Michael Hasselmo. 2007. First-in-first-out item replacement in a model of short-term memory based on persistent spiking. Cerebral Cortex 17: 1766–81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Kovacs, Kristof, and Andrew R. A. Conway. 2016. Process overlap theory: A unified account of the general factor of intelligence. Psychological Inquiry 27: 151–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kvist, Ann V., and Jan-Eric Gustafsson. 2008. The relation between fluid intelligence and the general factor as a function of cultural background: A test of Cattell’s Investment theory. Intelligence 36: 422–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Kyllonen, Patrick, and Raymond E. Christal. 1990. Reasoning ability is (little more than) working memory capacity? Intelligence 433: 389–433. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lakin, Joni M., and James L. Gambrell. 2012. Distinguishing verbal, quantitative, and figural facets of fluid intelligence in young students. Intelligence 40: 560–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Levacher, Julie, Marco Koch, Johanna Hissbach, Frank M. Spinath, and Nicolas Becker. 2022. You can play the game without knowing the rules—But you’re better off knowing them: The influence of rule knowledge on figural matrices tests. European Journal of Psychological Assessment 38: 15–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Li, Shu-Chen, Malina Jordanova, and Ulman Lindenberger. 1998. From good senses to good sense: A link between tactile information processing and intelligence. Intelligence 26: 99–122. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Lisman, John E., and Marco A. P. Idiart. 1995. Storage of 7 ± 2 short-term memories in oscillatory subcycles. Science 267: 1512–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Little, Daniel R., Stephan Lewandowsky, and Stewart Craig. 2014. Working memory capacity and fluid abilities: The more difficult the item, the more more is better. Frontiers in Psychology 5: 239. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Loesche, Patrick, Jennifer Wiley, and Marcus Hasselhorn. 2015. How knowing the rules affects solving the Raven progressive matrices test. Intelligence 48: 58–75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lovett, Andrew, and Kenneth Forbus. 2017. Modeling visual problem solving as analogical reasoning. Psychological Review 124: 60–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Lovett, Marsha C. 2005. A strategy-based interpretation of Stroop. Cognitive Science 29: 493–524. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Lozano, José H. 2015. Are impulsivity and intelligence truly related constructs? Evidence based on the fixed-links model. Personality and Individual Differences 85: 192–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lozano, Jose H., and Javier Revuelta. 2020. Investigating operation-specific learning effects in the Raven’s Advanced Progressive Matrices: A linear logistic test modeling approach. Intelligence 82: 101468. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lu, Ying, and Stephen G. Sireci. 2007. Validity issues in test speededness. Educational Measurement: Issues and Practice 26: 29–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Markman, Arthur B., and Dedre Gentner. 1993. Structural alignment during similarity comparisons. Cognitive Psychology 25: 431–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- McGrew, Kevin S. 2009. CHC theory and the human cognitive abilities project: Standing on the shoulders of the giants of psychometric intelligence research. Intelligence 37: 1–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McLoughlin, Shane, Ian Tyndall, and Antonina Pereira. 2020. Convergence of multiple fields on a relational reasoning approach to cognition. Intelligence 83: 101491. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Meyer, Christine. S., Priska Hagmann-von Arx, Sakari Lemola, and Alexander Grob. 2010. Correspondence between the general ability to discriminate sensory stimuli and general intelligence. Journal of Individual Differences 31: 46–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Neisser, Urlic. 1967. Cognitive Psychology. Englewood Cliffs: Prentice-Hall. [Google Scholar]
- Neubauer, Aljoscha C. 1990. Speed of information processing in the Hick paradigm and response latencies in a psychometric intelligence test. Personality and Individual Differences 11: 147–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Oaksford, Mike, and Nick Chater. 2007. Bayesian Rationality: The Probabilistic Approach to Human Reasoning. Oxford: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Oberauer, Klaus. 1993. Die Koordination kognitiver Operationen. Eine Studie zum Zusammenhang von “working-memory” und Intelligenz. Zeitschrift für Psychologie 201: 57–84. [Google Scholar]
- Oberauer, Klaus. 2016. Parameters, not processes, explain general intelligence. Psychological Inquiry 27: 231–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Oberauer, Klaus, Stephan Lewandowsky, Edward Awh, Gordon D. A. Brown, Andrew Conway, Nelson Cowan, Christopher Donkin, Simon Farrell, Graham J. Hitch, Mark J. Hurlstone, and et al. 2018. Benchmarks for models of short-term and working memory. Psychological Bulletin 144: 885–958. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Oberauer, K., and Hsuan-Yu Lin. 2017. An interference model of visual working memory. Psychological Review 124: 21–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Oberauer, Klaus, Ralf Schultze, Oliver Wilhelm, and Heinz-Martin Süß. 2005. Working memory and intelligence—Their correlation and their relation: Comment on Ackerman, Beier, and Boyle. Psychological Bulletin 131: 61–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Oberauer, Klaus, Heinz-Martin Süß, Oliver Wilhelm, and Werner W. Wittman. 2008. Which working memory functions predict intelligence? Intelligence 36: 641–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- O’Keefe, John, and Michael L. Recce. 1993. Phase relationship between hippocampal place units and the EEG theta rhythm. Hippocampus 3: 317–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Qasim, Salman E., Itzhak Fried, and Joshua Jacobs. 2021. Phase precession in the human hippocampus and entorhinal cortex. Cell 184: 3242–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pomper, Urlich, and Urlich Ansorge. 2021. Theta-rhythmic oscillation of working memory performance. Psychological Science 32: 1801–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Primi, Ricardo. 2002. Complexity of geometric inductive reasoning tasks: Contribution to the understanding of fluid intelligence. Intelligence 30: 41–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Raffone, Antonino, and Gezinus Wolters. 2001. A cortical mechanism for binding in visual memory. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 13: 766–85. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rasmussen, Daniel, and Chris Eliasmith. 2014. A spiking neural model applied to the study of human performance and cognitive decline on Raven’s Advanced Progressive Matrices. Intelligence 42: 53–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Raven, John C. 1938. Progressive Matrices. London: Lewis. [Google Scholar]
- Raven, John C., John. H. Court, and Jean Raven. 1983. Manual for Raven’s Progressive Matrices and Vocabulary Scales (Section 4: Advanced Progressive Matrices). London: H. K. Lewis. [Google Scholar]
- Ren, Xuezhu, Tengfei Wang, Sumin Sun, Mi Deng, and Karl Schweizer. 2018. Speeded testing in the assessment of intelligence gives rise to a speed factor. Intelligence 66: 64–71. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rey-Mermet, Alodie Miriam Gade, Alessandra S. Souza, Claudia C. von Bastian, and Klaus Oberauer. 2019. Is executive control related to working memory capacity and fluid intelligence? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 148: 1335–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Roelofs, Ardi. 2003. Goal-referenced selection of verbal action: Modeling attentional control in the Stroop task. Psychological Review 110: 88–125. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Sala, Giovanni, N. Deniz Aksayli, K. Semir Tatlidil, Tomoko Tatsumi, Yasuyuki Gondo, and Fernand Gobet. 2019. Near and far transfer in cognitive training: A second-order meta-analysis. Collabra: Psychology 5: 18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Salthouse, Timothy. 1993. Relations between running memory and fluid intelligence. Intelligence 43: 1–7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Sauseng, Paul, Charline Peylo, Anna L. Biel, Elisabeth V. C. Friedrich, and Carola Romberg-Taylor. 2019. Does cross-frequency phase coupling of oscillatory brain activity contribute to a better understanding of visual working memory? British Journal of Psychology 110: 245–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schmidt, James R. 2013. The Parallel Episodic Processing (PEP) model: Dissociating contingency and conflict adaptation in the item-specific proportion congruent paradigm. Acta Psychologica 142: 119–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Schubert, Anna-Lena. 2019. A meta-analysis of the worst performance rule. Intelligence 73: 88–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Schubert, Anna-Lena, and Gideon. T. Frischkorn. 2020. Neurocognitive psychometrics of intelligence: How measurement advancements unveiled the role of mental speed in intelligence differences. Current Directions in Psychological Science 29: 140–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schulze, Doreen, Andre Beauducel, and Burkhard Brocke. 2005. Semantically meaningful and abstract figural reasoning in the context of fluid and crystallized intelligence. Intelligence 33: 143–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schweizer, Karl. 2010. The relationship of attention and intelligence. In Handbook of Individual Differences in Cognition: Attention, Memory, and Executive Control. Edited by Aleksandra Gruszka, Garry Matthiews and Błażej Szymura. New York: Springer, pp. 247–62. [Google Scholar]
- Schweizer, Karl, Stephan J. Troche, and Thomas H. Rammsayer. 2018. Does processing speed exert an influence on the special relationship of fluid and general intelligence? Personality and Individual Differences 131: 57–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Shastri, Lokendra, and Venkat Ajjanagadde. 1993. From simple associations to systematic reasoning: A connectionist representation of rules, variables and dynamic bindings using temporal synchrony. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16: 417–94. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Sheppard, Leah D., and Philip A. Vernon. 2008. Intelligence and speed of information-processing: A review of 50 years of research. Personality and Individual Differences 44: 535–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Shokri-Kojori, Ehsan, and Daniel C. Krawczyk. 2018. Signatures of multiple processes contributing to fluid reasoning performance. Intelligence 68: 87–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Siegel, Markus, Melissa R. Warden, and Earl K. Miller. 2009. Phase-dependent neuronal coding of objects in short-term memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 106: 21341–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Sligte, Ilja G., Annelinde R. E. Vandenbroucke, H. Steven Scholte, and Victor A. F. Lamme. 2010. Detailed sensory memory, sloppy working memory. Frontiers in Psychology 1: 175. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Smoleń, Tomasz, and Adam Chuderski. 2015. The quadratic relationship between difficulty of intelligence test items and their correlations with working memory. Frontiers in Psychology 6: 1270. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Snow, Richard E., Patrick C. Kyllonen, and Brian Marshalek. 1984. The topography of ability and learning correlations. In Advances in the Psychology of Human Intelligence. Edited by Robert J. Sternberg. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, vol. 2, pp. 47–103. [Google Scholar]
- Spearman, Charles. 1927. The Abilities of Man. London: Macmillan. [Google Scholar]
- Stankov, Lazar. 1983. Attention and intelligence. Journal of Educational Psychology 75: 471–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Thissen, Alica, Marco Koch, Nicolas Becker, and Frank M. Spinath. 2018. Construct your own response: The cube construction task as a novel format for the assessment of spatial ability. European Journal of Psychological Assessment 34: 304–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Thomson, Godfrey H. 1919. The hierarchy of abilities. British Journal of Psychology 9: 337–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Thurstone, Louis L. 1938. Primary Mental Abilities. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. [Google Scholar]
- Troche, Stephan J., and Thomas H. Rammsayer. 2009. The influence of temporal resolution power and working memory capacity on psychometric intelligence. Intelligence 37: 489–96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Troche, Stephan. J., Felicitas L. Wagner, Annik E. Voelke, Claudia. M. Roebers, and Thomas. H. Rammsayer. 2014. Individual differences in working memory capacity explain the relationship between general discrimination ability and psychometric intelligence. Intelligence 44: 40–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Unsworth, Nash. 2019. Individual differences in long-term memory. Psychological Bulletin 145: 79–139. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Unsworth, Nash, Keisuke Fukuda, Edward Awh, and Edward K. Vogel. 2014. Working memory and fluid intelligence: Capacity, attention control, and secondary memory. Cognitive Psychology 71: 1–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Unsworth, Nash, Thomas S. Redick, Chad E. Lakey, and Diana L. Young. 2010. Lapses in sustained attention and their relation to executive and fluid abilities: An individual differences investigation. Intelligence 38: 111–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Usher, Markus, Jonathan D. Cohen, Henk Haarmann, and David Horn. 2001. Neural mechanism for the magical number 4: Competitive interactions and nonlinear oscillation. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 24: 151–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jarosz, Andrew F., and Jennifer Wiley. 2012. Why does working memory capacity predict RAPM performance? A possible role of distraction. Intelligence 40: 427–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jäger, Adolf O. 1982. Mehrmodale Klassifikation von Intelligenzleistungen: Experimentell kontrollierte Weiterentwicklung eines deskriptiven Intelligenz-struktur modells. Diagnostica 28: 195–225. [Google Scholar]
- Wiley, Jennifer, Andrew F. Jarosz, Patrick J. Cushen, and Gregory J. H. Colflesh. 2011. New rule use drives the relation between working memory capacity and Raven’s Advanced Progressive Matrices. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition 37: 256–63. [Google Scholar]
- Wilhelm, Oliver. 2005. Measuring reasoning ability. In Handbook of Understanding and Measuring Intelligence. Edited by O. Wilhelm and R. W. Engle. Thousand Oaks: Sage Publications, Inc., pp. 373–92. [Google Scholar]
- Wilhelm, Oliver, Andrea Hildebrandt, and Klaus Oberauer. 2013. What is working memory, and how can we measure it? Frontiers in Psychology 4: 433. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Williams, Ben A., and Stephen L. Pearlberg. 2006. Learning of three-term contingencies correlates with Raven scores, but not with measures of cognitive processing. Intelligence 34: 177–91. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Van Der Maas, Han L. J., Conor V. Dolan, Raoul P. P. P. Grasman, Jelte M. Wicherts, Hilde M. Huizenga, and Maartje E. J. Raijmakers. 2006. A dynamical model of general intelligence: The positive manifold of intelligence by mutualism. Psychological Review 113: 842–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Verguts, Tom, and Wim Notebaert. 2008. Hebbian learning of cognitive control. Psychological Review 115: 518–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Vernon, Philip E. 1964. The Structure of Human Abilities. London: Methuen. [Google Scholar]
- Vigneau, Francois, Andre F. Caissie, and Douglas A. Bors. 2006. Eye-movement analysis demonstrates strategic influences on intelligence. Intelligence 34: 261–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vogel, Edward K., Geoffrey F. Woodman, and Steven J. Luck. 2001. Storage of features, conjunctions, and objects in visual working memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance 27: 92–114. [Google Scholar]
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. |
© 2022 by the author. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Chuderski, A. Fluid Intelligence Emerges from Representing Relations. J. Intell. 2022, 10, 51. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence10030051
Chuderski A. Fluid Intelligence Emerges from Representing Relations. Journal of Intelligence. 2022; 10(3):51. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence10030051
Chicago/Turabian StyleChuderski, Adam. 2022. "Fluid Intelligence Emerges from Representing Relations" Journal of Intelligence 10, no. 3: 51. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence10030051
APA StyleChuderski, A. (2022). Fluid Intelligence Emerges from Representing Relations. Journal of Intelligence, 10(3), 51. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence10030051