Next Article in Journal
How to Constitute a Field of Merit: Structure and Flexibility in a Tibetan Buddhist Monastery’s Curriculum
Previous Article in Journal
To Never See Death: Yeats, Reincarnation, and Resolving the Antinomies of the Body-Soul Dilemma
Previous Article in Special Issue
A Cognitive Approach to Tantric Language
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Editorial

Introduction to “Cognitive Science and the Study of Yoga and Tantra”

by
Glen Alexander Hayes
1,* and
Sthaneshwar Timalsina
2
1
Department of Religion, Bloomfield College, 467 Franklin St, Bloomfield, NJ 07003, USA
2
Religious Studies, College of Arts and Letters, San Diego State University 5500 Campanile Drive, San Diego, CA 92182, USA
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Religions 2017, 8(9), 181; https://doi.org/10.3390/rel8090181
Submission received: 28 August 2017 / Revised: 31 August 2017 / Accepted: 31 August 2017 / Published: 6 September 2017
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Cognitive Science and the Study of Yoga and Tantra)
The range of disciplines known as the Cognitive Science of Religions (CSR), which has emerged in recent decades, embraces many areas and specializations within the Academy, including cognitive science, linguistics, neuroscience, and religious studies. The results of this exploration, some of which are offered in this Special Issue before you, are intriguing, profound, and yet still preliminary. As will be noted below, this “preliminary” nature of CSR is due to the fact that our scientific knowledge of the neurological system—let alone of how the mind “works”—is still in mostly early phases. Techniques in imaging and scanning the brain, as well as in understanding patterns and processes in perception and cognition, are advancing with every day, yet we are still at some remove from making all but the most general or speculative statements. This does not, of course, mean that we have to stand by and wait for the technology to improve or to reach some critical point. As with any meaningful discipline in scholarship and science, CSR proceeds in halting but definite steps, and we hope that the essays in this issue demonstrate what some of us in the area of Tantric Studies can provide to the dialogue. None of these essays are by cognitive scientists, so we ask our colleagues in the sciences to consider how the dialogue might be furthered through their own efforts in understanding Tantra.
For those unfamiliar with Tantric Studies (itself a fairly recent interdisciplinary field of academic pursuits),1 the field is one which examines “Tantra”, a broad range of originally pan-Asian religious traditions connected to Hinduism, Buddhism, Jainism, Taoism, and various indigenous communities. As with the ongoing scholarly dispute over basic methodologies, terminology and vocabulary in CSR, there has been lively debate over the key term of “Tantra”, as well as how far back it goes in history.2 Dating from no later than the 4th century CE in South Asia, Tantra is difficult to define, but perhaps best regarded through the lens of a “polythetic” category as proposed by Douglas Renfrew Brooks.3 Tantra may be regarded as historical systems of ritual practices in which an individual engages with a layered cosmology, utilizing diverse senses, cognitive capacities, and sexuality in order to achieve elevated states of consciousness and embodied liberation. There are innumerable iterations of Tantra, ranging from theistic to nontheistic, although most seek to “reverse” the flow of cosmic process and manifestation in order to lead the individual to some heightened, typically unitive, state. To achieve this, most Tantra employ psychophysical techniques from the many streams of Yoga, but also “transgressive” actions such as ritualized sexual intercourse and ingestion of “impure” substances.
Besides social and historical approaches to reading cultures, studies on mysticism have also helped shape the scholarship of Tantric studies in our times. Following in the footsteps of figures such as William James and Carl Jung, particularly in their interest in anything mystical, scholars such as Mircia Eliade (1969), Agehananda Bharati (1964), John Woodroffe (1972, 1974, 1978), W. T. Stace (1960) and R. C. Zaehner (1972) have addressed issues such as altered or transformed states of consciousness (samāveśa), various forms of ecstasy and samādhi states, and mediumship or possession. Tantric transgression of social norms has also contributed to some of the scholarship in the field of Tantra, and the works of Jeffrey Kripal (1998, 2001) exemplify this dimension. The interplay of mystical power that is cultivated by Tantric practitioners and the dynamics of real power in the political realm have been examined by scholars such as Hugh Urban (2001a, 2001b). Although the scholarship in these areas has not generally analyzed the cognitive domains of Tantric practice, it has nonetheless shaped the discourse on studying Tantra in the West, including our efforts to engage a cognitive approach.
Scholarship in Tantric Studies has been a global effort for many decades, and a leading figure has been Alexis Sanderson, who has not only provided the field with magisterial translations and studies (e.g., Sanderson 1990, 2007, 2009) of Śaiva Tantra, but who has also mentored and inspired a generation of other scholars, including Dominic Goodall (2004). Another major scholar has been the late André Padoux (1990, 2000, 2004, 2010), who examined Tantric ritual language and prepared a useful technical dictionary of Tantric terms. Mark Dyczkowski, from his base in Varanasi, India has also been prolific, with many translations and studies (e.g., Dyczkowski 1988, 2000, 2004a, 2004b, 2009). Raniero Gnoli has also provided us with superb translations and studies (e.g., Gnoli 1972), as have Raffaele Torella (2013) and John Nemec (2011). There has also been much fine scholarship in the area of Buddhist Tantra, as well as ongoing debates on whether Hindu or Buddhist Tantra developed first. Again, this is not intended as a literature survey, but for an overview of Buddhist Tantra in general (and on specific traditions), readers may consult Davidson (2005, 2012), Jackson (2004), Gray (2007), Orzech et al. (2010), Acri (2016), Isaacson (1998, 2013), Wallace (2001, 2011), Snellgrove (1959, 1987), Wayman (1991, 1993), and Wayman and Lessing (2008).
One of the many reasons why Tantra has been so successful in the history of religions (especially in Asia), and why it provides such a fine topic for study by the CSR, is that it has promised its followers enriched and deepened modes of human experience. Tantra takes both consciousness and embodiment seriously, and offers its own solutions to the dilemma of human suffering and the perils of worldly existence. The cosmic significance and cognition of not only sound and hearing, but quite literally all of the senses, makes Tantra a perfect subject for CSR. There have been countless practitioners of Tantra, but far fewer academics who possess the language skills and intellectual training to engage with the material. Thus, in this ongoing collaboration between CSR and Tantric Studies we will require the cooperation of neuroscientists, cognitive scientists, and engaged scholars from the humanities and social sciences. This dialogue can become problematic, but seems to us well worth the effort. It may well lead us to a deeper and broader understanding and appreciation of what it means to be human, always a worthy goal.
Although this is not the place to consider the history of cognitive science or to conduct a literature review, it is useful to look at a few key points in order to set a context for this special issue. Many of the theoretical origins of CS can be traced to developments dating back to the 1940s and 1950s, as well as to the works of linguists such as Noam Chomsky and George Miller (Goldberg 2008, p. 59). Today, the field of CS is incredibly diverse, and includes (but is not limited to) artificial intelligence, psychology, neuroscience, linguistics, anthropology, philosophy and, most recently, religious studies.
One recent emphasis, especially in CSR, is to understand how religious experience “works” from the levels of neural and cognitive functions “up,” or, as noted by Jensine Andresen (2001, p. 1, cited in Goldberg 2008, p. 63) “from the bottom up,” rather than simply as a “sui generis” phenomenon. Without specifically discounting the more cosmic aspirations of the human religious quest, CSR seeks out the “nooks and crannies,” as it were, of the interfacing between the human brain/cognitive system and the surrounding cosmos. While setting aside the major ontological dilemma of the existence of the sacred and the “sui generis” nature of religious experience, this approach nicely complements the central emphasis on context by historians of religions. A person may be having a vision of a divine being, but that experience is embedded not only in the sociocultural and religious contexts of the individuals, but also in the neural and cognitive systems of the human body.
While the insights of William James, W. T. Stace, R. C. Zaehner and others have contributed to the overall positive tone of CSR (e.g., that religion is not necessarily “pathological”), the field owes a great deal to a number of recent thinkers, including Pascal Boyer (1994, 2001), Stewart Guthrie (1980, 1993), E. Thomas Lawson and Robert McCauley (Lawson and McCauley 1990, 2002) and Dan Sperber (1996). Their emphasis on the cognitive bases of religious experience has been influential in moving the field forward.
Another critical dimension of CSR has come from efforts in the areas of neurobiology and neuroscience, including the work of James Ashbrook (1993), Eugene d’Aquili and Andrew Newberg (D’Aquili and Newberg 1993, 1999, 2000), E. Gelhorn and W. F. Kiely (Gelhorn and Kiely 1972), Michael Persinger (1987), Jensine Andresen (2000), Christof Koch (2004), and Patrick McNamara (2001, 2009, 2016) among others. Many of these works have been controversial, to say the least, but all of them have furthered the collaborations between cognitive science and religious studies, leading to CSR. McNamara, whose work partly inspired a recent essay by Hayes (2014) on using cognitive science in the study of Hindu Tantra, was also the focus of an excellent symposium featuring Ann Taves, Jared Lindahl, Travis Chilcott, and Kelly Bulkeley (Taves et al. 2011). This dialogue, featuring four scholars who are doing groundbreaking work in CSR illustrates some of the dynamics emerging as scholars of religious studies engage with neuroscientists such as McNamara. Again, we hope that this current special issue adds to this dynamic.
From the prolific field of contemporary, or conceptual metaphor theory, CSR has benefitted by many interesting works by scholars such as George Lakoff and Mark Johnson (Lakoff and Johnson 1980, 1999), Mark Turner (2002, 2006), Anthony Ortony (1993), Barre Toelken (1995), and Zoltan Kövecses (1986, 1990, 1999, 2000, 2002, 2005, 2006, 2008, 2015). Emerging partly from these studies of metaphor, we also have the superb work on conceptual blending theory by Gilles Fauconnier and Mark Turner (Fauconnier and Turner 2002) as well as Mark Turner’s (2014) update on the theory. Edward Slingerland (2008) provides a magisterial overview of the possible interfaces between the humanities and cognitive sciences and, more recently, a CSR study of Chinese Taoism (Slingerland 2014). As noted above, we have also had splendid work from Ann Taves (2009), Travis Chilcott (2015, 2016), and Kelly Bulkeley (1994, 2004, 2005, 2017). Bulkeley has made important contributions in bringing together cognitive scientists and religious studies scholars, and more specifically in the area of the cognitive aspects of dreams and imagination in religious experience. Ellen Goldberg examined haṭha yoga using cognitive science in a useful essay (Goldberg 2005). Finally, the co-editors of this special issue, Glen Hayes (2003, 2005, 2006, 2012c, 2014) and Sthaneshwar Timalsina (2007, 2012, 2013, 2015a, 2015b, 2016), have also published numerous works in recent years dealing with various aspects of CSR in the study of Tantra and Yoga.
There are new publications and collaborations in the field of CSR virtually every month, if not weekly. At noted learned societies such as the American Academy of Religion (AAR) there are now established program units such as the Cognitive Science of Religion Group at the AAR, and smaller societies such as the Society for Tantric Studies (STS) have included panels on CSR in recent conferences (beyond the 2016 meeting). Readers could certainly add their own examples to this list. There are a number of excellent academic journals dedicated to CSR such as the Journal of the Cognitive Science of Religion, Journal of Cognitive Historiography, Journal of Consciousness Studies, Journal of Cognition and Culture, Religion, Brain, & Behavior, and the International Journal for the Psychology of Religion (IJPR). In 2016 we saw the publication of an important anthology/workbook bringing together many scholars of cognitive science and religious studies: Religion: Mental Religion, edited by Niki Clements (Clements 2016). Although intended for a general audience, this book provides us with a good “snapshot” of the state of the field in 2016, with chapters devoted to evolutionary psychology, cognitive linguistics, conceptual blending, neuroscience, entheogens, theory of mind, and dream states. Taken all together, these developments, as well as this special issue, demonstrate that CSR is indeed a promising avenue for emerging scholarly inquiry, and we hope that the following essays enhance the dialogue. Our attempt to present a volume on Tantric Studies and Cognitive Science is based on this simple premise: we can investigate and analyze some of the most interesting domains of human experience using insights and methods from cognitive science.

Essays in this Special Issue

In developing this new field at the intersection between contemplative practices and cognitive science, the first challenge is the need for a new vocabulary, as suggested by Hayes (2014). However, it can be difficult to negotiate the boundary between the liberating and transforming experiences found in tantric and yogic practices on one hand and the reductive and embodied domains of cognitive science. Keeping this in mind, our first essay, by Jeffrey Ruff, explores selected issues of confusion, mindfulness, humor, and dispassionate vulnerability to establish a conversation between contemplative and esoteric traditions on one side and neuroscience, cognitive science, and cognitive linguistics on the other. The argument Ruff has made does not merely develop some terms and redefine some concepts. He moves beyond this basic premise and argues for a bi-directional influence, with the underlying assumption that this conversation will help us better understand contemplative traditions. Ruff clearly outlines that this is not an effort to subsume one discipline within another. On the contrary, he argues that these disciplines are irreplaceable and incommensurable. With this premise, he seeks to establish a new paradigm that works with incommensurable concepts to assist in understanding cultural phenomena. This paper thus lays the foundation for the need of a disciplinary dialogue between cognitive science and contemplative practices.
The confluence between these disciplines has a potential to illuminate various archaic forms of beliefs and practices, and the language used in packaging such practices. Tantric language in particular is typically obscure, and pursuing a literal meaning often fails to unravel the texts and traditions. Keeping this in mind, Sthaneshwar Timalsina explores the cognitive domains of Tantric language. Tantric texts are saturated with metaphors, and metonymy is central to Tantric symbolism, both in literary and visual forms. Even Tantric rituals are an extension of metonymic transactions. Various concepts from Vedic and autochthonous practices are integrated in Tantric practice, and both the private, meditative discourse and public ritual domains demonstrate a complex blending of concepts from different sources. Reading Tantric texts or unpacking the symbolism beneath archaic literature, following Timalsina, is easier if some of the concepts from cognitive linguistics are applied in this effort. Timalsina argues that the application of the contemporary theory of body schema, metonymy, metaphor, and conceptual blending supports a much deeper understanding of Tantric language. In both the empowered language of mantric speech and in the cosmic visual diagrams of maṇḍala, the compression of meaning from different inputs is essential, and the source itself needs to be deciphered by applying some or another metaphor theory. Mystical experience, the central goal of contemplative practice, is regarded as accessible through the aspirant’s ability to explore a cosmology of multisensory layered meanings. Thus, any scholarly effort in understanding the texts or visualizing deity images may fruitfully engage the analytical decompression of meaning through the application of some cognitive linguistic theories. Timalsina argues that this approach has more to offer in understanding the cultural domains than the models developed during colonial and imperial times, as well as in more recent scholarly approaches.
The first concern some scholars may have upon learning of this new endeavor is: how can the confluence of the spiritual practices of Tantra with the necessarily reductive disciplines of contemporary neuroscience, cognitive science, and other behavioral studies even be possible? The general premise underlying this posture is just an extension of the problem of the ongoing dialogue (and often disagreement) between religion and science in general.4 The dilemma of this stance, however, is the problematic system of rigid, binary categories of ‘religion’ versus ‘science,’ and the ongoing effort to keep them epistemologically apart. This effort, while understandable to some degree, has caused more confusion than it has solved problems. Furthermore, what constitutes ‘religious’ in the Abrahamic traditions may not apply when reading Asian traditions. Mind-body dualism is one such area.5 In order to demonstrate a unique understanding of the body-mind relationship in the Tantric paradigm, Loriliai Biernacki seeks to link an understanding of the relation between mind and body with an analysis of the philosophy of the renowned Kashmiri polymath Abhinavagupta (ca. 1000 CE) and the methods of neuroscientist and psychiatrist Giulio Tononi. Biernacki argues that both Abhinava and Tononi have remarkably similar methods in linking the mind and the body, and to ground this claim, she introduces Tononi’s Integrated Information Theory, particularly its underlying assumption of the need to start from a phenomenology of experience to explain consciousness and resolve the mind-body problem. Abhinavagupta, Biernacki points out, makes similar connections when addressing the concepts of knowledge (jñāna) and action (kriyā). Both Abhinavaguptian and Tononi’s models deviate from Cartesian dualism and the problems that ensue.
There are a number of underlying assumptions in the Abhinavaguptian paradigm that Biernacki identifies in her attempt to establish a bridge between his and Tononi’s philosophy of consciousness. For Abhinava, matter is fundamentally real, and materiality and subjectivity are interlinked. Following the premise that the world is real and causally operative, Abhinava explores a greater communication between the domains of subjectivity and materiality. For Tononi, information is both causal and intrinsic. This is where Biernacki finds a greater communication between his philosophy and Abhinava’s ‘dual-aspect monism.’ She argues that Abhinavaguptian monism is the culmination of panpsychism, and on this ground, she suggests that Tononi’s theory provides a better explanation when linked with Abhinava’s philosophy. Biernacki finally argues that Tononi’s assertion that information is both ‘causal and intrinsic’ mirrors Abhinavagupta’s understanding that action (kriyā) is a fundamental component of subjectivity.
In the last several decades, there has been a growth of research in contemplative practices and neuroscience, with observable data analysis and neural imaging based on fMRI and MRI research of various meditation practices.6 These efforts have been used by scholars and scientists to argue that there may be a direct correlation between mental health and yoga, meditation, chanting, and mindfulness practices, among others. These studies have also pointed to the direction of a greater communication between mind, body, and the ways our corporeal states and functions directly affect our mental wellbeing. Extending the arguments of Biernacki, Jeffrey Lidke’s paper explores the possible interactions between the mind and the body, with a focus on an initial analysis of yogic practice and its possible effect on the nervous system.
Lidke appeals in his paper for further neuroscientific research on Tantric practices, with an underlying assumption that salient features of Tantric practice might have an observable effect on our nervous system. His paper examines meditative practices, particularly the apophatic or sense-denying contemplative practices, combined with outward-gazing kataphatic or sense-activating ritual practices as found in Abhinavaguptian Tantric literature. These practices suggest the possibility of a harmony between a simultaneous activation of different sensory-nervous systems, recalling the multi-layered cosmologies of many Tantric traditions. The “bi-directional gaze” of Abhinava, in Lidke’s understanding, has a heightened cognitive activity where the subject finds a balance between being active in the world matched with contemplatively being withdrawn from the elements that obstruct the subject’s experience of oneness identical with the highest reality, Śiva. In this complex mode of cognitive processing, Lidke finds a total Tantric perfection. His argument is that this state of maximized cognitive practice simultaneously activates the parasympathetic and sympathetic nervous systems, although these conclusions still need to be supported by additional scientific research. Lidke’s arguments stem from his basic stance that such further neurological research might identify a potential transformation in a subject’s somatic states as a result of contemplative practices. In this, Lidke expresses the underlying view that there is much to be gained through emerging collaboration of CSR and Tantric Studies.
Just as Lidke argues for a better understanding of contemplative practice through neurological research with a focus on one particular Tantric meditative practice, Travis Chilcott, another contributor of the volume, advances his arguments based on another aspect of Tantra: the practices of devotion, bhakti. One of the problems in studying Tantra is the open-endedness of its disciplinary parameters, without boundaries thatseparate other disciplines and practices within Hindu and Buddhist contemplative traditions. Bhakti, a devotional practice and also an emotive state with the surge of love, is one such example that overlaps parameters of everyday Hinduism, Buddhism, and Tantric practices. This paper explores the transformation of cognition in the devotional practices of bhakti in order to unravel the emotional transformation through bhakti, with a focus on Gauḍīya Vaiṣṇavism. Chilcott argues that the cultivation of selfless love, in the case of bhakti, can be compared with the therapeutic model of schema therapy. In bhakti practice, as Chilcott argues, there is a focus on “working through” the feelings of possessiveness over objects of the world, with a systematic deconstruction of “my-ness” (mamatā). In these practices, following Chilcott, there is a conscious attempt to rework and modulate how one relates to and responds to objects in the world and the subject’s relationship with the worldly objects. Chilcott concludes with an argument that connectionist theories of cognition and learning have the potential to offer an explanatory framework for understanding the dynamics of bhakti-oriented practices epitomized in Gauḍīya Vaiṣṇava discourse.
There are fundamental philosophical differences between Mahāyāna Buddhist and Śaiva Śākta Tantric practices. Nevertheless, numerous deities, maṇḍalas, mantras, and rituals that are commonly shared demonstrate a cultural fluidity in which Tantric Śaivism and Vajrayāna Buddhism evolved. Keeping this transaction in mind, the last two chapters in this volume focus on Buddhist Tantric practices, the first by Richard K. Payne and the second by Brendan Ozawa-de Silva. In his pioneering essay, Payne engages Buddhist Tantric ritual for an understanding of “ritual syntax.” The private contemplative domain and the domain of bhakti each have their own spheres. Rituals, with their long history in pan-Indian culture, are central to temple religion, and can be publicly performed. The calming and transformative effects of the contemplative practices and the surge of emotion through bhakti are not as easy to objectively analyze as rituals found in the public domain. Payne argues that the cognitive domains of ritual transactions provide a fuller theoretical basis for comprehending complex social transactions in ritualized activities. Payne’s analogy of ritual and language rests on some demarcation where production is not the same as analysis, syntax is not the same as semantics, ritual and cognition are not the same as mental processes, and syntax is not the same as language.
Payne uses insight meditation and homa, fire ritual, as instances for the cognitive study of ritual. His analogy of ‘ritual syntax’ here is to map the ordering of elements, as well as organizing elements in a hierarchical order. Payne observes that, just as syntax allows sentential elements to move within a sentence, ritual tends towards a form of invariance. Payne makes a distinction between the ritual domain and syntax by observing that rituals are often modeled on ordinary activities, producing some form of motivation. In this analysis, Payne conceives of cognition as operating at the intersection of body, mind, and the environment, rather than as exclusively mental objects and operations. By borrowing Catherine Bell’s conception of ritualization, Payne concludes in his paper that the dichotomy between the mind and the body can be avoided, and he uses ritual homa as an example of the intersection between the mental and physical. His conclusion is grounded on the assumption that ordinary activity is an essential factor for ritual syntax, as it serves as the model for a ritual. From within ordinary activity, Payne finds that metaphoric elements are applied to the ritual domain in order to discern and to attribute meaning.
Contemplative practices are private, and their transformative role in moral and ethical life is a topic for further consideration. Various Tantric practices transgress societal norms, and Tantric texts, Hindu and Buddhist alike, are full of such examples. If there is a measurable subjective transformation by means of contemplative practice, or if there is a positive effect of meditative practices, it is quite reasonable to explore a possible ethical consciousness by means of such practices. The ethical question becomes particularly poignant in the Buddhist Tantric paradigm, as the ethical in the human in this paradigm does not have any divine origin, and is dependently originated in some or another enactive process. Brendan Ozawa-de Silva addresses this issue in the last chapter of this volume.
Ozawa-de Silva argues that the issue of ethics must be addressed as a framework for understanding contemplative practice. In this regard, he explores the Tibetan Buddhist Lojong tradition and the various secularized practices stemming from it, considering the cultivation of compassion as an overarching theme in his discussion. There are two underlying assumptions in his analysis. First, he argues that the spiritual practice of compassion has a potential for secularized understanding of compassion in the global humanistic paradigm. Beyond this, the cultivation of compassion has the potential for functioning as a guiding principle in cultivating moral subjects in our global world. Ozawa-de Silva’s focus on the Lojong tradition highlights the development of compassion in progressive stages. Ozawa-de Silva also points out that there is not a sharp distinction between emotion and cognition, and he finds this particularly helpful in advancing the argument that cultivating certain concepts can have a measurable transformation in one’s emotional life, resulting in the development of moral subjects by means of compassion practices.
These essays are offered as just one step in the emerging dialogue between CSR and more traditional disciplines in the humanities and social sciences. In this case, we consider that the potential for a dialogue between Tantric Studies and CSR is promising and well worth taking to new levels and in new directions. Already, we have seen initial collaborations in learned societies such as the American Academy of Religions and the Society for Tantric Studies. This approach, we hope, helps advance both the ‘soft’ and ‘hard’ sciences by each providing the analytical framework and research materials for the other. The underlying Tantric philosophical monism, a greater exchange between body and mind in this philosophical paradigm, and an intricate relation between cognitive and emotional domains, are some of the factors to make this conversation both inevitable and productive.
Tantric studies is still in its infancy. Thousands of manuscripts that have been digitized in the past three decades are waiting to be studied and analyzed (yet many not digitized are sadly deteriorating in archives and private collections). Besides Śaiva Tantras and the Buddhist Vajrayāna texts and traditions, there are Vaiṣṇava Tantras, Jain Tantras, and Islamic Tantras. Many Tantric texts evade the parameters of any specific sect and there are sets of texts dedicated to magic, supernatural phenomena, and so on. There are rituals and indigenous practices that involve trance and various potions. Some Tantras address metaphysical and philosophical concerns, while most reflect the daily life of the religious practitioners. A cognitive approach to reading Tantric texts and practices has not only the potential to bridge the two disciplines of religious studies and cognitive science. It can also help us to explain archaic and often misunderstood domains of human experience. It is up to future scholars to carry on the legacy of studying the nexus of religion and science, the project that was initially envisioned by pioneers such as (James [1902] 1980).

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Acri, Andrea, ed. 2016. Esoteric Buddhism in Mediaeval Maritime Asia: Network of Masters, Texts, Icons. Singapore: ISEAS—Yusof Ishak Institute. [Google Scholar]
  2. Andresen, Jensine. 2000. Meditation Meets Behavioral Medicine: The Story of Experimental Research in Medicine. Journal of Consciousness Studies 7: 17–76. [Google Scholar]
  3. Andresen, Jensine. 2001. Introduction: Towards a Cognitive Science of Religion. In Religion in Mind: Cognitive Perspectives on Religious Belief, Ritual, and Experience. Edited by Jensine Andresen. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 1–44. [Google Scholar]
  4. Ashbrook, James B. 1993. Brain, Culture, and the Human Spirit: Essays from an Emergent Evolutionary Perspective. Lanham: University Press of America. [Google Scholar]
  5. Bharati, Agehananda. 1964. The Tantric Tradition. London: Rider. [Google Scholar]
  6. Boyer, Pascal. 1994. The Naturalness of Religious Ideas: A Cognitive Theory of Religion. Berkeley: University of California Press. [Google Scholar]
  7. Boyer, Pascal. 2001. Religion Explained: The Evolutionary Origins of ReligiousThought. New York: Basic Books. [Google Scholar]
  8. Brooks, Douglas Renfrew. 1998. The Secret of the Three Cities: An Introduction to Hindu Sakta Tantrism. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. [Google Scholar]
  9. Bulkeley, Kelly. 1994. The Wilderness of Dreams: Exploring the Religious Meanings of Dreams in Modern Western Culture. SUNY Series in Dream Studies; Albany: SUNY Press. [Google Scholar]
  10. Bulkeley, Kelly. 2004. The Wondering Brain: Thinking about Religion With and Beyond Cognitive Neuroscience. New York: Routledge. [Google Scholar]
  11. Bulkeley, Kelly. 2005. Soul, Psyche, Brain: New Directions in the Study of Religion and Brain-Mind Science. New York: Palgrave Macmillan. [Google Scholar]
  12. Bulkeley, Kelly. 2017. An Introduction to the Psychology of Dreaming, 2nd ed. New York: Praeger. [Google Scholar]
  13. Chilcott, Travis. 2015. Directly perceiving Kṛṣṇa: Accounting for perceptual experiences of deities within the framework of naturalism. Religion 45: 532–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Chilcott, Travis. 2016. Transforming Adverse Cognition on the Path of Bhakti: Rule-Based Devotion, ‘My-Ness,’ and the Existential Condition of Bondage. Religions 7: 49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Clements, Nikki Kasumi, ed. 2016. Religion: Mental Religion. MacMillan Interdisciplinary Handbooks. New York: Gale, New York: Cengage. [Google Scholar]
  16. D’Aquili, Eugene G., and Andrew B. Newberg. 1993. Religious and Mystical States: A Neuropsychological Model. Zygon: Journal of Religion and Science 28: 177–99. [Google Scholar]
  17. D’Aquili, Eugene G., and Andrew B. Newberg. 1999. The Mystical Mind: Probing the Biology of Religious Experience. Minneapolis: Fortress. [Google Scholar]
  18. D’Aquili, Eugene G., and Andrew B. Newberg. 2000. The Neuropsychology of Religious and Spiritual Experience. Journal of Consciousness Studies 7: 251–66. [Google Scholar]
  19. Davidson, Ronald M. 2005. Tibetan Renaissance: Tantric Buddhism in the Rebirth of Tibetan Culture. New York: Columbia University Press. [Google Scholar]
  20. Davidson, Ronald M. 2012. Indian Esoteric Buddhism: A Social History of the Tantric Movement. New York: Columbia University Press. [Google Scholar]
  21. Dyczkowski, Mark S. G. 1988. The Canon of the Śaivāgama and the Kubjikā Tantras of the Western Kaula Tradition. Albany: State University of New York Press. [Google Scholar]
  22. Dyczkowski, Mark S. G. 2000. Kubjikā, Kālī, Tripura, and Trika. Nepal Research Centre Publications 22. Stuttgart: Franz Steiner Verlag. [Google Scholar]
  23. Dyczkowski, Mark S. G. 2004a. The Cult of the Goddess Kubjikā. In A Journey in the World of the Tantras. Varanasi: Indika Books, pp. 193–292. [Google Scholar]
  24. Dyczkowski, Mark S. G. 2004b. Kubjikā the Erotic Goddess: Sexual Potency, Transformation and Reversal in the Heterodox Theophanies of the Kubjikā Tantras. In A Journey in the World of the Tantras. Varanasi: Indika Books, pp. 175–92. [Google Scholar]
  25. Translated and Edited by Mark S. G. Dyczkowski. 2009, Manthānabhairavatantram Kumārikākhaṇḍaḥ: The Section Concerning the Virgin Goddess of the Tantra of the Churning Bhairava. 14 vols. Varanasi: D. K. Printworld.
  26. Eliade, Mircea. 1969. Yoga: Immortality and Freedom. Translated by Willard R. Trask. Bollingen Series, No. LVI; Princeton: Princeton University Press. [Google Scholar]
  27. Fauconnier, Gilles, and Mark Turner. 2002. The Way We Think: Conceptual Blending and Mind’s Hidden Complexities. New York: Basic Books. [Google Scholar]
  28. Gelhorn, Ernst, and William F. Kiely. 1972. Mystical states of consciousness: Neurophysiological and clinical aspects. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease 154: 399–405. [Google Scholar]
  29. Gnoli, Raniero. 1972. Luce delle sacre Scritture (Tantrāloka) di Abhinavagupta. Milan: Torino Unione Tipografico-Editrice. [Google Scholar]
  30. Goldberg, Ellen. 2005. Cognitive Science and Hathayoga. Zygon: Journal of Religion and Science 40: 613–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Goldberg, Ellen. 2008. Cognitive Science. In Studying Hinduism: Key Concepts and Methods. Edited by Sushil Mittal and Gene Thursby. London: Routledge, pp. 59–71. [Google Scholar]
  32. Goodall, Dominic. 2004. The Parākhyatantra. A Scripture of the Śaiva Siddhānta. Pondicherry: Institut Français de Pondichéry. [Google Scholar]
  33. Gray, David. 2007. The Cakrasamvara Tantra (The Discourse of Sri Heruka): Śrīherukābhidhāna: A Study and Annotated Translation. New York: American Institute of Buddhist Studies. [Google Scholar]
  34. Guthrie, Stewart. 1980. A Cognitive Theory of Religion. Current Anthropology 21: 181–203. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Guthrie, Stewart. 1993. Faces in the Clouds: A New Theory of Religion. New York: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
  36. Hayes, Glen Alexander. 2003. Metaphoric worlds and yoga in the Vaiṣṇava Sahajiyā Tantric traditions of medieval Bengal. In Yoga: The Indian Tradition. Edited by Ian Whicher and David Carpenter. New York: RoutledgeCurzon, pp. 162–84. [Google Scholar]
  37. Hayes, Glen Alexander. 2005. Contemporary Metaphor Theory and Alternative Views of Krishna and Rādhā in Vaishnava Sahajiyā Tantric Traditions. In Alternative Krishnas: Regional and Vernacular Variations on a Hindu Deity. Edited by Guy Beck. Albany: SUNY Press, pp. 19–32. [Google Scholar]
  38. Hayes, Glen Alexander. 2006. The Guru’s Tongue: Metaphor, Imagery, and Vernacular Language in Vaiṣṇava Sahajiyā Traditions. Pacific World: Journal of the Institute of Buddhist Studies 3: 41–71. [Google Scholar]
  39. Hayes, Glen Alexander. 2011. Tantric Studies: Issues, Methods, and Scholarly Collaborations. The Journal of Hindu Studies 4: 221–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Hayes, Glen Alexander. 2012a. Tantric Studies, Part 2: The Flagstaff Meetings: Issues, Methods, and Scholarly Collaborations. The Journal of Hindu Studies 5: 137–44. (accessed on 2 September 2017). [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Hayes, Glen Alexander. 2012b. Tantra. An entry in the Oxford Bibliographies Online. Available online: http://www.oxfordbibliographies.com (accessed on 2 September 2017). [CrossRef]
  42. Hayes, Glen Alexander. 2012c. Conceptual Blending Theory, ‘Reverse Amnesia’, and the Study of Tantra. The Journal of Hindu Studies 5: 193–209. (accessed on 2 September 2017). [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Hayes, Glen Alexander. 2014. Possible Selves, Body Schemas, and Sādhana: Using Cognitive Science and Neuroscience in the Study of Medieval Vaiṣṇava Sahajiyā Hindu Tantric Texts. Religions 5: 684–99. (accessed on 2 September 2017). [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Isaacson, Harunaga. 1998. Tantric Buddhism in India (from c. 800 to c. 1200). In Buddhismus in Geschichte und Gegenwart. Band II. Hamburg: Hamburg University, pp. 23–49. [Google Scholar]
  45. Isaacson, Harunaga. 2013. Yogācāra and Vajrayāna according to Ratnākaraśānti. In The Foundation for Yoga Practitioners: The Buddhist Yogācārabhūmi Treatise and Its Adaptation in India, East Asia, and Tibet. Edited by Ulrich Timme Kragh. Cambridge and London: Harvard University Press, pp. 1036–105. [Google Scholar]
  46. Jackson, Roger. 2004. Tantric Treasures: Three Collections of Mystical Verse from Buddhist India. New York: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
  47. James, William. 1980. The Varieties of Religious Experience: A Study in Human Nature. Middlesex: Penguin. First published 1902. [Google Scholar]
  48. Koch, Christof. 2004. The Quest for Consciousness: A Neurobiological Approach. Englewood: Roberts and Company. [Google Scholar]
  49. Kövecses, Zoltan. 1986. Metaphors of Anger, Pride and Love. Philadelphia: Benjamins. [Google Scholar]
  50. Kövecses, Zoltan. 1990. Emotion Concepts. New York: Springer. [Google Scholar]
  51. Kövecses, Zoltan. 1999. Metaphor. Does it constitute or reflect cultural models? In Metaphor in Cognitive Linguistics. Edited by Raymond W. Gibbs Jr. and Gerard Steen. Amsterdam and Philadelphia: John Benjamins, pp. 167–88. [Google Scholar]
  52. Kövecses, Zoltan. 2000. Metaphor and Emotion: Language, Culture and Body in Human Feeling. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
  53. Kövecses, Zoltan. 2002. Metaphor: A Practical Introduction. New York: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
  54. Kövecses, Zoltan. 2005. Metaphor and Culture: Universality and Variation. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
  55. Kövecses, Zoltan. 2006. Language, Mind and Culture: A Practical Introduction. Oxford: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
  56. Kövecses, Zoltan. 2008. Conceptual Metaphor Theory: Some Criticisms and Alternative Proposals. Annual Review of Cognitive Linguistics 6: 168–84. [Google Scholar]
  57. Kövecses, Zoltan. 2015. Where Metaphors Come From: Reconsidering Context in Metaphor. New York: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
  58. Kripal, Jeffrey J. 1998. Kali’s Child: The Mystical and the Erotic in the Life and Teachings of Ramakrishna. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. [Google Scholar]
  59. Kripal, Jeffrey J. 2001. Roads of Excess, Palaces of Wisdom: Eroticism and Reflexivity in the Study of Mysticism. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. [Google Scholar]
  60. Lakoff, George, and Mark Johnson. 1980. Metaphors We Live By. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. [Google Scholar]
  61. Lakoff, George, and Mark Johnson. 1999. Philosophy in the Flesh. The Embodied Mind and Its Challenge to Western Thought. New York: Basic Books. [Google Scholar]
  62. Lawson, E. Thomas, and Robert N. McCauley. 1990. Rethinking Religion: Connecting Cognition and Culture. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
  63. Lawson, E. Thomas, and Robert N. McCauley. 2002. Bringing Ritual to Mind: Psychological Foundations of Cultural Forms. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
  64. McNamara, Patrick. 2001. Religion and the Frontal Lobes. In Religion in Mind: Cognitive Perspectives on Religious Belief, Ritual, and Experience. Edited by Jensine Andresen. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 237–56. [Google Scholar]
  65. McNamara, Patrick. 2009. The Neuroscience of Religious Experience. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
  66. McNamara, Patrick. 2016. Dreams and Visions: How Religious Ideas Emerge in Sleep and Dreams. New York: Praeger. [Google Scholar]
  67. Nemec, John. 2011. The Ubiquitous Śiva: Somānanda’s Śivadṛṣṭi and His Tantric Interlocutors. New York: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
  68. Ortony, Anthony, ed. 1993. Metaphor and Thought, 2nd ed. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
  69. Orzech, Charles, Richard Payne, and Henrik Sorensen. 2010. Esoteric Buddhism and the Tantras in East Asia. Leiden: Brill. [Google Scholar]
  70. Padoux, Andre. 1990. Vac: The Concept of the Word in Selected Hindu Tantras. Delhi: Sri Satguru Publications. [Google Scholar]
  71. Padoux, Andre. 2000. Tāntrikābhidhānakośa: Dictionnaire des Termes Techniques de la Littérature Hindoue Tantrique. Edited by Hélène Brunner and Gerhard Oberhammer. Vienna: Austrian Academy of Science, vol. 1. [Google Scholar]
  72. Padoux, Andre. 2004. Tāntrikābhidhānakośa: Dictionnaire des Termes Techniques de la Littérature Hindoue Tantrique. Vienna: Austrian Academy of Science, vol. 2. [Google Scholar]
  73. Padoux, Andre. 2010. Comprendre le Tantrisme: Les Sources Hindoues. Paris: Albin Michel. [Google Scholar]
  74. Persinger, Michael. 1987. Neuropsychological Bases of God Beliefs. New York: Praeger. [Google Scholar]
  75. Sanderson, Alexis. 1990. The Visualization of the Deities of the Trika. In L’Image Divine Culte et Meditation dans L’Hindousme. Paris: Editions du CNRS, pp. 31–88. [Google Scholar]
  76. Sanderson, Alexis. 2007. The Śaiva Exegesis of Kashmir. In Mélanges tantriques à la mémoire d’Hélène Brunner/Tantric Studies in Memory of Hélène Brunner. Edited by Dominic Goodall and André Padoux. Pondicherry: IFI/EFEO, pp. 231–442. [Google Scholar]
  77. Sanderson, Alexis. 2009. The Śaiva Age: The Rise and Dominance of Śaivism during the Early Medieval Period. In Genesis and Development of Tantrism. Edited by Shingo Einoo. Institute of Oriental Culture Special Series; Tokyo: Institute of Oriental Culture, University of Tokyo, vol. 23, pp. 41–350. [Google Scholar]
  78. Slingerland, Edward. 2005. Conceptual Blending, Somatic Marking, and Normativity: A Case Example from Ancient China. Cognitive Linguistics 16: 557–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  79. Slingerland, Edward. 2008. What Science Offers the Humanities: Integrating Body and Culture. New York: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
  80. Slingerland, Edward. 2014. Trying Not to Try: The Art and Science of Spontaneity. New York: Crown Publications. [Google Scholar]
  81. Smith, Jonathan Z. 1978. Map Is Not Territory: Studies in the History of Religions. Leiden: E. J. Brill. [Google Scholar]
  82. Smith, Jonathan Z. 1982. Imagining Religion: From Babylon to Jonestown. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. [Google Scholar]
  83. Smith, Jonathan Z. 1987. To Take Place: Toward Theory in Ritual. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. [Google Scholar]
  84. Translated and Edited by David Snellgrove. 1959, Hevajra Tantra. 2 vols, Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  85. Snellgrove, David. 1987. Indo-Tibetan Buddhism. 2 vols, Boston: Shambhala. [Google Scholar]
  86. Sperber, Dan. 1996. Explaining Culture: A Naturalistic Approach. Oxford and Cambridge: Blackwell. [Google Scholar]
  87. Stace, Walter Terrance. 1960. Mysticism and Philosophy. Philadelphia: Lippincott. [Google Scholar]
  88. Taves, Ann. 2009. Religious Experience Reconsidered: A Building-Block Approach to the Study of Religion and Other Special Things. Princeton: Princeton University Press. [Google Scholar]
  89. Taves, Ann, Jared Lindahl, Travis Chilcott, and Kelly Bulkely. 2011. Review symposium on Patrick McNamara, The Neuroscience of Religious Experience (2009). Religion 41: 71–89. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  90. Timalsina, Sthaneshwar. 2007. Metaphors, Rasa, and Dhvani: Suggested Meaning in Tantric Esotericism. Method and Theory in the Study of Religion 19: 134–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  91. Timalsina, Sthaneshwar. 2012. Holographic Bodies and Metaphoric Limbs in Hindu Myths. Zeitschrift für Indologie und Südasienstudien 2: 167–86. [Google Scholar]
  92. Timalsina, Sthaneshwar. 2013. Imagining Reality: Image and Visualization in Classical Hinduism. SERAS: Southeast Regional Association for Asian Studies 35: 50–69. [Google Scholar]
  93. Timalsina, Sthaneshwar. 2015a. Tantric Visual Culture: A Cognitive Approach. London: Routledge. [Google Scholar]
  94. Timalsina, Sthaneshwar. 2015b. Language of Images: Visualization and Meaning in Tantras. New York: Peter Lang. [Google Scholar]
  95. Timalsina, Sthaneshwar. 2016. A Cognitive Approach to Tantric Language. Religions 7: 139. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  96. Toelken, Barre. 1995. Morning Dew and Roses: Nuance, Metaphor, and Meanings in Folksongs. Chicago: University of Illinois Press. [Google Scholar]
  97. Torella, Raffaele. 2013. The Īśvarapratyabhijñākārikā of Utpaladeva. Delhi: Motilal Banarsidass. [Google Scholar]
  98. Turner, Mark. 2002. The Cognitive Study of Art, Language, and Literature. Poetics Today 23: 9–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  99. Turner, Mark, ed. 2006. The Artful Mind: Cognitive Science and the Riddle of Human Creativity. New York: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
  100. Turner, Mark. 2014. The Origin of Ideas: Blending, Creativity, and the Human Spark. New York: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
  101. Urban, Hugh B. 2001a. Songs of Ecstasy: Tantric and Devotional Songs from Colonial Bengal. New York: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
  102. Urban, Hugh B. 2001b. The Economics of Ecstasy: Tantra, Secrecy and Power in Colonial Bengal. New York: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
  103. Urban, Hugh B. 2003. Tantra: Sex, Secrecy, Politics, and Power in the Study of Religion. Berkeley: University of California Press. [Google Scholar]
  104. Wallace, Vesna. 2001. The Inner Kalacakratantra: A Buddhist Tantric View of the Individual. New York: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
  105. Wallace, Vesna. 2011. The Kālacakratantra: The Chapter on Sādhanā, Together with the Vimalaprabhā Commentary. New York: American Institute of Buddhist Studies. [Google Scholar]
  106. Wayman, Alex. 1991. Yoga of the Guhyasamājatantra. The Arcane Lore of Forty Verses, a Buddhist Tantra Commentary. Delhi: Motilal Banarsidass. [Google Scholar]
  107. Wayman, Alex. 1993. The Buddhist Tantras: Light on Indo-Tibetan Esotericism. Delhi: Motilal Banarsidass. [Google Scholar]
  108. Wayman, Alex, and Ferdinand D. Lessing. 2008. Introduction to Buddhist Tantric Systems. Delhi: Motilal Banarsidass. [Google Scholar]
  109. White, David Gordon, ed. 2000. Tantra in Practice. Princeton Readings in Religion. Princeton: Princeton University Press. [Google Scholar]
  110. John Woodroffe, and (pseudonym: Arthur Avalon), transs. 1972, Tantra of the Great Liberation: Mahānirvāna Tantra. New York: Dover.
  111. John Woodroffe, and (pseudonym: Arthur Avalon), transs. 1974, The Serpent Power: Being the Ṣaṭ-Cakra-Nirūpaṇa and Pādukā-Paňcaka: Two Works on Laya-Yoga, Translated from the Sanskrit, with Introduction and Commentary, 7th ed. New York: Dover.
  112. John Woodroffe, and (pseudonym: Arthur Avalon), transs. 1978, Principles of Tantra: The Tantra-Tattva of Śrīyukta Śiva Candra Vidyāṇava Bhattacārya Mahodaya, 5th ed. 2 vols, Madras: Ganesh.
  113. Zaehner, Robert Charles. 1972. Zen, Drugs, and Mysticism. New York: Vintage Books. [Google Scholar]
1
On the history of Tantric Studies and the Society for Tantric Studies (STS), see (Hayes 2011, 2012a). As recent scholarship is most prolific, the essays in this Special Issue of Religions only examine small segments of the field.
2
For an excellent introduction to the study of Tantra, including translations of many texts, see White (2000). On selected aspects of the history of scholarship on Hindu Tantra see Brooks (1998), Urban (2003) and Hayes (2011, 2012b). Urban provides a wealth of material concerning the complicated issues related to the study of Tantra in Western scholarship and in popular culture.
3
See (Brooks 1998). His useful discussion of the ‘polythetic’ nature of the category of Tantra, following earlier ideas of Jonathan Z. Smith (1978, 1982, 1987), may be found on (pp. 53–72). This debate over the very category of “Tantra” was an early point of discussion in meetings of the Society for Tantric Studies (see Hayes 2011). For a good working definition of Tantra, see White (2000, p. 9). Hayes (2012c) provides a recent bibliography on Hindu Tantra.
4
For a very useful discussion of this see Edward Slingerland (2008).
5
On the problems of mind-body dualism in the study of Hinduism, see (Goldberg 2008).
6
For an important work by a practicing neuroscientist see (McNamara 2009). His approach was criticized by religious studies scholars; see (Taves et al. 2011).

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Hayes, G.A.; Timalsina, S. Introduction to “Cognitive Science and the Study of Yoga and Tantra”. Religions 2017, 8, 181. https://doi.org/10.3390/rel8090181

AMA Style

Hayes GA, Timalsina S. Introduction to “Cognitive Science and the Study of Yoga and Tantra”. Religions. 2017; 8(9):181. https://doi.org/10.3390/rel8090181

Chicago/Turabian Style

Hayes, Glen Alexander, and Sthaneshwar Timalsina. 2017. "Introduction to “Cognitive Science and the Study of Yoga and Tantra”" Religions 8, no. 9: 181. https://doi.org/10.3390/rel8090181

APA Style

Hayes, G. A., & Timalsina, S. (2017). Introduction to “Cognitive Science and the Study of Yoga and Tantra”. Religions, 8(9), 181. https://doi.org/10.3390/rel8090181

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop