Strange Bedfellows: Meditations on the Indispensable Virtues of Confusion, Mindfulness and Humor in the Neuroscientific and Cognitive Study of Esoteric and Contemplative Traditions1
Abstract
:“Hey! rub-a-dub, ho!, rub-a-dub, three maids in a tub.And who do you think were there?The butcher, the baker, the candlestick maker,And all of them going to the fair.”[3]
1. A New, “New Vocabulary”: Exploring Cognition, Neurosciences, Esoteric Traditions, and Contemplative Communities
Four Concerns: Other Research Projects, Conceptual Confusion, Ethical Issues, Defining “Reality”
2. Lidke’s Bi-Directional Gaze
2.1. Intrinsic Problems: Generalization and Sample Size
2.2. Understanding the New Normal Produced by Meditation Practices
3. Spezio: A New Ethnographic Neuroscience [1]
That result fascinated me—I would have dropped everything in order to find out what the cats were trying to do or say to the researchers. After all, when human beings behave that way we come up with a pretty fancy catalogue of virtues in order to account for it. But, of course, I was stupidly supposing that the point of these efforts was to understand animals, and it wasn’t at all. The point was simply to Do Science….([15], p. 225)
3.1. Research Experiments and Mindfulness
3.2. Humor and Contemplative Studies
3.3. The Humor and the Problems with Conceptual Comprehension
4. Conclusions
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Michael Spezio. “‘Bridge Laws’ and the ‘Neural Encoding’ of Mindfulness: Ethnographic Neuroscience and Opportunities for Interdisciplinary Contemplative Studies.” In Paper presented at “Listening Closely: Toward an Interdisciplinary Ethnographic Neuroscience of Contemplative practice,” (Contemplative Studies Group), the Annual Meeting of the Academy of Religion, Atlanta, GA, USA, 21 November 2015.
- Jeffrey S. Lidke. “The Potential of the Bi-Directional Gaze: A Call for Neuroscientific Research on the Simultaneous Activation of the Sympathetic and Parasympathetic Nervous Systems through Tantric Practice.” In Paper presented at “Listening Closely: Toward an Interdisciplinary Ethnographic Neuroscience of Contemplative practice,” (Contemplative Studies Group), the Annual Meeting of the Academy of Religion, Atlanta, GA, USA, 21 November 2015.
- Iona Opie, and Peter Opie, eds. (1951) 1997; “Rub-a-dub-dub.” In The Oxford Dictionary of Nursery Rhymes. New York: Oxford University Press. p. 447.
- Glen Alexander Hayes. “Possible Selves, Body Schemas, and Sādhana: Using Cognitive Science and Neuroscience in the Study of Medieval Vaiṣṇava Sahajiyā Hindu Tantric Texts.” Religions 5 (2014): 684–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sthaneshwar Timalsina. Tantric Visual Culture: A Cognitive Approach. New York: Routledge, 2015. [Google Scholar]
- Geoffrey Samuel. “Between Buddhism and Science, Between Mind and Body.” Religions 5 (2014): 560–79. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Norman A. S. Farb. “From Retreat Center to Clinic to Boardroom? Perils and Promises of the Modern Mindfulness Movement.” Religions 5 (2014): 1062–86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Robert N. McCauley, and E. Thomas Lawson. Bringing Ritual to Mind: Psychological Foundations of Cultural Forms. New York: Cambridge University Press, 2002. [Google Scholar]
- E. Thomas Lawson. “Toward a Cognitive Science of Religion.” Numen 47 (2000): 338–49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Harvey Whitehouse. Modes of Religiosity: A Cognitive Theory of Religious Transmission. New York: Altamira, 2004. [Google Scholar]
- “CBCT.” Available online: https://tibet.emory.edu/cognitively-based-compassion-training/index.html (accessed on 11 March 2016).
- Charles D. Laughlin, and Eugene G. d’Aquili. Biogenetic Structuralism. New York: Columbia University Press, 1974. [Google Scholar]
- Eugene G. d’Aquili, Charles D. Laughlin, and John McManus. The Spectrum of Ritual: A Biogenetic Structural Analysis. New York: Columbia University Press, 1979. [Google Scholar]
- Andrew Newberg, Eugene d’Aquili, and Vince Rause. Why God Won’t Go Away: Brain Science and the Biology of Belief. New York: Ballantine, 2001. [Google Scholar]
- Vicki Hearne. Adam’s Task: Calling Animals by Name. New York: Skyhorse Publishing, 2007. [Google Scholar]
- Auguste Barth. “Des Religions de L’Inde.” Revue De L'histoire Des Religions 5 (1882): 227–52. [Google Scholar]
- Jean Filliozat. Religion, Philosophy, Yoga: A Selection of Articles. reprint version; Translated by Maurice Shukla. Delhi: Motilal Banarsidass, 1991. [Google Scholar]
- Barbara Lex. “The Neurobiology of Ritual Trance.” In The Spectrum of Ritual: A Biogenetic Structural Analysis. Edited by Eugene G. d’Aquili, Charles Laughlin and John McManus. New York: Columbia University Press, 1979, pp. 117–51. [Google Scholar]
- Ernst Gellhorn. “The Emotions and the Ergotropic and Trophotropic Systems.” Psychologische Forschung 34 (1970): 48–94. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ernst Gellhorn, and William F. Kiely. “Autonomic Nervous System in Psychiatric Disorder.” In Biological Psychiatry. Edited by J. Mendels. New York: Wiley, 1973. [Google Scholar]
- Jeremy Barris. Sometimes Always True: Undogmatic Pluralism in Politics, Metaphysics, and Epistemology. New York: Fordham, 2015. [Google Scholar]
- 1These meditations or reflections grew out of conversations and responses to papers presented at “Listening Closely: Toward an Interdisciplinary Ethnographic Neuroscience of Contemplation” (Contemplative Studies Group) at the Annual Meeting of the Academy of Religion, Atlanta, 21 November 2015. In particular, there are three interrelated meditations. 1. What do the concepts involved in a neuroscientific study of contemplative practice require of us: what new ideas and vocabulary do we need? What do we already know? And, where do we go from here? 2. Specific response to Michael Spezio paper [1]. 3. Specific responses to Jeffrey Lidke’s paper [2]. This article was written in response to their conference papers. I have not had the opportunity to review any revisions that they may have subsequently incorporated for inclusion in this volume.
- 2Neuroimaging technology and the move of the super-computing to desktops and cell phones has affected both affordability and availability of neuroimaging technology in such a way to practically test various contemplative practices about which scholars of the previous century mostly merely theorized. There are exceptions. Some of this kind of testing of meditation in laboratory settings goes back to the 1970s and before, but those projects were limited in ways that now seem, at least theoretically, to be surmountable.
- 3Michael Spezio ([1], p. 1) employs the term Interdisciplinary Contemplative Studies (ICS) for self-reference, and I think that this expresses his recognition of an already crowded fields of overlapping (but not identical) approaches. ICS already has a research program, and so can be distinguished from some other ones (CSR, Biogenetic Structuralism, and others) that I briefly explore in this section. I do not know whether scholars of Tantra should adopt this term, or invent their own, or leave the matter undecided.
- 4Samuel [6] provides considerable insights into the different models of reality, their incommensurability, and their mutual and historically grounded effects on each other. He gives special attention to what science might learn from Buddhism, reversing and critiquing the naïve realism of scientific projects. Farb [7] contributes a sense of the ethical context, and Spezio extends this concern with ethics in his article.
- 5I would even suggest this, especially McCauley and Lawson’s [8] work (although there are many sources to choose from in this area) because it is carefully reasoned and clearly argued. It may be that CSR concepts, such as Superhuman Agency or Immediacy, or counterintuitive properties fall too far outside the practical research concerns of the scholars and communities discussed here, but being aware of these concepts and theories will help these projects to at least see other research programs: their “new vocabularies” and the twists and turns that come with a paradigm shift. There has been more recently a critical response to CSR that could additionally contribute to this “new vocabulary.”
- 6I am indebted to Vicki Hearne ([15], p. 226) for this kind of phrasing (“genuine difficulties and conceptual confusions”), where she is critiquing conceptual failures within science in general and, in particular, the theory of behaviorism. She does nuance this criticism with recognition that her argument is sharply defined and not a rejection of scientific approaches in general. I introduce and work through some her thought and examples in Section 3.
- 7Although his emphasis is different, this is the same kind of argument that Samuel ([6], Section 2 and generally throughout his article) makes about Buddhism “enrich[ing] and extend[ing] Western scientific understandings, rather than simply reducing them in order to fit in with what we think we already know” ([6], p. 576).
- 8Barth (1879; English 1883 [16]). I first read this quoted in Jean Filliozat’s 1931 work on yoga, p. 93; reprinted ([17], p. 270). Without going into a list, there have been many social scientific theories of contemplative behavior that treat at least some of its claims and practices as pathological. Although scholars of Tantra and Buddhist contemplation typically no longer fall into these characterizations, the stereotypes have not disappeared. Also, I am not suggesting the Buddhist mindfulness or lovingkindness, yoga, and Tantric practice are all the same thing, rather that dismissive Western attitudes have generally and unfairly stereotyped the traditions of Buddhists, Hindus, Daoists, and other traditions that they conceived as “mystical” or non-rational. More plainly, scientific and social scientific and philosophical traditions in the West have been clearly negative and judgmental about other traditions (or alternatively, romantic and idealistic, which have their own sets of distortions).
- 9By doing this, I simply beg the indulgence from the neuroscientists who might be following this discourse. Stimulation of ergotropic and trophotropic systems includes various forms of external stimuli, cortical activity in the brain, autonomic effects, somatic effects, and behavioral effects ([18], p. 136). “Arousal” and “rest” are generalizations at the observable “behavioral” level—the coherent manifestations and results of all of these systems and subsystems functioning in the ways that they do. See Lex [18] throughout. For the purposes of clarity, as I hope to make clear, this simple distinction will suffice for this part of the argument. Ultimately, it will be the neuroscientists, physical anthropologists, and others with particular forms of scientific expertise who teach the scholars of Tantra how to appropriately phrase these issues when developing this part of our “new vocabulary”.
- 12It is not my purpose to actually construct the arguments necessary here, but I will suggest that Plato’s “aporia” (being perplexed and confused, and arguably recognizing the importance of this state), the use of similar concepts in Ortega y Gasset’s sense of life as a dialectic and of life as a project, or in Heidegger and Wittgenstein’s thought, and in Post-structuralism. Zen Buddhists and Philosophical Daoism also take up confusion and specific programs of not knowing. The very esoteric nature of the Hindu Tantras, offers multiple points of reflection regarding different kinds of clarity and confusion. My point here is not to conclusively argue this in a cross cultural way, but to show that contemplative and philosophical communities (including the ones we are examining here) know a lot about confusion and not knowing, and suggest or argue in favor of working with confusion in ways that typical scientific thinking and popular forms of naïve realism are specifically formed to avoid. If the project we are exploring here is to succeed, it needs to recognize the texture and necessity of working through these counter-intuitive and pervasive issues in sustained and rigorous ways.
- 15I am deeply indebted to Jeremy Barris, scholar of philosophy, for this joke and the line of reasoning that I am exploring here. His insights into humor and meaning, sense and sensibility have largely come from personal communication and team-teaching. For readers who find this line of reasoning intriguing, I would recommend the forms it takes across a variety of contexts in his Sometimes Always True: Undogmatic Pluralism in Politics, Metaphysics, and Epistemology [21].
© 2016 by the author; licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC-BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Ruff, J.C. Strange Bedfellows: Meditations on the Indispensable Virtues of Confusion, Mindfulness and Humor in the Neuroscientific and Cognitive Study of Esoteric and Contemplative Traditions1. Religions 2016, 7, 113. https://doi.org/10.3390/rel7090113
Ruff JC. Strange Bedfellows: Meditations on the Indispensable Virtues of Confusion, Mindfulness and Humor in the Neuroscientific and Cognitive Study of Esoteric and Contemplative Traditions1. Religions. 2016; 7(9):113. https://doi.org/10.3390/rel7090113
Chicago/Turabian StyleRuff, Jeffrey C. 2016. "Strange Bedfellows: Meditations on the Indispensable Virtues of Confusion, Mindfulness and Humor in the Neuroscientific and Cognitive Study of Esoteric and Contemplative Traditions1" Religions 7, no. 9: 113. https://doi.org/10.3390/rel7090113
APA StyleRuff, J. C. (2016). Strange Bedfellows: Meditations on the Indispensable Virtues of Confusion, Mindfulness and Humor in the Neuroscientific and Cognitive Study of Esoteric and Contemplative Traditions1. Religions, 7(9), 113. https://doi.org/10.3390/rel7090113