Long-Term Impacts of Early Musical Abilities on Academic Achievement: A Longitudinal Study
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Theoretical Framework
2.1. Music Learning and Academic Achievement
2.2. Musical and Cognitive Abilities
2.3. Early Reading and Mathematical Skills and Early Musical Abilities
2.4. Social Background, School Learning, and Music Learning
3. Objectives
4. Method
4.1. Participants
4.2. Measures
4.2.1. Musical Aptitude Test
4.2.2. Musical Perception and Musical Reproduction
- (1)
- Melody discrimination: deciding whether two successive short, sung melodies were the same or different (6 items);
- (2)
- Chord analysis: determining the number of notes heard at once (5 items);
- (3)
- Tempo discrimination: short, simple piano excerpts with the same or different tempo at repetition (5 items);
- (4)
- Interval discrimination: two successive ascending/descending intervals played on the piano (7 items);
- (5)
- Rhythm discrimination: two successive rhythmic patterns played on a snare drum (6 items);
- (6)
- Timbre discrimination: comparing the sounds of instruments from the same or a different instrument family with a slightly similar timbre (5 items);
- (7)
- Chord discrimination: two successive chords (6 items);
- (8)
- Dynamics: comparing the dynamics of five-beat music excerpts played on the piano (2 items).
4.2.3. Pitch-Related Abilities and Rhythm-Related Abilities
- (1)
- Melody discrimination: deciding whether two successive short, sung melodies were the same or different (6 items);
- (2)
- Chord analysis: determining the number of notes heard at once (5 items);
- (3)
- Interval discrimination: two successive ascending/descending intervals played on the piano (7 items);
- (4)
- Chord discrimination: two successive chords (6 items);
- (5)
- Interval-singing (18 items);
- (6)
- Melody-singing (7 items).
- (1)
- Tempo discrimination: short, simple piano excerpts with the same or different tempo at repetition (5 items);
- (2)
- Rhythm discrimination: two successive rhythmic patterns played on a snare drum (6 items);
- (3)
- Rhythm-clapping (8 items).
4.2.4. Intelligence
4.2.5. Word Reading
4.2.6. Elementary Arithmetic Skills
4.2.7. School Marks
4.3. Procedure
5. Results
5.1. Descriptive Statistics
5.2. Intercorrelations of Variables
5.3. Regression Models
5.3.1. Regression Model I
5.3.2. Regression Model II
6. Discussion
7. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Asztalos, Kata, and Benő Csapó. 2017. Development of musical abilities: Cross-sectional computerbased assessments in educational contexts. Psychology of Music 45: 682–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Babo, Gerard D. 2004. The relationship between instrumental music participation and standardized assessment achievement of middle school students. Research Music in Music Education 22: 14–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Barkóczi, Ilona, and Csaba Pléh. 1977. Kodály Zenei Nevelési Módszerének Pszichológiai Hatásvizsgálata [An Examination of the Efficiency of Kodály’s Music Educational Method]. Kecskemét: Kodály Institute. [Google Scholar]
- Berg, Derek H. 2008. Working memory and arithmetic calculation in children: The contributory roles of processing speed, short-term memory, and reading. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 99: 288–308. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Berkowska, Magdalena, and Simone D. Bella. 2009. Acquired and congenital disorders of sung performance: A review. Advances in Cognitive Psychology 5: 69–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Besson, Mireille, Julie Chobert, and Céline Marie. 2011. Transfer of training between music and speech: Common processing, attention, and memory. Frontiers in Psychology 2: 94. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Buckingham, Jennifer, Kevin Wheldall, and Robyn Beaman-Wheldall. 2013. Why poor children are more likely to become poor readers: The school years. Australian Journal of Education 57: 190–213. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bugos, Jennifer A., and Darlene DeMarie. 2017. The effects of a short-term music program on preschool children’s executive functions. Psychology of Music 45: 855–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bugos, Jennifer A., William M. Perlstein, Christina S. McCrae, Timothy. S. Brophy, and Purvis H. Bedenbaugh. 2007. Individualized piano instruction enhances executive functioning and working memory in older adults. Aging & Mental Health 11: 464–71. [Google Scholar]
- Bull, Rebecca, Kimberly A. Espy, and Sandra A. Wiebe. 2008. Short-term memory, working memory, and executive functioning in pre-schoolers: Longitudinal predictors of mathematical achievement at age 7 years. Journal of Developmental Neuropsychology 33: 205–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Butzlaff, Ron. 2000. Can music be used to teach reading? Journal of Aesthetic Education 34: 167–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cabanac, Arnaud, Leonid Perlovsky, Mary-Claude Bonniot-Cabanac, and Michel Cabanac. 2013. Music and academic performance. Behavioural Brain Research 256: 257–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Catterall, James S., Richard Chapleau, and John Iwanaga. 1999. Involvement in the arts and human development: General involvement and intensive involvement in music and theatre arts. In Champions of Change: The Impact of the Arts on Learning. Edited by Edward B. Fiske. Washington: Arts Education Partnership and President’s Committee on the Arts and Humanities, pp. 1–18. [Google Scholar]
- Cheek, Joyce M., and Lyle R. Smith. 1999. Music training and mathematics achievement. Adolescence 34: 759–61. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
- Corrigall, Kathleen A., E. Glenn Schellenberg, and Nicole M. Misura. 2013. Music training, cognition and personality. Frontiers in Psychology 4: 1–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Costa-Giomi, Eugenia. 1999. The effect of three years of piano instruction on children’s cognitive development. Journal of Research in Music Education 47: 198–212. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- David, Dana, Lesly Wade-Woolley, John R. Kirby, and Katharine Smithrim. 2007. Rhythm and reading development in school-age children: A longitudinal study. Journal of Research in Reading 30: 169–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Degé, Franziska, and Gudrun Schwarzer. 2011. The effect of a music program on phonological awareness in preschoolers. Frontiers in Psychology 2: 124. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Degé, Franziska, Claudia Kubicek, and Gudrun Schwarzer. 2011. Music lessons and intelligence: A relation mediated by executive functions. Music Perception 29: 195–201. [Google Scholar]
- Degé, Franziska, Claudia Kubicek, and Gudrun Schwarzer. 2015. Associations between musical abilities and precursors of reading in preschool aged children. Frontiers in Psychology 6: 1220. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Dowling, W. Jay. 2002. The Development of music perception and cognition. In Foundations of Cognitive Psychology: Core Readings. Edited by Daniel J. Levitin. Cambridge: MIT Press, pp. 481–502. [Google Scholar]
- Drake, Carolyn, Mari Riess Jones, and Clarisse Baruch. 2000. The development of rhythmic attending in auditory sequences: Attunement, referent period, focal attending. Cognition 77: 251–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Duncan, Greg J., Chantelle J. Dowsett, Amy Claessens, Katherine Magnuson, Aletha C. Huston, Pamela Klebanov, Linda Pagani, Leon Feinstein, Mimi Engel, Jeanne Brooks-Gunn, and et al. 2007. School readiness and later achievement. Developmental Psychology 43: 1428–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Durand, Marianne, Charles Hulme, Rebecca Larkin, and Margaret Snowling. 2005. The cognitive foundations of reading and arithmetic skills in 7- to 10-year-olds. Journal Experimental Child Psychology 91: 113–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Esteki, Mahnaz. 2013. Effectiveness of “Music Training” on reorganization of brain and poor intellectual abilities in female students with dyscalculia (7-9 years old). Global Journal of Arts Education 3: 16–20. [Google Scholar]
- Frischen, Ulrike, Gudrun Schwarzer, and Franziska Degé. 2019. Comparing the effects of rhythm-based music training and pitch-based music training on executive functions in preschoolers. Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience 13: 41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Gembris, Heiner. 2006. The development of musical abilities. In MENC Handbook of Musical Cognition. Edited by R. Colwell. Oxford: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Gévayné, Janurik Márta. 2010. A Zenei Hallási Képességek Fejlődése és Összefüggése Néhány Alapkészséggel 4–8 Éves Kor Között [Development of Music Listening Skills and Their Relationship to Some Basic Learning Skills between the Ages of 4 and 8]. Ph.D. dissertation, University of Szeged, Szeged, Hungary. [Google Scholar]
- Gordon, Edwin E. 1989. Advanced Measures of Music Audiation. Chicago: GIA Publications. [Google Scholar]
- Gouzouasis, Peter, Martin Guhn, and Nand Kishor. 2007. The predictive relationship between achievement and participation in music and achievement in core Grade 12 academic subjects. Music Education Research 9: 81–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Haley, Jennifer A. 2001. The Relationship between Instrumental Music Instruction and Academic Achievement in Fourth Grade Students. Doctoral dissertation, Pace University, New York, NY, USA. Dissertation Abstracts International 62: 2969A. [Google Scholar]
- Hargreaves, David J., and Alexandar Aksentijevic. 2011. Music, IQ, and the executive function. British Journal of Psychology 102: 306–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Helmbold, Nadin, Stefan Troche, and Thomas Rammsayer. 2006. Temporal information processing and pitch discrimination as predictors of general intelligence. Canadian Journal of Experymental Psychology 60: 294–306. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Helmrich, Barbara H. 2010. Window of opportunity? Adolescence, music, and algebra. Journal of Adolescent Research 25: 557–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ho, Yim-Chi, Mei-Chun Cheung, and Agnes S. Chan. 2003. Music training improves verbal but not visual memory cross-sectional and longitudinal explorations in children. Neuropsychology 17: 439–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Holochwost, Steven J., Cathy B. Propper, Dennis Wolf, Michael T. Willoughby, Kelly R. Fisher, Jacek Kolacz, Vanessa V. Volpe, and Sara Jaffee. 2017. Music education academic achievement, and executive functions. Psychology of Aesthetics Creativity, and the Arts 11: 147–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hurwitz, Irving, Peter H. Wolff, Barrie D. Bortnick, and Klára Kokas. 1975. Non-musical effects of the Kodaly music curriculum in primary grade children. Journal of Learning Disabilities 8: 167–74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Huttenlocher, Peter R. 2002. Neural Plasticity. The Effects of Environment on the Development of the Celebral Cortex. Cambridge: Harvard University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Jackendoff, Ray, and Fred Lerdahl. 2006. The capacity for music: What is it and what’s special about it? Cognition 100: 33–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Janurik, Márta. 2009. Hogyan viszonyulnak az általános és középiskolás tanulók a klasszikus zenéhez? [How do elementary and secondary school students feel about classical music?]. Új Pedagógiai Szemle 59: 47–64. [Google Scholar]
- Janurik, Márta, and Krisztián Józsa. 2012. Findings of a three months long music training programme. Hungarian Educational Research Journal 2: 4. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Janurik, Márta, and Krisztián Józsa. 2013. A zenei képességek fejlődése négy- és nyolcéves kor között. [Development of musical abilities between ages 4 and 8.]. Magyar Pedagógia 113: 75–99. [Google Scholar]
- Janurik, Márta, Norbert Szabó, and Krisztián Józsa. 2019. The relationship of musical perception and the executive function among 7-year-old children. In EDULEARN19 Proceedings: 11th International Conference on Education and New Learning Technologies. Edited by Luis Gómez Chova, Augustin López Martínez and Ignazio Candel Torres. Valencia: IATED Academy, pp. 4818–26. [Google Scholar]
- Jaschke, Artur C., Henkjan Honing, and Erik J. A. Scherder. 2018. Longitudinal analysis of music education on executive functions in primary school children. Frontiers in Neuroscience 12: 103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Jenks, Kathleen M., Ernest C. D. M. van Lieshout, and Jan de Moor. 2009. The relationship between medical impairments and arithmetic development in children with cerebral palsy. Journal of Child Neurology 24: 528–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Johnson, Christopher. M., and Jenny E. Memmott. 2006. Examination of relationships between music programes of differing quality and standardised test results. Journal of Research in Music Education 54: 293–307. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Józsa, Krisztián, and Éva D. Molnár. 2013. The relationship between mastery motivation, self-regulated learning and school success: A Hungarian and wider European perspective. In Handbook of Self-Regulatory Processes in Development: New Directions and International Perspectives. Edited by Karen Caplovitz Barrett, Nathan A. Fox, George A. Morgan, Deborah J. Fidler and Lisa A. Daunhauer. London: Psychology Press, pp. 265–304. [Google Scholar]
- Józsa, Krisztián, and Rita Kelemen. 2007. The development of elementary math: Results form a large scale longitudinal study. In 12th European Conference for Research on Learning and Instruction: Developing Potentials for Learning. Edited by Benő Csapó and Csaba Csíkos. Szeged: University of Szeged, p. 80. [Google Scholar]
- Kells, Deanne. 2008. The Impact of Music on Mathematics Achievement. Available online: https://media2.kindermusik.com/website/2016/11/Research_Schools_Kindermusik_Impact-of-MM-on-SocialEmotionalDevelopment_2016.pdf (accessed on 20 July 2020).
- Kinney, Daryl V. 2008. Selected demographic variables, school music participation, and achievement test scores of urban middle school students. Journal of Research of Music Education 56: 145–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Krajewski, Kristin, and Wolfgang Schneider. 2009. Exploring the impact of phonological awareness, visual-spatial working memory, and preschool quantity-number competencies on mathematics achievement in elementary school: Findings from a 3-year- longitudinal study. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 103: 516–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Leisuk, Teresa. 2015. Music perception ability of children with executive function deficits. Psychology of Music 43: 530–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lukács, Borbála, and Ferenc Hombolygó. 2019. Task-dependent mechanisms in the perception of music and speech: Domain-specific transfer effects of elementary school music education. Journal of Research in Music Education 67: 153–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McMullen, Erin, and Jenny R. Saffran. 2004. Music and language: A developmental comparison. Music Perception 21: 289–311. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Melby-Lervag, Monica, Solveig-Alma H. Lyster, and Charles Hulme. 2012. Phonological skills and their role in learning to read: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin 138: 322–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Miendlarzewska, Ewa A., and Wiebke J. Trost. 2013. How musical training affects cognitive development: Rhythm, reward and other modulating variables. Frontiers in Neuroscience 7: 279. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Miksza, Peter. 2007. Music participation and socioeconomic status as correlates of change: A longitudinal analysis of academic achievement. Bulletin of the Council for Research in Music Education 172: 41–58. [Google Scholar]
- Moreno, Sylvain, Carlos Marques, Andreia Santos, Manuela Santos, Sao L. Castro, and Mireille Besson. 2009. Musical training influences linguistic abilities in 8-year-old children: More evidence for brain plasticity. Cerebral Cortex 19: 712–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Moreno, Sylvain, Deanna Friesen, and Ellen Bialystok. 2011. Effect of music training on promoting preliteracy skills: Preliminary causal evidence. Music Perception 29: 165–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Morgan, Paul L., George Farkas, Marianne M. Hillemeier, and Steven Maczuga. 2009. Risk factors for learning-related behavior problems at 24 months of age: Population-based estimates. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology 37: 401–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Morrison, Steven J. 1994. Music students and academic growth. Music Educators Journal 81: 33–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Moyeda, Iris X. G., Ixtlixóchitl C. Gómez, and María T. P. Flores. 2006. Implementing a musical program to promote preschool children’s vocabulary development. Early Childhood Research and Practice 8: n1. [Google Scholar]
- Nagy, József. 2004. A szóolvasó készség fejlődésének kritériumorientált diagnosztikus feltérképezése [The diagnostic and criterion-referenced assessment of the development of the skill of word reading]. Magyar Pedagógia 104: 123–42. [Google Scholar]
- Neisser, Ulric, Gwyneth Boodoo, Thomas J. Bouchard Jr., A. Wade Boykin, Nathan Brody, Stephen J. Ceci, Diane Halpern, John C. Loehlin, Robert Perloff, Robert J. Stenberg, and et al. 1996. Intelligence: Knowns and unknowns. American Psychologist 51: 77–101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nisbet, Steven. 1991. Mathematics and music. The Australian Mathematics Teacher 47: 4–8. [Google Scholar]
- Okada, Brooke M., and L. Robert Slevc. 2020. Musical training: Contributions to executive function. In An Integrative Approach to Cognitive and Working Memory Training: Perspectives from Psychology, Neuroscience, and Human Development. Edited by Michael F. Bunting, Jared M. Novick, Michael R. Dougherty and Randall W. Engle. Oxford: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Overy, Katie, Roderick I. Nicolson, Angela J. Fawcett, and Eric F. Clarke. 2003. Dyslexia and music: Measuring musical timing skills. Dyslexia 9: 18–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Patel, Aniruddh D. 2012. Language, music, and the brain: A resource-sharing framework. In Language and Music as Cognitive Systems. Edited by Patrick Rebuschat, Martin Rohrmeier, John Hawkins and Ian Cross. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 204–23. [Google Scholar]
- Peretz, Isabelle. 2009. Music, language and modularity framed in action. Psychologica Belgica 49: 157–75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Portowitz, Adena, Kylie A. Peppler, and Mike Downton. 2014. In Harmony: A technology-based music education model to enhance musical understanding and general learning skills. International Journal of Music Education 32: 242–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Raven, John, J. C. Raven, and John H. Court. 1998. Manual for Raven’s Progressive Matrices and Vocabulary Scales. Oxford: Oxford Psychologists Press. [Google Scholar]
- Santos-Luiz, Carlos, Lisete S. M. Mónico, Leandro S. Almeida, and Daniela Coimbra. 2015. Exploring the long-term associations between adolescents’ music training and academic achievement. Musicae Scientiae 20: 512–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Schellenberg, E. Glenn. 2004. Music lessons enhance IQ. Psychological Science 15: 511–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schellenberg, E. Glenn. 2006. Long-term positive associations between music lessons and IQ. Journal of Educational Psychology 98: 457–68. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Schellenberg, E. Glenn. 2011. Examining the association between music lessons and intelligence. British Journal of Psychology 102: 283–302. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Schellenberg, Glenn, and Michael W. Weiss. 2013. Music and cognitive abilities. In The Psychology of Music. Edited by Diana Deutsch. Cambridge: Academic Press, pp. 499–550. [Google Scholar]
- Schneider, Wolfgang, and Jan C. Näslund. 1999. Impact of early phonological processing skills on reading and spelling in school: Evidence from the Munich longitudinal study. In Individual Development from 3 to 12: Findings from the Munich Longitudinal Study. Edited by Franz E. Weinert and Wolfgang Schneider. New York: Cambridge University Press, pp. 126–47. [Google Scholar]
- Shuter-Dyson, Rosamund. 1999. Musical ability. In The Psychology of Music, 2nd ed. Edited by Diana Deutsch. San Diego: Cambridge University Press, pp. 627–51. [Google Scholar]
- Sloboda, John. 1994. Music performance: Expression and the development of exccellence. In Musical Perceptions. Edited by Rita Aiello and John Sloboda. New York: Oxford University Press, pp. 152–69. [Google Scholar]
- Spelke, Elizabeth. 2008. Effects of music instruction on developing cognitive systems at the foundations of mathematics and science. In Learning, Arts, and the Brain. Edited by Carolyn Asbury and Barbara Rich. New York: The Dana Foundation, pp. 17–49. [Google Scholar]
- Surján, Noémi, and Márta Janurik. 2018. A zenei észlelés fejlettségének vizsgálata hagyományos és számítógépes tesztfelvétellel. [Examining the development of musical perception with traditional and computer-based testing.]. Gyermeknevelés 6: 32–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tierney, Adam, and Nina Kraus. 2013a. Music training for the development of reading skills. In Progress in Brain Research. Edited by Michael M. Merzenich, Mor Nahum and Thomas M. Van Vleet. Cambridge: Academic Press, vol. 207, pp. 209–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tierney, Adam, and Nina Kraus. 2013b. The ability to tap to a beat relates to cognitive, linguistic, and perceptual skills. Brain and Language 124: 225–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Vaughn, Kathryn. 2000. Music and mathematics: Modest support for the oft-claimed relationship. Journal of Aesthetic Education 34: 149–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wenger, Win, and Susan H. Wenger. 1990. Training music sight-reading and perfect pitch in young children, as a way to enhance their intelligence. Journal of the Society for Accelerative Learning and Teaching 15: 77–89. [Google Scholar]
- Wong, Patrick C. M., and Tyler K. Perrachione. 2007. Learning pitch patterns in lexical identification by native English-speaking adults. Applied Psycholinguistics 28: 565–85. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Young, Laura N., Sara Cordes, and Ellen Winner. 2014. Arts involments predicts academic achievement only when the child has a musical instrument. Educational Psychology: An Internationak Journal of Experimental Psychology 34: 849–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zanutto, Daniel R. 1997. The effect of instrumental music instruction on academic achievement (high school students). Doctoral dissertation, The University of California at Davis, Davis, CA, USA. Dissertation Abstracts International 58: 3871A. [Google Scholar]
- Ziegler, Johannes C., and Usha Goswami. 2005. Reading acquisition, developmental dyslexia, and skilled reading across languages: A psycholinguistic grain size theory. Psychological Bulletin 131: 3–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Holliman, Andrew J., Clare Wood, and Sheely Kieron. 2010. Does speech rhythm sensitivity predict children’s reading ability 1 year later? Journal of Educational Pychology 102: 356–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Law, Lily. 2012. Assessing and Understanding Individual Differences in Music Perception Abilities. Doctoral dissertation, Department of Psychology, The University of York, Heslington, UK. [Google Scholar]
- Thackray, R. 1969. An Investigation into Rhythmic Abilities. Music Education Research Papers No. 4. London: Novello & Co Ltd. [Google Scholar]
Tests and Subtests | Number of Items | Cronbach’s α | Min–Max | Mean | SD |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Musical abilities combined | 75 | 0.89 | 0–100 | 42.63 | 14.11 |
Musical perception | 42 | 0.70 | 0–100 | 57.96 | 12.23 |
Musical reproduction | 33 | 0.93 | 0–100 | 26.53 | 21.63 |
Pitch-related abilities | 49 | 0.88 | 0–100 | 37.62 | 15.18 |
Rhythm-related abilities | 19 | 0.71 | 0–100 | 55.92 | 20.33 |
Raven IQ | 35 | 0.89 | 0–100 | 78.03 | 16.89 |
Word reading | 67 | 0.81 | 0–100 | 75.15 | 11.31 |
Mathematics | 77 | 0.92 | 0–100 | 65.82 | 14.09 |
GPA | 12 | 0.92 | 1–5 | 3.98 | 0.71 |
Variables | 1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 | 6 | 7 | 8 | 9 | 10 | 11 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1. Musical perception | – | ||||||||||
2. Musical reproduction | 0.37 | – | |||||||||
3. Pitch-related abilities | 0.71 | 0.86 | – | ||||||||
4. Rhythm-related abilities | 0.39 | 0.69 | 0.49 | – | |||||||
5. Musical abilities combined | 0.72 | 0.91 | 0.96 | 0.69 | – | ||||||
6. Arithmetic skills | 0.23 | 0.19 | 0.19 | 0.31 | 0.25 | – | |||||
7. Word reading | 0.23 | 0.18 | 0.16 | 0.34 | 0.23 | 0.37 | – | ||||
8. Raven IQ | 0.27 | 0.16 | 0.26 | 0.19 | 0.24 | 0.46 | 0.25 | – | |||
9. Mothers’ education | 0.31 | 0.19 | 0.31 | 0.17 | 0.28 | 0.18 | 0.18 | 0.48 | – | ||
10. Instrumental training | 0.38 | 0.42 | 0.48 | 0.33 | 0.49 | 0.11 | 0.15 | 0.16 | 0.20 | – | |
11. GPA | 0.30 | 0.32 | 0.29 | 0.43 | 0.38 | 0.47 | 0.46 | 0.47 | 0.41 | 0.19 | – |
Regression Models | R2 | β | R2 Change | F | p |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Step1 | |||||
Mothers’ education | 0.17 | 0.41 | 0.17 | 14.19 | <0.001 |
Step 2 | |||||
Mothers’ education | 0.25 | ||||
Raven IQ | 0.27 | 0.35 | 0.10 | 12.28 | <0.001 |
Step 3A | |||||
Mothers’ education | 0.19 | ||||
Raven IQ | 0.33 | ||||
Musical abilities combined | 0.33 | 0.26 | 0.06 | 10.80 | <0.001 |
Step 3B | |||||
Mothers’ education | 0.19 | ||||
Raven IQ | 0.33 | ||||
Musical perception | 0.09 | ||||
Musical reproduction | 0.33 | 0.21 | 0.06 | 8.00 | <0.001 |
Step 3C | |||||
Mothers’ education | 0.23 | ||||
Raven IQ | 0.31 | ||||
Rhythm-related abilities | 0.37 | ||||
Pitch-related abilities | 0.39 | −0.05 | 0.12 | 10.37 | <0.001 |
Regression Models | R2 | β | R2 Change | F | p |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Step 1 | |||||
Mothers’ education | 0.24 | ||||
Raven IQ | 0.16 | ||||
Word reading | 0.26 | ||||
Mathematical skills | 0.41 | 0.25 | 0.41 | 11.47 | <0.001 |
Step 2A | |||||
Mothers’ education | 0.20 | ||||
Raven IQ | 0.17 | ||||
Word reading | 0.22 | ||||
Mathematical skills | 0.23 | ||||
Musical abilities combined | 0.43 | 0.16 | 0.02 | 9.88 | <0.001 |
Step 2B | |||||
Mothers’ education | 0.21 | ||||
Raven IQ | 0.17 | ||||
Word reading | 0.22 | ||||
Mathematical skills | 0.23 | ||||
Musical perception | 0.04 | ||||
Musical reproduction | 0.43 | 0.15 | 0.00 | 8.15 | <0.001 |
Step 2C | |||||
Mothers’ education | 0.23 | ||||
Raven IQ | 0.17 | ||||
Word reading | 0.18 | ||||
Mathematical skills | 0.22 | ||||
Rhythm-related abilities | 0.26 | ||||
Pitch-related abilities | 46 | −0.04 | 0.03 | 9.01 | <0.001 |
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. |
© 2022 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Janurik, M.; Józsa, K. Long-Term Impacts of Early Musical Abilities on Academic Achievement: A Longitudinal Study. J. Intell. 2022, 10, 36. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence10030036
Janurik M, Józsa K. Long-Term Impacts of Early Musical Abilities on Academic Achievement: A Longitudinal Study. Journal of Intelligence. 2022; 10(3):36. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence10030036
Chicago/Turabian StyleJanurik, Márta, and Krisztián Józsa. 2022. "Long-Term Impacts of Early Musical Abilities on Academic Achievement: A Longitudinal Study" Journal of Intelligence 10, no. 3: 36. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence10030036
APA StyleJanurik, M., & Józsa, K. (2022). Long-Term Impacts of Early Musical Abilities on Academic Achievement: A Longitudinal Study. Journal of Intelligence, 10(3), 36. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence10030036