Next Article in Journal
The Influence of Culture Capital, Social Security, and Living Conditions on Children’s Cognitive Ability: Evidence from 2018 China Family Panel Studies
Previous Article in Journal
Utilizing the Metaverse for Learner-Centered Constructivist Education in the Post-Pandemic Era: An Analysis of Elementary School Students
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Essay

Post-Operative Cognitive Impairment: A Cognitive Epidemiology Perspective

Max-Delbrück-Center for Molecular Medicine in the Helmholtz Association (MDC), Molecular Epidemiology Research Group, D-13125 Berlin, Germany
J. Intell. 2022, 10(1), 18; https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence10010018
Submission received: 29 December 2021 / Revised: 1 March 2022 / Accepted: 8 March 2022 / Published: 11 March 2022

Abstract

:
Cognitive epidemiology investigates cognitive predictors of health and disease outcomes. Post-operative cognitive impairment is a common complication of surgery but has been neglected as a health outcome in cognitive epidemiology research. This is despite the fact that knowledge of cognitive predictors of post-operative cognitive impairment can be utilized for risk stratification, informed decision-making (in elective surgery), and personalized care of patients during the postoperative period. In this narrative review, the current literature on cognitive predictors of post-operative cognitive impairment and gaps therein are summarized.

1. Cognitive Epidemiology

Cognitive epidemiology is a growing research field that addresses individual differences in intelligence at its peak level in early adulthood, i.e., prior to any ageing- or disease-associated declines, (also termed ‘pre-morbid IQ’) as a candidate risk factor for health and disease outcomes (Deary and Batty 2007; Deary 2012). Luckily to epidemiologists, short of actual measurement (like in birth cohorts), pre-morbid IQ can be estimated ‘post-hoc’ at any point across the lifespan by performance on vocabulary tests (Bright and van der Linde 2020). This is because vocabulary is a ‘crystallized’ ability (Cattell 1963) and as such immune to declines (Crawford et al. 2001). Self-report proxies of pre-morbid IQ such as socioeconomic status (SES), household income, education, and occupation (e.g., Cheng and Furnham 2019), which together with pre-morbid IQ can be referred to as ‘cognitive reserve’ parameters (Stern et al. 2020), are also frequently used. Numerous studies have demonstrated links of a lower achievement on these measures with an increased risk of later negative health outcomes including premature mortality (Batty et al. 2007; Calvin et al. 2011; Melchior et al. 2006), cancer (Batty et al. 2009), accelerated cognitive ageing (Caamaño-Isorna et al. 2006; Huang et al. 2020b), and COVID-19 infection (Batty et al. 2020).

2. Cognitive Epidemiology in a Surgical Setting

Surgery, particularly in older age, comes with a risk of complications, and some of them are of a cognitive nature. Post-operative delirium (POD) is characterized by agitation or apathy and affects around 19–28% of patients during the days after surgery (Ho et al. 2021); post-operative cognitive dysfunction (POCD) describes a decline in cognitive test performance across surgery that can last weeks to years. At 3 to 6 months, POCD can be detected in around 10–25% of patients (Borchers et al. 2021). This article aims to provide a narrative review of the currently limited evidence on the cognitive epidemiology of post-operative cognitive impairment. To this end, associations of cognitive reserve parameters with POD and POCD risk from studies of any design will be summarized, differences that emerge for these two outcomes will be drawn out, and knowledge gaps will be highlighted for future systematic examination in view to stimulate further research.
Of note, any epidemiological research in a surgical setting is highly complicated as compared with population-based settings. In the latter, one can take advantage of large cohorts that track participants over time and monitor them for occurrence of disease outcomes. Participants are recruited after detailed pre-planning, at a time when they are (presumably) of good general health and are happy to donate their time for research upon recruitment and (often) several times during the years or decades thereafter. In surgical settings, in contrast, patients are approached at a time of psychological distress and poor health and are followed up for a limited time period only, usually up to hospital discharge or a few months after surgery. Drop-out is common and sample sizes are often small. A focus is mainly on hard facts, such as blood-based biomarkers or medication plans, rather than more or less easily quantifiable (and in terms of immediate survival certainly less relevant) parameters such as pre-morbid IQ or education. It is, therefore, difficult to apply cognitive epidemiology research methods developed in population-based settings to the analysis of post-operative complications, such as POD or POCD. The studies reviewed here reflect this difficulty. They also vary considerably in terms of study design, follow-up period, surgery/anesthesia type, and patient characteristics, many of which can be risk factors in their own right, but will not be at the focus of this work. In addition, studies differ in their definitions of POCD, which can be based on various statistical techniques applied to scores from repeat neuropsychological testing or brief screening instruments. For POD, in contrast, comparability between studies is aided by the use of standardized instruments such as Confusion Assessment Method (CAM) or an ICU-specific version (CAM-ICU). A majority of studies reviewed here also had not set out to target the cognitive epidemiology of POD or POCD but reported patients’ cognitive reserve parameters (most frequently education) in ‘baseline characteristics’ tables comparing ‘POD’ with ‘no POD’ or ‘POCD’ with ‘no POCD’ groups. This lack of statistical adjustment is not necessarily a problem in this context; however: other than genetic or early-life exposures, no confounding factor could be expected to lead to both a low pre-morbid IQ and POCD for instance, giving rise to a spurious statistical association in absence of a true link between the two. In this respect, cognitive epidemiology takes on a special kind of role in the investigation of health and disease: cognitive reserve exposures can be expected to stand at (or near) the outset of a causal chain leading up to a disease outcome in older age.

2.1. The Cognitive Epidemiology of POD

2.1.1. Education and POD

The evidence on education and POD risk appears mixed. In the Successful Aging after Elective Surgery (SAGES) study of 566 over 70-year-old patients undergoing major non-cardiac surgery in the US, POD defined by a combination of CAM and chart review occurred in 24% of patients. Self-reported education was not associated with POD risk (Cavallari et al. 2016, 2017; Cizginer et al. 2017; Hshiesh et al. 2017; Inouye et al. 2016; Tripp et al. 2021; Vasunilashorn et al. 2017). Another large study, the PAWEL study based in Germany (Deeken et al. 2022), as well as smaller cohort studies from Europe, Asia, and South America support these null results (Jiang et al. 2017; Kazmierski et al. 2013; Mahanna-Gabrielli et al. 2020; Puustinen et al. 2016; Ristescu et al. 2021; Sauer et al. 2017; Seo et al. 2014; Xue et al. 2016), whereas other investigations do suggest a low education as a risk factor for POD (Dworkin et al. 2016; Gu et al. 2021; Guan et al. 2021; Huang et al. 2021; Ma et al. 2021; McAlpine et al. 2008; Ordóñez-Velasco and Hernández-Leiva 2021; Schoen et al. 2011; Sprung et al. 2017; Su et al. 2019; Tenpaku et al. 2021; Tobar et al. 2018). Interestingly, one recent Chinese study of 323 laryngectomy patients reported evidence in the opposite direction. Here, 85.7% of patients who developed POD (defined according to CAM) had a high school degree, whereas only 61.4% of patients without POD had a high school degree; the remaining patients had attained less than high school. The difference was statistically significant and suggested a protective association of a low education with a reduced POD risk (Wang et al. 2019).
Systematic reviews mirror this incoherent evidence. One that focused on cardiac surgery settings concluded that a low education was not a risk factor for POD (Gosselt et al. 2015); another describes a low education as a risk factor for POD (Bilotta et al. 2013) but cites two studies of which one finds no such association (DeCrane et al. 2011) and the other did not assess education (Gusmao-Flores et al. 2012). A number of other systematic reviews on risk factors for POD neglected education altogether (Aitken et al. 2017; Oh et al. 2015; Raats et al. 2016), which could be an oversight or may result from education infrequently being associated with POD. Clearly, the evidence on education and POD risk warrants systematic review and meta-analysis for quantification of any potential associations.

2.1.2. Occupation and POD

In the SAGES study, the only study reporting on occupation and POD, complexity and managerial demands in patients’ self-reported longest held occupation were not associated with POD risk, both across the total study sample and when analyzed for females and males separately (Saczynski et al. 2014).

2.1.3. Estimated Pre-Morbid IQ and POD

Pre-morbid IQ apparently has been assessed in only two studies in the context of POD. In a study of 37 older cardiac surgery patients in the UK, a trend short of statistical significance was observed for a lower National Adult Reading Test (NART) in patients who developed POD compared with those who did not (Brown et al. 2011). In SAGES, patients scoring higher on an estimate of pre-morbid IQ (Wechsler Test of Adult Reading, WTAR) were at a reduced POD risk in univariate (Cizginer et al. 2017) and multivariate-adjusted analysis (Saczynski et al. 2014), and with a substantial effect size: each 0.5 standard deviation higher WTAR score was associated with a 38% reduced POD risk when controlling for age, sex, ethnicity, and English as a second language. The association appeared to be driven by females in particular (Saczynski et al. 2014). An analysis of a subsample of the first 300 patients enrolled into SAGES had already indicated a trend in this direction (Fong et al. 2015).

2.1.4. Composites and POD

One way to operationalize exposures for cognitive epidemiology research is to calculate composite (e.g., latent) variables of reserve, which can avoid parameter-specific measurement error (Deary and Johnson 2010). In SAGES, a latent composite was calculated from estimated pre-morbid IQ, education, and self-reported lifetime engagement in cognitive activities. The resulting composite (thus far) appears to have been assessed as a moderator of the relationship of post-operative C-reactive protein concentrations with POD risk only (Cizginer et al. 2017) rather than as a potential predictor of POD in its own right.

2.2. The Cognitive Epidemiology of POCD

2.2.1. Education and POCD

Compared with POD, the evidence on education and POCD is more well-described and consistent. In a 2017 meta-analysis of 15 studies from Europe, USA, Australia, and Asia, with follow-up periods of one day to six months after surgery (Feinkohl et al. 2017), education was strongly inversely associated with POCD risk: each one additional year of education was associated with a 10% reduced POCD risk. Meta-regression indicated that the finding was independent of follow-up period, sample age, % males, and surgery type, suggesting that the association of education with reduced POCD risk may be universal. Similar observations were made for education as a categorical variable: patients with more than high school education were overall at 29% reduced POCD risk compared with patients who only attained a high school degree (Feinkohl et al. 2017). To name one landmark study included in the meta-analysis, the International Study of Post-Operative Cognitive Dysfunction (ISPOCD 1), found that among 1011 older surgical patients recruited in eight European countries and the USA, patients with more than a high school education had even a halved risk of POCD at one week after surgery compared with patients with less than high school education (Moller et al. 1998). Studies from various European and Asian countries and the USA that were not included in the 2017 meta-analysis (Grichnik et al. 1999) or published thereafter (Bendikaite and Vimantaite 2020; Ehsani et al. 2020; Huang et al. 2020a; Klinger et al. 2019; Li et al. 2020; Wang et al. 2018; Yuhe et al. 2020; Zhang et al. 2019; Zhou et al. 2020) too, consistently indicate that patients with a lower educational level are at increased POCD risk at discharge (Zhang et al. 2019) within days of surgery (Ehsani et al. 2020; Li et al. 2020; Zhao et al. 2020; Zhou et al. 2020) but also during six to 12 weeks after surgery (Grichnik et al. 1999; Yuhe et al. 2020). The fact that observations extend to several months after surgery is important because studies on POCD with longer follow-up periods offer preferred scope for conclusions: during shorter follow-up periods immediate effects of surgery on brain function including a potential influence of POD can prevail. Null results on education and POCD appear relatively rare and coincide with a low statistical power (Claes et al. 2018; Li et al. 2019; Ni et al. 2015; Relander et al. 2020).

2.2.2. Occupation and POCD

A single study has compared occupational background between patients with and without POCD. In a cohort of middle-aged to older CABG patients in Finland, no association of occupation (categorized as ‘manual routine’, ‘qualified manual’, ‘nonmanual’) and POCD risk was observed (Relander et al. 2020). However, the study of 99 patients (13% with POCD) was again limited by a low statistical power.

2.2.3. Estimated Pre-Morbid IQ and POCD

Only two studies, both set in Australia, appear to have investigated estimated pre-morbid IQ as a stand-alone exposure. Both found no association with POCD risk. One of older atrial fibrillation ablation patients used the NART and found no association with POCD at two days or at three months after surgery (Medi et al. 2013). Another study of 53 knee surgery patients aged ≥50 years used a revision of the WTAR (Test of Premorbid Functioning, Pearson 2003) as an estimate of pre-morbid IQ and found no association with POCD at six months (Scott et al. 2017).

2.2.4. Composites and POCD

In a unique study of 42 middle-aged to older CABG patients, Ropacki et al. (Ropacki et al. 2007) calculated a composite from estimated pre-morbid IQ, education, occupation, as well as ethnicity, sex, and region of country in the US. The reasoning behind inclusion of the latter three parameters was not well substantiated and may have led to the observation that the composite was not associated with risk of POCD at hospital discharge. In fact, a statistically non-significant trend of a lower POCD risk in the ‘low cognitive reserve’ group (55.5%) compared with the ‘high cognitive reserve’ group (77.3%) was observed in this study (Ropacki et al. 2007).

2.3. Summary

A low education can be considered a firm risk factor for POCD. Although we can only speculate as to underlying mechanisms, lifestyle-associated parameters such as lifetime exposure to smoking and metabolic dysfunction may be plausible mediators enhancing the vulnerability of the brain to surgery effects. For POD, the evidence for associations with education appears more mixed and warrants systematic assessment including meta-analysis and meta-regression to determine whether or not any between-study differences in design, instruments, or patient characteristics may account for differences in results. As it currently stands, it appears that the link of education with POD may be weaker as compared with its association with POCD, or it may be absent. This pattern adds to mounting evidence indicating that POD and POCD, despite both having symptoms related to brain dysfunction, are in fact distinct conditions (Daiello et al. 2019) with a distinct epidemiology. The evidence on occupation and pre-morbid IQ in POD/POCD risk is extremely limited at present and further studies are needed to evaluate their roles.

3. Conclusions and Outlook

Based on the evidence presented here, cognitive reserve parameters may warrant consideration in any epidemiological studies on lifespan risk factors for post-operative cognitive impairment. They could act as potential confounders: patients with low education may be at increased risk of developing chronic diseases for instance (Wraw et al. 2015) as well as POCD (Feinkohl et al. 2017), which could lead to a spurious statistical association between chronic disease and POCD. To aid epidemiological research into POD and POCD, we thus clearly need to strengthen our research efforts to gain a fuller understanding of their cognitive epidemiology.
Although cognitive screening instruments such as the Mini Mental State Examination (MMSE) are not ideal for definition of cognitive impairment, in busy clinics with limited time resources, they are certainly preferable to having no cognitive assessment at all (Deary 2021) and could be a way to boost POCD research in particular. Of note, pre-morbid IQ estimated by vocabulary remains stable across surgery (Brown et al. 2011). Thus, it is conceivable that future studies could measure this (and other) cognitive reserve markers after surgery, at a time when detailed assessments may be felt to be less burdensome to patients, and still produce valid results.
Ultimately, once we have a better understanding of the cognitive epidemiology of post-operative cognitive impairment, we can apply this knowledge for risk stratification and personalized medicine. We will be able to empower patients for informed decision-making in elective surgery settings and can additionally monitor at-risk individuals for cognitive deficits during the post-operative period. With cognitive reserve (to some extent) modifiable, pre-operative intervention programs could be set up and tested. We, thus, need to recruit large cohorts of surgical patients and test not only for POD and POCD, but additionally (before or after surgery) collect data on a diverse set of cognitive reserve markers. Some rare studies in this direction, such as SAGES, have already shown us how.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

Ethical review and approval were waived for this narrative review article as no data from human participants were collected.

Informed Consent Statement

Not applicable.

Data Availability Statement

Not applicable.

Conflicts of Interest

The author declares no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Aitken, Sarah Joy, Fiona M. Blyth, and Vasi Naganathan. 2017. Incidence, prognostic factors and impact of postoperative delirium after major vascular surgery: A meta-analysis and systematic review. Vascular Medicine 22: 387–97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  2. Batty, G. David, Ian J. Deary, and Linda S. Gottfredson. 2007. Premorbid (early life) IQ and later mortality risk: Systematic review. Annals of Epidemiology 17: 278–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  3. Batty, G. David, Ian J. Deary, Michelle Luciano, Drew M. Altschul, Mika Kivimäki, and Catherine R. Gale. 2020. Psychosocial factors and hospitalisations for COVID-19: Prospective cohort study based on a community sample. Brain Behavior and Immunity 89: 569–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Batty, G. David, Laust H. Mortensen, Catherine R. Gale, Martin J. Shipley, Beverly A. Roberts, and Ian J. Deary. 2009. IQ in late adolescence/early adulthood, risk factors in middle age, and later cancer mortality in men: The Vietnam Experience Study. Psychooncology 18: 1122–6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Bendikaite, Ruta, and Renata Vimantaite. 2020. Cognitive Impairment Prevalence and Impact on Quality of Life for Patients after Cardiac Surgery. The Heart Surgery Forum 23: E590–E594. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Bilotta, Federico, M. P. Lauretta, A. Borozdina, Viktor M. Mizikov, and Giovanni Rosa. 2013. Postoperative delirium: Risk factors, diagnosis and perioperative care. Minerva Anestesiologica 79: 1066–76. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  7. Borchers, Friedrich, Claudia Spies, Insa Feinkohl, Wolf-Rüdiger Brockhaus, Antje Kraft, Petra Kozma, Marinus Fislage, Simone Kühn, Catinca Ionescu, Saya Speidel, and et al. 2021. Methodology of measuring postoperative cognitive dysfunction: A systematic review. British Journal of Anaesthesia 126: 1119–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Bright, Peter, and Ian van der Linde. 2020. Comparison of methods for estimating premorbid intelligence. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation 30: 1–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  9. Brown, Laura J. E., Harriet S. Ferner, Jennie Robertson, Nicholas L. Mills, Renzo Pessotto, Ian J. Deary, and Alasdair M. J. MacLullich. 2011. Differential effects of delirium on fluid and crystallized cognitive abilities. Archives of Gerontology and Geriatrics 52: 153–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  10. Caamaño-Isorna, Francisco, Montserrat Corral, Augustin Montes-Martínez, and Bahi Takkouche. 2006. Education and dementia: A meta-analytic study. Neuroepidemiology 26: 226–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Calvin, Catherine M., Ian J. Deary, Candida Fenton, Beverly A. Roberts, Geoff Der, Nicola Leckenby, and G. David Batty. 2011. Intelligence in youth and all-cause-mortality: Systematic review with meta-analysis. International Journal of Epidemiology 40: 626–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. Cattell, Raymond B. 1963. Theory of fluid and crystallized intelligence. Journal of Educational Psychology 54: 1–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Cavallari, Michele, Weiying Dai, Charles R. G. Guttmann, Dominik S. Meier, Long H. Ngo, Tammy T. Hshieh, Amy E. Callahan, Tamara G. Fong, Eva Schmitt, Bradford C. Dickerson, and et al. 2016. Neural substrates of vulnerability to postsurgical delirium as revealed by presurgical diffusion MRI. Brain 139: 1282–94. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  14. Cavallari, Michele, Weiying Dai, Charles R. G. Guttmann, Dominik S. Meier, Long H. Ngo, Tammy T. Hshieh, Tamara G. Fong, Eva Schmitt, Daniel Z. Press, Thomas G. Travison, and et al. 2017. Longitudinal diffusion changes following postoperative delirium in older people without dementia. Neurology 89: 1020–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Cheng, Helen, and Adrian Furnham. 2019. Correlates of Adult Vocabulary Task Performance: Findings from a British Cohort. Journal of Intelligence 7: 2. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  16. Cizginer, Sevdenur, Edward Marcantonio, Sarinnapha Vasunilashorn, Alvaro Pascual-Leone, Mouhsin Shafi, E. M. Schmitt, S. K. Inouye, and R. N. Jones. 2017. The Cognitive Reserve Model in the Development of Delirium: The Successful Aging After Elective Surgery Study. Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry and Neurology 30: 337–45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  17. Claes, Annes J., Suzanne de Backer, Paul Van de Heyning, Annick Gilles, Vincent Van Rompaey, and Griet Mertens. 2018. Postoperative cognitive dysfunction after cochlear implantation. European Archives of Otorhinolaryngology 275: 1419–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  18. Crawford, John R., Ian J. Deary, John Starr, and Lawrence J. Whalley. 2001. The NART as an index of prior intellectual functioning: A retrospective validity study covering a 66-year interval. Psychological Medicine 31: 451–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  19. Daiello, Lori A., Annie M. Racine, Ray Yun Gou, Edward R. Marcantonio, Zhongcong Xie, Lisa J. Kunze, Kamen V. Vlassakov, Sharon K. Inouye, Richard N. Jones, David Alsop, and et al. 2019. Postoperative Delirium and Postoperative Cognitive Dysfunction: Overlap and Divergence. Anesthesiology 131: 477–91. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  20. Deary, Ian J. 2012. Looking for ‘system integrity’ in cognitive epidemiology. Gerontology 58: 545–53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Deary, Ian J. 2021. Two Cheers for the Cognitive Irregulars: Intelligence’s Contributions to Ageing Well and Staying Alive. Journal of Intelligence 9: 41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  22. Deary, Ian J., and G. David Batty. 2007. Cognitive epidemiology. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health 61: 378–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  23. Deary, Ian J., and Wendy Johnson. 2010. Intelligence and education: Causal perceptions drive analytic processes and therefore conclusions. International Journal of Epidemiology 39: 1362–69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. DeCrane, Susan K., Laura Sands, Meghan Ashland, Eunjung Lim, Tiffany L. Tsai, Sudeshna Paul, and Jacqueline M. Leung. 2011. Factors associated with recovery from early postoperative delirium. Journal of PeriAnesthesia Nursing 26: 231–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  25. Deeken, Friedrike, Alba Sánchez, Michael A. Rapp, Michael Denkinger, Simone Brefka, Juliane Spank, Carola Bruns, Christine A. F. von Arnim, Olivia C. Küster, Lars O. Conzelmann, and et al. 2022. Outcomes of a Delirium Prevention Program in Older Persons After Elective Surgery: A Stepped-Wedge Cluster Randomized Clinical Trial. JAMA Surgery 157: e216370. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  26. Dworkin, Andy, David S. Lee, Amber R. An, and Sarah J. Goodlin. 2016. A Simple Tool to Predict Development of Delirium After Elective Surgery. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society 64: e149–e153. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  27. Ehsani, Roghayeh, Soudabeh Djalali Motlagh, Behrooz Zaman, Saloumeh Sehat Kashani, and Mohammed R. Ghodraty. 2020. Effect of General Versus Spinal Anesthesia on Postoperative Delirium and Early Cognitive Dysfunction in Elderly Patients. Anesthesiology and Pain Medicine 10: e101815. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Feinkohl, Insa, Georg Winterer, Claudia Spies, and Tobias Pischon. 2017. Cognitive Reserve and the Risk of Postoperative Cognitive Dysfunction. Deutsches Aerzteblatt International 114: 110–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  29. Fong, Tamara G., Tammy T. Hshieh, Bonnie Wong, Doug Tommet, Richard. N. Jones, Eva M. Schmitt, Margaret R. Puelle, Jane S. Saczynski, Edward R. Marcantonio, and Sharon K. Inouye. 2015. Neuropsychological profiles of an elderly cohort undergoing elective surgery and the relationship between cognitive performance and delirium. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society 63: 977–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  30. Gosselt, Alex N., Arjen J. Slooter, Pascal R. Boere, and Irene J. Zaal. 2015. Risk factors for delirium after on-pump cardiac surgery: A systematic review. Critical Care 19: 346. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  31. Grichnik, Katherine P., Alexander J. Ijsselmuiden, Thomas A. D’Amico, David H. Harpole Jr., William D. White, James A. Blumenthal, and Mark F. Newman. 1999. Cognitive decline after major noncardiac operations: A preliminary prospective study. Annals of Thoracic Surgery 68: 1786–91. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Gu, Wan-Jie, Jian-Xing Zhou, Run-Qing Ji, Lu-Yang Zhou, and Chun-Mei Wang. 2021. Incidence, risk factors, and consequences of emergence delirium after elective brain tumor resection. The Surgeon. (online ahead of print). [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  33. Guan, Hui-Lian, He Liu, Xiao-Yi Hu, Mannan Abdul, Ming-Sheng Dai, Xing Gao, Xue-Fen Chen, Yang Zhou, Xun Sun, Jian Zhou, and et al. 2021. Urinary albumin creatinine ratio associated with postoperative delirium in elderly patients undergoing elective non-cardiac surgery: A prospective observational study. CNS Neuroscience & Therapeutics. (online ahead of print). [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Gusmao-Flores, Dimitri, Jorge I. Salluh, Ricardo Á. Chalhub, and Lucas C. Quarantini. 2012. The confusion assessment method for the intensive care unit (CAM-ICU) and intensive care delirium screening checklist (ICDSC) for the diagnosis of delirium: A systematic review and meta-analysis of clinical studies. Critical Care 16: R115. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  35. Ho, Mu-Hsing, Jessica Nealon, Ezinne Igwe, Victoria Traynor, Hui-Chen R. Chang, Kee-Hsin Chen, and Jed Montayre. 2021. Postoperative Delirium in Older Patients: A Systematic Review of Assessment and Incidence of Postoperative Delirium. Worldviews on Evidence-Based Nursing 18: 290–301. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Hshiesh, Tammy T., Jane Saczynski, Ray Yun Gou, Edward R. Marcantonio, Richard N. Jones, Eva Schmitt, Zara Cooper, Douglas Ayres, John Wright, and Sharon K. Inouye. 2017. Trajectory of functional recovery after postoperative delirium in elective surgery. Annals of Surgery 265: 647–53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  37. Huang, Hua-Wei, Guo-Bin Zhang, Hao-Yi Li, Chun-Mei Wang, Yu-Mei Wang, Xiu-Mei Sun, Jing-Ran Chen, Guan-Qiang Chen, Ming Xu, and Jian-Xin Zhou. 2021. Development of an early prediction model for postoperative delirium in neurosurgical patients admitted to the ICU after elective craniotomy (E-PREPOD-NS): A secondary analysis of a prospective cohort study. Journal of Clinical Neuroscience 90: 217–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Huang, Huimei, Fei Lin, Liming Cen, Ren Jing, and Linghui Pan. 2020a. Cancer-Related Anemia Is a Risk Factor for Medium-Term Postoperative Cognitive Dysfunction in Laparoscopic Surgery Patients: An Observational Prospective Study. Neural Plasticity 2020: 4847520. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Huang, Liang-Yu, He-Ying Hu, Zuo-Teng Wang, Ya-Hui Ma, Qiang Dong, Lan Tan, and Jin-Tai Yu. 2020b. Association of Occupational Factors and Dementia or Cognitive Impairment: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Journal of Alzheimers Disease 78: 217–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Inouye, Sharon K., Edward R. Marcantonio, Cyrus M. Kosar, Douglas Tommet, Eva M. Schmitt, Thomas G. Travison, Jane S. Saczynski, Long H. Ngo, David C. Alsop, and Richard N. Jones. 2016. The short-term and long-term relationship between delirium and cognitive trajectory in older surgical patients. Alzheimers & Dementia 12: 766–75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  41. Jiang, Xin, Dong Chen, Yahao Lou, and Zhongshi Li. 2017. Risk factors for postoperative delirium after spine surgery in middle- and old-aged patients. Aging Clinical and Experimental Research 29: 1039–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Kazmierski, Jakub, Andrzej Banys, Joanna Latek, Julius Bourke, and Ryszard Jaszewski. 2013. Cortisol levels and neuropsychiatric diagnosis as markers of postoperative delirium: A prospective cohort study. Critical Care 17: R38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  43. Klinger, Rebecca Y., Mary Cooter, Tiffany Bisanar, Niccolo Terrando, Miles Berger, Mihai V. Podgoreanu, Mark Stafford-Smith, Mark F. Newman, and Joseph. P. Mathew. 2019. Intravenous Lidocaine Does Not Improve Neurologic Outcomes after Cardiac Surgery: A Randomized Controlled Trial. Anesthesiology 130: 958–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  44. Li, Hui, Ting-Ting Wu, Lin Tang, Qing Liu, Xin-Zhao Mao, Jun-Mei Xu, and Ru-Pring Dai. 2020. Association of global DNA hypomethylation with post-operative cognitive dysfunction in elderly patients undergoing hip surgery. Acta Anaesthesiologica Scandinavica 64: 354–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Li, Yue, Dan Huang, Diansan Su, Jie Chen, and Liqun Yang. 2019. Postoperative cognitive dysfunction after robot-assisted radical cystectomy (RARC) with cerebral oxygen monitoring: An observational prospective cohort pilot study. BMC Anesthesiology 19: 202. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Ma, Jun, Chuanyao Li, Wei Zhang, Ling Zhou, Shuhua Shu, Sheng Wang, Di Wang, and Xiaoqing Chai. 2021. Preoperative anxiety predicted the incidence of postoperative delirium in patients undergoing total hip arthroplasty: A prospective cohort study. BMC Anesthesiology 21: 48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Mahanna-Gabrielli, Elizabeth, Kathy Zhang, Frederick E. Sieber, Hung Mo Lin, Xiaoyu Liu, Margaret Sewell, Stacie G. Deiner, and Kenneth S. Boockvar. 2020. Frailty Is Associated with Postoperative Delirium But Not with Postoperative Cognitive Decline in Older Noncardiac Surgery Patients. Anesthesia and Analgesia 130: 1516–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. McAlpine, Jessica N., Eric J. Hodgson, Steven Abramowitz, Sandra M. Richman, Yidan Su, Michael G. Kelly, Michael Luther, Laura Baker, Daniel Zelterman, Thomas J. Rutherford, and et al. 2008. The incidence and risk factors associated with postoperative delirium in geriatric patients undergoing surgery for suspected gynecologic malignancies. Gynecologic Oncology 109: 296–302. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Medi, Caroline, Lisbeth Evered, Brendan Silbert, Andrew Teh, Karen Halloran, Joseph Morton, Peter Kistler, and Jonathan Kalman. 2013. Subtle post-procedural cognitive dysfunction after atrial fibrillation ablation. Journal of the American College of Cardiology 62: 531–39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  50. Melchior, Maria, Lisa F. Berkman, Ichiro Kawachi, Nancy Krieger, Marie Zins, Sebastien Bonenfant, and Marcel Goldberg. 2006. Lifelong socioeconomic trajectory and premature mortality (35–65 years) in France: Findings from the GAZEL Cohort Study. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health 60: 937–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Moller, Jakob T., Pierre Cluitmans, Lars S. Rasmussen, Peter Houx, Jaume Canet, Patrick Rabbitt, Jelle Jolles, Katrine Larsen, Chris D. Hanning, Olivier Langeron, and et al. 1998. Long-term postoperative cognitive dysfunction in the elderly: ISPOCD1 study. Lancet 351: 857–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Ni, Cheng, Ting Xu, Yang Tian, Yongzheng Han, Qingsheng Xue, Min Li, and Xiangyang Guo. 2015. Cerebral oxygen saturation after multiple perioperative influential factors predicts the occurrence of postoperative cognitive dysfunction. BMC Anesthesiology 15: 156. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  53. Oh, Esther S., Meng Li, Tolulope M. Fafowora, Sharon K. Inouye, Cathy H. Chen, Lori M. Rosman, Constantine G. Lyketsos, Frederick E. Sieber, and Milo A. Puhan. 2015. Preoperative risk factors for postoperative delirium following hip fracture repair: A systematic review. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 30: 900–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  54. Ordóñez-Velasco, Lina M., and Edgar Hernández-Leiva. 2021. Factors associated with delirium after cardiac surgery: A prospective cohort study. Annals of Cardiac Anaesthesia 24: 183–89. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Puustinen, Jukka, Liisa Luostarinen, Markku Luostarinen, Veijo Pulliainen, Heini Huhtala, Marjo Soini, and Jaana Suhonen. 2016. The Use of MoCA and Other Cognitive Tests in Evaluation of Cognitive Impairment in Elderly Patients Undergoing Arthroplasty. Geriatric Orthopaedic Surgery & Rehabilitation 7: 183–87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Raats, Jelle W., Stijn L. Steunenberg, Dominique C. de Lange, and Lijckle van der Laan. 2016. Risk factors of post-operative delirium after elective vascular surgery in the elderly: A systematic review. International Journal of Surgery 35: 1–6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Relander, Kristiina, Marja Hietanen, Kirsi Rantanen, Juhani Rämö, Antti Vento, Kari-Pekka Saastamoinen, Risto O. Roine, and Lauri Soinne. 2020. Postoperative cognitive change after cardiac surgery predicts long-term cognitive outcome. Brain and Behavior 10: e01750. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Ristescu, Anca Irina, Georgiana Pintilie, Mihaela Moscalu, Daniel Rusu, and Ioana Grigoras. 2021. Preoperative Cognitive Impairment and the Prevalence of Postoperative Delirium in Elderly Cancer Patients-A Prospective Observational Study. Diagnostics 11: 275. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Ropacki, Susan A., Arthur B. Bert, Michael T. Ropacki, Brook L. Rogers, and Robert A. Stern. 2007. The influence of cognitive reserve on neuropsychological functioning following coronary artery bypass grafting (CABG). Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 22: 73–85. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  60. Saczynski, Jane S., Sharon K. Inoye, Cyrus M. Kosar, Doug Tommet, Edward R. Marcantonio, Tamara Fong, Tammy Hshieh, Sarinnapha Vasunilashorn, Eran D. Metzger, Eva Schmitt, and et al. 2014. Cognitive and brain reserve and the risk of postoperative delirium in older patients. Lancet Psychiatry 1: 437–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  61. Sauer, Anne-Mette C., Dieuwke S. Veldhuijzen, Thomas H. Ottens, Arjen J. C. Slooter, Cor J. Kalkman, and Diederik van Dijk. 2017. Association between delirium and cognitive change after cardiac surgery. British Journal of Anaesthesia 119: 308–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  62. Schoen, Julika, Joscha Meyerrose, Hauke Paarmann, Matthias Heringlake, Michael Hueppe, and Klaus-Ulrich Berger. 2011. Preoperative regional cerebral oxygen saturation is a predictor of postoperative delirium in on-pump cardiac surgery patients: A prospective observational trial. Critical Care 15: R218. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  63. Scott, Julia E., Jane L. Mathias, Anthony C. Kneebone, and Jeganath Krishnan. 2017. Postoperative cognitive dysfunction and its relationship to cognitive reserve in elderly total joint replacement patients. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology 39: 459–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Seo, Jin S., Seung W. Park, Young S. Lee, Chan Chung, and Young B. Kim. 2014. Risk factors for delirium after spine surgery in elderly patients. Journal of the Korean Neurosurgical Society 56: 28–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Sprung, Juraj, Rosebud O. Roberts, Toby N. Weingarten, Alexendre Nunes Cavalcante, David S. Knopman, Ronald C. Petersen, Andrew C. Hanson, Darrell R. Schroeder, and David O. Warner. 2017. Postoperative delirium in elderly patients is associated with subsequent cognitive impairment. British Journal of Anaesthesia 119: 316–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Stern, Yaakov, Eider Arenaza-Urquijo, David Bartres-Faz, Sylvie Belleville, Marc Cantilon, Gael Chetelat, Michael Ewers, Nicolai Franzmeier, Gerd Kempermann, William S. Kremen, and et al. 2020. Whitepaper: Defining and investigating cognitive reserve, brain reserve, and brain maintenance. Alzheimer’s & Dementia 16: 1305–11. [Google Scholar]
  67. Su, Lijing, Yilu Yan, Wenjuan Huang, Qin Xu, Jinhua Liao, Huimin Lin, Dandan Wu, Sailan Li, and Rongfang Hu. 2019. Risk factors for intensive care unit delirium after cardiac operation. Zhonghua Wei Zhong Bing Ji Jiu Yi Xue 31: 165–71. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Tenpaku, Yosuke, Masayuki Satoh, Kenji Kato, Kazuhisa Fujinaga, Yuji Haruki, Hiroki Nakahashi, Keisuke Morikawa, Yasunori Imaoka, Hiroyuki Takemura, and Hiroshi Tatsumi. 2021. Prediction of Postoperative Delirium after Gastrointestinal Surgery Using the Mie Constructional Apraxia Scale. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders Extra 11: 306–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Tobar, Eduardo, Mario A. Abedrapo, Jaime A. Godoy, Jose L. Llanos, Mauricio J. Díaz, Rodrigo Azolas, Gunther R. Bocic, Jaime A. Escobar, Rodrigo A. Cornejo, and Carlos M. Romero. 2018. Impact of hypotension and global hypoperfusion in postoperative delirium: A pilot study in older adults undergoing open colon surgery. Brazilian Journal of Anesthesiology 68: 135–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Tripp, Bridget A., Simon T. Dillon, Min Yuan, John M. Asara, Sarinnapha M. Vasunilashorn, Tamara G. Fong, Eran D. Metzger, Sharon K. Inouye, Zhongcong Xie, Long H. Ngo, and et al. 2021. Targeted metabolomics analysis of postoperative delirium. Scientific Reports 11: 1521. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Vasunilashorn, Sarinnapha M., Simon T. Dillon, Sharon K. Inouye, Long H. Ngo, Tamara G. Fong, Richard N. Jones, Thomas G. Travison, Eva M. Schmitt, David C. Alsop, Steven D. Freedman, and et al. 2017. High C-Reactive Protein Predicts Delirium Incidence, Duration, and Feature Severity After Major Noncardiac Surgery. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society 65: e109–e116. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  72. Wang, Runjia, Gongming Wang, Yang Liu, and Mengyuan Zhang. 2018. Preoperative smoking history is associated with decreased risk of early postoperative cognitive dysfunction in patients of advanced age after noncardiac surgery: A prospective observational cohort study. Journal of International Medical Research 47: 689–701. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  73. Wang, Yiru, Huiqian Yu, Hui Qiao, Chan Li, Kaizheng Chen, and Xia Shen. 2019. Risk Factors and Incidence of Postoperative Delirium in Patients Undergoing Laryngectomy. Otolaryngology Head and Neck Surgery 161: 807–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  74. Wraw, Christina, Ian J. Deary, Catherine R. Gale, and Geoff Der. 2015. Intelligence in youth and health at age 50. Intelligence 53: 23–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  75. Xue, Peng, Ziyu Wu, Kunpeng Wang, Chuanquan Tu, and Xiangbo Wang. 2016. Incidence and risk factors of postoperative delirium in elderly patients undergoing transurethral resection of prostate: A prospective cohort study. Neuropsychiatric Disease and Treatment 12: 137–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  76. Yuhe, Ke, Sophia T. H. Chew, An S. Ang, Roderica R. G. Ng, Nantawan Boonkiangwong, Weiling Liu, Anastasia H. H. Toh, Michael G. Caleb, Roger C. M. Ho, and Lian K. Ti. 2020. Comparison of postoperative cognitive decline in patients undergoing conventional vs miniaturized cardiopulmonary bypass: A randomized, controlled trial. Annals of Cardiac Anaesthesia 23: 309–14. [Google Scholar]
  77. Zhang, Xixue, Hui Li, Yating Lv, Zhenghong Zhu, Xiaoyong Shen, Qi Lu, Wei Wang, Zhaoxin Wang, Zhaoshun Jiang, Lvjun Yang, and et al. 2019. Premorbid Alterations of Spontaneous Brain Activity in Elderly Patients with Early Post-operative Cognitive Dysfunction: A Pilot Resting-State Functional MRI Study. Frontiers in Neurology 10: 1062. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  78. Zhao, Wenshuai, Yanan Hu, Hui Chen, Xifan Wang, Liping Wang, Yu Wang, Xiaohong Wu, and Fei Han. 2020. The Effect and Optimal Dosage of Dexmedetomidine Plus Sufentanil for Postoperative Analgesia in Elderly Patients with Postoperative Delirium and Early Postoperative Cognitive Dysfunction: A Single-Center, Prospective, Randomized, Double-Blind, Controlled Trial. Frontiers in Neuroscience 14: 549516. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  79. Zhou, Hongli, Fuyu Li, Wanlin Ye, Maozhou Wang, Xian Zhou, Jianguo Feng, Li Liu, and Xiaobin Wang. 2020. Correlation Between Plasma CircRNA-089763 and Postoperative Cognitive Dysfunction in Elderly Patients Undergoing Non-cardiac Surgery. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience 14: 587715. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Feinkohl, I. Post-Operative Cognitive Impairment: A Cognitive Epidemiology Perspective. J. Intell. 2022, 10, 18. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence10010018

AMA Style

Feinkohl I. Post-Operative Cognitive Impairment: A Cognitive Epidemiology Perspective. Journal of Intelligence. 2022; 10(1):18. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence10010018

Chicago/Turabian Style

Feinkohl, Insa. 2022. "Post-Operative Cognitive Impairment: A Cognitive Epidemiology Perspective" Journal of Intelligence 10, no. 1: 18. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence10010018

APA Style

Feinkohl, I. (2022). Post-Operative Cognitive Impairment: A Cognitive Epidemiology Perspective. Journal of Intelligence, 10(1), 18. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence10010018

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop