Next Article in Journal
God, Gould, and the Panda’s Thumb
Next Article in Special Issue
Inverted Totems: On the Significance of “Woke” in the Culture Wars
Previous Article in Journal
Teaching Moral Ethics through Sermons: A Case Study on Gregory of Nyssa
Previous Article in Special Issue
The Hegemonic Character of Techno-Functional Neo-Immanentism and Its Relationship with Culture Wars
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Have the Inhabitants of France, Great Britain, Spain, and the US Been Secularized? An Analysis Comparing the Religious Data in These Countries

by
Vidal Díaz de Rada
and
Javier Gil-Gimeno
*
I-Communitas, Institute for Advanced Social Research, Public University of Navarra, 31006 Pamplona, Spain
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Religions 2023, 14(8), 1005; https://doi.org/10.3390/rel14081005
Submission received: 1 May 2023 / Revised: 23 July 2023 / Accepted: 28 July 2023 / Published: 7 August 2023
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Culture Wars and Their Socioreligious Background)

Abstract

:
This paper carries out a comparative analysis of the religious beliefs and practices of residents in France, Spain, the United Kingdom, and the United States of America, using two waves of the World Values Survey. The main objective is to investigate the impact that secularization has had on the religious experience in these countries. More specifically, the prospection is carried out around the Christian creed in its Protestant and Catholic manifestations, understood as the majority beliefs in these countries. To carry out this task, we compiled a series of data distributed around the following categories: Contextualization: The importance of religion within different aspects of life; level of religiosity and membership in religious denominations; the sphere of beliefs: Belief in God, belief in life after death, belief in hell, and belief in heaven; scope of practices; and the nones. Subsequently, we carry out an explanatory-interpretative analysis articulated around four questions or challenges faced by these religious forms in the context of secularization: 1. The crisis of Christianity; 2. the thesis of European exceptionalism; and 3. the rise of the nones. In conclusion, the data analyzed allow us to affirm—with nuances—the following: 1. The existence of a process of dechurching in the heart of Christianity; 2. the confirmation that the European case is exceptional if we compare it with other trends or other cultural programs of secularization; 3. that the area of greatest dechurching is linked to community practice, something that allows this research to adhere to Davie’s thesis, which defines the current religious situation as believing without belonging; and 4. as a consequence of the process of dechurching, there is a rise of a social group without religious adscription: The nones.

1. Introduction

Religion has been and is a field of study of great tradition within the sociological discipline. In Weber (1978), Max Weber tells us that the sociologist of religion must investigate religiously oriented social action. That is to say, the sociologist would not be interested so much in solving the metaphysical mystery surrounding the ultimate realities behind the religious phenomenon or behind suprasensible beings, but in analyzing how such beliefs are articulated in concrete modes of social interaction, and how they have an impact on what we consider real or social reality. To do so, we have to study the concrete phenomenon in a context—also concrete in action. The one we are going to focus on in this paper is that of secular modernity. As Charles Taylor (2007) points out, we live in the Secular Age, a time in which people develop their existence within an ‘immanent frame’, that is, in a context characterized by an order that can be understood without reference to external interventions.
We might be tempted to think that the religious is limited to social action around a series of divine figures and their impact on the different levels or systems of social life. To think of the reality of religion in general and of religion in particular in this way would be to disregard an important part of the religious reality or of the processes of social sacralization. In this sense, and in order to make an approach with guarantees to the object of study of religion in times of secularization, we consider it important to dwell on the analysis of the five stages of religious evolution analyzed by Robert N. Bellah (1969). For this North American sociologist of religion, societies have gone through five religious stages up to the present time: Primitive, archaic, historical, pre-modern, and modern.
Without wishing to be exhaustive—since this is not the main object of the study we are undertaking—we will briefly present them. The first would be characterized by the sacralization of the everyday, whether in the form of nature (animals and plants, as occurs in totemism) or of the ancestors. 2. In the second stage, a hiatus begins to occur between this and the other world, and in it, the gods begin to be the suprasensible elements par excellence. Both planes of reality are connected through what is known as the divine monarchy, as occurs with pre-axial or pre-prophetic Judaism. In these societies, the fundamental rituality is sacrifice, understood as a way of connecting the two shores of reality (Hénaff 2010); 3. In the third stage, the hiatus between this and the other world becomes stronger and is articulated in a reflexive way, oriented towards the human being’s search for salvation. Although venerating the divinity remains a crucial element in this phase of religious evolution, the human being begins to acquire an ever greater protagonism in his relationship with the divine: The central issue is no longer ingratiating oneself with the divinity (as was the case in the sacrificial era), but salvation—knowing whether we have been chosen or not, as the Protestants propose through the doctrine of predestination—which is why in this phase it is essential to articulate ways of salvation (Weber 1978) that effectively provide indications or soteriological certainties. This is the epoch of the development of universal religions (Weber 1983). 4. In the fourth stage, Bellah focuses on the Protestant rupture as a moment in which religiosity becomes individualized. The personal relationship of the individual with the divinity becomes the axis of this new religiosity (James 1917; Luckmann 1967; Beck 2009). 5. Finally, the fifth stage would be the modern stage. In it we witness a “collapse of the dualism that was so crucial to all the historic religions” (Bellah 1969, p. 79), which further strengthens the importance of the individual in terms of religious experience. In this sense, the modern stage would not be an a-religious stage, but one in which the “fundamental symbolization (…) is that of a dynamic multidimensional self capable, within limits, of continual self-transformation and capable, again within limits, of remaking the world including the very symbolic forms with which he deals with it, even the forms that state the unalterable conditions of his own existence” (Bellah 1969, p. 81). Another difference of the modern stage with respect to the previous ones is that, as we have already noticed previously, it develops in a context of ‘immanent frame’.
Therefore, the modern religious situation has not so much to do with the disappearance of religion or of the human capacity to sacralize. As Simmel would say, such capacity would be a matter of the human according to Simmel (2012), but rather it would attend to a metamorphosis (Lenoir 2005), to a transformation of the religious articulated around the transformations experienced in the rest of the social systems.
Thus, the feature that would define the situation in the modern era would not be that of the disappearance of the religious, but that of the articulation of ‘many altars’ (Berger 2014) from which a large number, a great plurality of forms and ways of being religious are articulated, in a scenario in which the backdrop is the capacity and obligation of the individual to endow his existence with meaning.
This implies that one of the ‘mantras’ (allow us to de-name it this way) on which what we will call the hegemonic thesis of secularization was articulated, has been proven wrong, which went as follows: “The more modern a society is, the more secular; and the more secular, the less religious” (Casanova 2009, pp. 1056–57). Although modern societies have reoriented themselves towards the world, that is, they have indeed become secularized, this has not implied a process of desacralization. In modern societies, Durkheim’s statement can be attested to when he points out in Durkheim (1995) that the circle of sacred things cannot be limited once and for all. What has happened in modern societies is that the contract of exclusivity that the religious had established with the divine has been broken, proliferating a long list of new formulas of religiosity based on sacralizations of the mundane, ranging from re-sacralizations of nature, to new sacralizations of social phenomena arising in the present age: Revolutionary cults (Mathiez 2012), the person (Durkheim 1973; Joas 2014), the civil (Bellah 1967), and so on. Similarly, modern cults can have a public (Casanova 1994) or private (James 1917; Luckmann 1967; Beck 2009) orientation. Therefore, and in accordance with the non-evolutionary evolutionary principle that is articulated through the observation that in social evolution “nothing is ever lost” (Bellah 2011; Donald 1991), in secularized societies, forms characteristic of past stages (for example, historical religions, including Christianity in its different manifestations) coexist with others that emerge as a result of the logics of action characteristic of our time.
Although this is the reality revealed by an exhaustive analysis of religion today, in this paper, we will focus on the situation of historical religious cults—specifically Christianity and its hegemonic forms—in four Western countries: France, Great Britain, Spain, and the USA. We should point out first of all that the specific socio-cultural contexts will offer us particularities of which we will try to record, although in the discussion, we will focus on the aspects they share, making nuances where necessary. Therefore, the main objective of this paper is to comparatively analyze—based on the data offered by the World Values Survey—the situation of Christianity in the context of secular modernity according to the data from the four selected countries.
This general objective implies several specific objectives: 1. Considering the functional differentiation of social systems, we want to analyze religion as one of the main axes where social life is articulated (Habermas 1998, p. 4); 2. to know the religious self-definition and its relationship with belonging to different religions; 3. the proposal of the privatization of religion (Casanova 2012) gives rise to two specific objectives: a. Belief in God, life after death, hell, and heaven surpasses religious self-definition, that is, the level of belief is higher than religious self-definition; b. knowledge of religious practices; considering that modern societies do not need to be organized as moral communities unified by a system of practices (Casanova 2012); and 4. delimitation of less religious people, specifying their characteriological traits.
It is also important to point out that when it comes to articulating the presentation of the results, it seemed interesting to cross-check the data with some basic socio-demographic variables such as sex, age (15–30, 31–49, 50–64, and 65 and more years), level of studies (lower, middle, and upper), relationship with the activity (working, unemployed, retired, student, and housewife), whether or not the respondent is part of the working population, and whether or not he/she is an immigrant.
In order to articulate our proposal, we present the following structure: In Section 2, we carry out an exhaustive presentation of the materials and methods to be used, delving into aspects related to how the World Values Survey collects the data, what information it offers us, and, most importantly, how we are going to use it. In Section 3, we present the main results organized around the following categories of analysis: Contextualization: The importance of religion within different aspects of life; level of religiosity and membership in religious denominations; the sphere of beliefs: Belief in God, belief in life after death, belief in hell, and belief in heaven; scope of practices; and the nones. We then carry out an explanatory-interpretative analysis articulated around four issues or challenges facing these religious forms in the context of secularization: 1. The “crisis” of Christianity; 2. the rise of the nones. Finally, and by way of a summary of the work carried out, we present in brief the results of our study.

2. Materials and Methods

One of the aspects that characterize social research at the dawn of the second decade of the century is the expansion of research of an international nature (among others, Harkness et al. 2003, 2010; Johnson et al. 2019), carrying out scientific studies of social, political, economic, religious, and cultural aspects of people in the world (World Values Survey 2022). Since the end of the 1970s, the expansion of this line of research followed the next decade with the founding of the World Values Survey (World Values Survey 2022).
A few years later, in 1984, appeared the International Social Survey Programme—ISSP, founded by American, German, British, and Australian organizations. Its purpose is to conduct annual surveys on a variety of topics relevant to the social sciences (The International Social Survey Programme 2022). In 2001, a turning point in this evolution took place in the form of the appearance of the European Social Survey, an international scientific survey conducted in Europe every two years (European Social Survey 2022), which will be in its tenth edition at the end of 2022.
This contextualization serves to explain the search for relevant data on religion, the topic of this article, although it has been difficult to find comparable research on both topics for France, Spain, the United Kingdom, and the United States. After an exhaustive search, it was decided to use the values surveys conducted by the European Values Study and the World Values Survey, based primarily on methodological comparability and the availability of data for these countries. Section 2.1 presents the methodological details of both studies, also explaining the use of two sources, one conducted between 2017 and 2018 and the other between 2005 and 2007. The denominations are sufficiently intuitive to reveal that the first corresponds to information collected in the years 2017 and 2018, the main database, and the other for information collected between the years 2005 and 2007. The first one will be the most used, using only the second one in order to determine the evolution in the last ten years.
These surveys, in addition to allowing knowledge of religious beliefs and practices, collect information on the place of birth of the interviewees and their parents, which makes it possible to identify whether they are first- or second-generation emigrants. The four countries analyzed have experienced large migratory flows of people with different religious beliefs. On some occasions, these people have continued with their beliefs in the countries of destination, and on others, there has been a process of assimilation—or secularization—to the habits and beliefs of the countries where they live:
-
9.7% of the people interviewed (464 cases) were born in another country and both of their parents were born in another country, so they will be defined as first-generation immigrants (see Table A1 in Appendix A).
-
There are also 8.2% of respondents (392 cases) in which one of the parents was born in another country and the respondent was born in the country where he/she resides, which leads to defining them as second-generation migrants.
-
There is a small group of respondents (1.8%, 85 cases) who were not born in the country where they reside but whose parents are natives there. The small size of this group, and the impossibility of aggregating it with any of the previous groups, led us to exclude them from the analysis.
When aggregated, 18% of the participants are immigrants, 9.7% of which are first-generation immigrants and 8.2% of which are second-generation immigrants. This allows the elaboration of two variables, one that differentiates between natives and immigrants and a second one that differentiates between types of immigrants. France and the United Kingdom are the countries with the highest rate of immigrants, with 23% and 21%, respectively, followed by the United States of America (17%). In Spain, the immigrant rate is notably lower, at 13%, with the distinctive element being the low number of second-generation immigrants (barely 15%, when the average for the four countries is 46% of immigrants).
The native–immigrant variable is significant in this study, as has already occurred in other surveys carried out by the World Values Survey (in waves 5 and 6, for example). The same occurs with other basic sociodemographic variables that we will also use in this work. When it comes to articulating the presentation of the results, it seemed interesting to cross-check the data with:
-
Socio-demographic variables: Sex, level of studies (lower, middle, and upper), and age1.
-
Socio-economic variables: Relationship with activity (working, unemployed, retired, student, or housewife) and whether or not the respondent is part of the working population.
The first are “stable” variables, as they do not change throughout life, and several studies (Davie 1994; Pérez-Agote 2015, and so on) have reported differences in the beliefs and practices of men and women, as well as those with more or less education. Age, the third sociodemographic variable, provides information on—on the one hand—the influence of the life cycle on religious beliefs and, on the other, the very influence of “getting older”, being closer to death.
The socioeconomic variables present less stability but are important insofar as they reflect the main activity of the respondent, an aspect related to the time available and the possibility of attending religious practices.
In short, these are “stable” variables in the interviewees, variables commonly used in social research to “segment” opinions and to locate subgroups with beliefs and habits different from those of the population as a whole. Having this information will allow the creation of a set of characteristics (for example, being male, intermediate age, etc.) that determine religious beliefs and habits.

2.1. About the Sources Used

As we have noted, the data used in this article come from general population surveys conducted by the European Values Study (https://europeanvaluesstudy.eu/) (accessed on 3 March 2023). These are face-to-face surveys of random samples of the population over 18 years of age living in private households. The universe under study is persons residing in each country, regardless of nationality or language, chosen using random procedures that make several visits to the households in the event that it is not possible to establish contact during the first visit. The samples follow the principles set by the European Values Study, based primarily on the collection of quality information and transparency of the processes (European Values Study 2020).
Of all the data files, this research uses the so-called Join EVS/WVS 2017–2022 (European Values Study 2022), elaborated by integrating the information collected by the European Values Study and the World Values Survey. The questionnaires were prepared independently by each organization. Thus, the European Values Study plans and conducts surveys in European countries using its questionnaire and methodological guidelines. The World Values Survey does the same in countries outside Europe, using its own questionnaire and methodological guidelines. Based on both studies, we carried out the Join EVS/WVS 2017–2022, which includes the questions and items common to both studies.
Of all the questions included in the Join EVS/WVS 2017–2022 in this work, we use the ones called “Religion Values”, specifically, in the Joint EVS/WVS v3.0., background questions WVS7-Q164 (importance of God in your life), Q165 to Q168 (beliefs), Q171 (attendance to religious services), Q172 (frequency with which you pray, only in European countries), Q171 (definition about your level of religiosity), and Q289 about professed religion (see Appendix B). Table 1 shows the sample sizes, the sampling error, and the dates of data collection.
The information in Table 1 shows a large difference in sample sizes between territories, which could lead to inaccuracies given, for example, the large sample size in the United States. This implies that the responses of each American are considered twice as large as the responses of the Spaniards. This non-proportional affixation, a consequence of the application of different sampling fractions2, means that when we wish to speak jointly of all the samples, it will be necessary to weight the data (Scheaffer et al. 2006). We have opted for decreasing those over-represented countries by using weighting coefficients, considering that each of the countries has a sample of 1200 cases.
Table 2 shows the original samples, the weighting coefficients, and the data considered in each country.
It has been pointed out that one of the objectives is to compare the information with the situation in the previous decade. In the case of the comparison with the 2005/07 results, the data come from the fifth wave of the World Values Survey, conducted between 2005 and 2007 (World Values Survey 2008). Table 3 shows the sample sizes, sampling error, and weighting coefficients used so that each respondent has the same relevance, considering a sample of 1001 cases in each country. The last column of this table shows the dates of data collection, so this study will be called “Study years 2005/07”.
Initially, frequency distributions will be used in order to make the first approximation of the data. To determine the influence of the traits of the people interviewed (6 identification variables3) on the content variables of the second section, we used Cramer’s V statistic, which indicates the magnitude of the association between two variables considering a range between 0 and 1: A value of 0 means no relationship and a value of 1 means total relationship; although, it should be noted that in tables of two variables, the value is usually much lower than 0.3. More interesting than the value of this statistic is its significance, which indicates whether the result obtained can be extrapolated to the population.
In the rest of the paper, logistic regressions will detect the influence of the different sociodemographic variables on the variable under study. Logistic regressions used the variables related to religion as the dependent term. The use of this technique will make it possible to determine the incidence of sociodemographic variables on the self-definition of oneself as a religious person, eliminating the influence of other sociodemographic variables. The independent variables considered were sex, age (15–30, 31–49, 50–64, and 65 and more years), level of studies (lower, middle, and upper), relationship with activity (working, unemployed, retired, student, and housewife), whether or not the respondent is part of the working population, and whether or not he/she is an immigrant4. These terms have been coded considering the last category as a reference, and their coefficients are interpreted as the effect on the dependent variable of a one-unit change in the independent variable, keeping all other variables constant.
To determine the distributions of each country in the religious variables, we used Bar charts (grouped), grouped bars to show the evolution between 2005/2007 and 2017, stacked bars to show the influence of religious denomination and the fact of belonging or not to a religion, and stacked columns to determine the influence of being born in the country of residence or not. In tables, we represent the results of the values of the regressions, that is, to account for the influence of the sociodemographic variables on each religious variable.

3. Results

We will now present the main results according to the selected dimensions of analysis, which are as follows:
-
Contextualization: The importance of religion within different aspects of life.
-
Level of religiosity and membership in religious denominations.
-
The sphere of beliefs: Belief in God, belief in life after death, belief in hell, and belief in heaven.
-
Scope of practices.
-
The nones.

3.1. Contextualization: The Importance of Religion within Different Aspects of Life

Figure 1 presents the consideration of the residents of the countries under study with respect to six aspects of life, with the possibility of answering ‘very important, ‘rather’, ‘not very’, or ‘not at all important’. Family is ‘very important’ for nine out of ten respondents, being the aspect that reaches the highest degree of importance: Nine out of ten define it as ‘very important’.
Considering only the ‘very important’ response, work and friends are the next aspects in order of importance, answers chosen by slightly more than half of the respondents (54% and 53%, respectively). However, when this response and the next one (‘rather’) are considered together, leisure time comes in second place (42 ‘very’ +47 ‘rather’), followed by friends and work. In both of these, approximately 80% of respondents consider them important, although—as noted—work and friends show higher choices of the most important response.
Focusing attention on the last two aspects of importance, located on the right-hand side of Figure 1, the situation changes considerably. Starting with politics, 49% of respondents consider it as ‘important’ in their life when the ‘very’ and ‘rather important’ responses are added together; although the ‘very important’ response is the lowest of all those considered.
Furthermore, 57% (29% + 28%) of respondents do NOT consider religion important in their lives, and only one in five consider it to be very important. This is, in short, the least valued aspect of the six aspects considered.
The country-by-country analysis shown in Figure 25 reveals little variation in family, friends, and leisure (standard deviations of 0.02, 0.06, and 0.09, respectively), increasing notably in politics (0.18), religion (0.25), and work (0.29). The latter shows the greatest differences between countries, an aspect to which we will not devote attention as it is far from the object of study of this paper. Religion is the second aspect that varies the most between countries, with a low value given by the French and great importance in North Americans standing out. Focusing on religion, Figure 3 shows the evolution over the last 10 years, since the 2005/07 measurement:
-
France presented the lowest levels of ‘very important’ (13%) in 2005, a situation that hardly changes a decade later. Now, the high number of people considering it ‘rather important’ in 2005 is reduced by 5 percentage points in 2017.
-
The UK had ‘very important’ levels of 20% in 2005, dropping five percentage points in a decade. ‘Rather important’, meanwhile, is up three points over this period.
-
The situation in Spain is slightly different from the rest of the countries, being the first country with a slight increase in ‘very important’ and a slight decrease in ‘rather important’.
-
Respondents from the United States experience the greatest decrease in the importance of religion, especially when considering the evaluation of religion as “very important’. The 47% who expressed this opinion in 2005/07 fell to 33% in the latest wave, a decline of 14 percentage points. The same trend is found in the ‘rather important’ category, although with a lower incidence (2 percentage points).
In short, there has been a decline in the importance of religion among respondents, primarily in the United States, with a grouped decline (very + rather) of 16%, followed by a 5% decline in France. In the United Kingdom, this situation is fainter, showing 2 percentage points, with the Spanish showing a different situation with a slight increase (1%) in the importance of religion.
At this point, it will be interesting to determine how the different sociodemographic variables influence the perception of the importance of religion, information that is shown in the figures in Appendix C. Beginning with gender, women consider it more important than men in all countries, with differences—in the ‘very important’ response—of between 3 and 5%, increasing to 11% in the case of Spain. The differences are similar in the case of ‘rather’, a category in which Spain returns to normality, and it is the United Kingdom where the differences between men and women increase. Spain and the United Kingdom are the countries with the largest differences between men and women (Cramer’s V6 of 0.17 and 0.16, respectively, both with significance < 0.0001).
In the case of age, the high percentages of the ‘very important’ response in the United States stand out (between 27 and 49%), with an increase with increasing age. This trend (an increasing importance of religiosity as age increases) is not as clear in European society (France, the United Kingdom, and Spain). However, in all countries, older people give more importance to religion, especially in Spain.
Despite this trend, in France and the United Kingdom, the youngest group (under 30 years of age) give more important than the next group (between 31 and 49 years of age and between 50 and 64 years of age). The most important differences in the case of age are found in Spain (Cramer’s V 0.32) and the United States (Cramer’s V 0.18), both with significance < 0.0001.
With regards to educational level, people with lower levels of education give greater importance to religion. Focusing on European countries, France and Spain stand out, where approximately half (44% in France and 49% in Spain) of people with a low level of education consider religion ‘very’ and ‘rather’ important; a situation that in the United Kingdom reaches 40%. This percentage decreases notably among those who have completed higher education, dropping to 33% in France and 26% in Spain, the lowest figures in the series. These figures are notably lower than those presented in North American society, with a greater importance of religion at all levels of studies, although greater in people with a low level of education, where 76% consider it ‘very’ and ‘rather important’. The common trend in the four countries is a reduction in this importance among those with higher levels of education, but with a “moderate” decrease in the case of the European countries, less accentuated in the case of the United Kingdom. The greatest differences are again found in Spain and the United States (Cramer’s V of 0.21 and 0.14, respectively, both with significance < 0.0001).
With regards to the relationship with activity, each of the groups will be differentiated in detail:
-
Religion is considered of little or no importance in all European countries in approximately 7 out of 10 employed people (71% in Spain, 69% in France, and 67% in the United Kingdom), a rate similar to that of the unemployed. This trend changes in the United Kingdom, where it drops to 59%. Only 46% of American workers (employees) consider religion little or unimportant, similar to the unemployed.
-
Students are the next group in terms of the number of respondents who do not consider religion important, with percentages that reach 60–64% in France and Spain, respectively, dropping to 50% in the United Kingdom and the United States.
-
The percentage of retired people who attach little importance to religion is slightly more than half of the population in France and the United Kingdom (58% and 55%, respectively), falling significantly in Spain (45%) and even more in the United States (32%).
-
Something similar happens to those who carry out unpaid work, with high considerations of ‘not very important’ and ‘not at all important’ in the United Kingdom and France (63% and 56%, respectively), and very low in the United States and Spain (37% and 30%, respectively).
Again, Spain is the country where there is the greatest difference (Cramer’s V of 0.32), followed by France and, at a short distance, the United States (both with Cramer’s V of 0.16, all with significance < 0.0001).
In terms of geographic origin, most of those born in the country where they reside are characterized by a low importance of religion in their life (France with 69% and the UK with 68%), reducing to 62% in the case of Spaniards and up to 43% in those residing in the United States.
In all European countries, the first generation is characterized by attaching great importance to religion in their life, with approximately 60% of those interviewed considering it very and fairly important. In the second generation, religion loses importance and is only important for slightly more than half of the residents in France and the United Kingdom and for one in four Spaniards. The greatest differences between the groups occur in the United Kingdom and France (Cramer’s V of 0.31 and 0.28, with significance < 0.0001), and there is no relationship in the United States.
In short, the groups that attach more importance to religion are women (10 percentage points difference with respect to men), people over 64 years of age, respondents with low education, those with unpaid domestic jobs and retirees, and first-generation immigrants. The comparison of Cramer’s V’s reveals the greater influence of geographic origin in France and the United Kingdom, as well as age and activity relatedness in Spain (and somewhat less in the United States).
In the same way, in this section, we have analyzed the importance of religion within different aspects of life, by way of contextualization, and it has revealed the low importance of religion when compared with family, work, friends, leisure time and politics. It is an importance that, moreover, has decreased since 2005/07.

3.2. Level of Religiosity and Membership in Religious Denominations

Having exposed the low importance of religion in the life of the interviewees in the previous paragraph, it is time to determine the “religious denomination”, to what extent the interviewees consider themselves religious, as well as their membership in religions. These two aspects will be addressed in this section.
Starting with “religious denomination”, 47% of the residents in the countries under study consider themselves as religious, 39% as non-religious, and 15% has convinced atheists. Figure 4 shows the responses to this question for the different countries considered, where we can observe the lower number of religious people in France and the United Kingdom, as well as the high religiosity of North Americans and Spaniards. The United Kingdom also stands out for the high number of people who declare themselves non-religious, at 48%, while the differentiating element in France is that a quarter of its population consider themselves convinced atheists, in line with what has been detected by some experts (among others, Cuchet 2018).
The evolutionary analysis since 2005/07 shows a large decline in religious people in the United States, with a decrease of 19 percentage points, a marked increase (13%) in people declared as non-religious, and even more for atheism, which triples in the period analyzed (Figure 5). This is a recent phenomenon that has generated research to find out who they are, where they come from, and to what extent it is a stable phenomenon or whether it will grow over time. Some researchers—Burge, for example—have defined them as nones, a phenomenon that according to the Cooperative Congressional Election Study of Harvard University affects 31.3% of Americans; that is, almost a third of Americans have no religious affiliation (Burge 2021).
The decline in self-declared religious people is notably lower in France and the United Kingdom (6 and 8 percentage points, respectively). As for the non-religious, we can observe stability in France and a slight increase in the United Kingdom (6 percentage points), as well as a significant increase (7%) in atheists in France and a small increase in the United Kingdom (2%). In other words, the decline in religiosity in France leads to a greater number of atheists, while in England, there is a greater preference for the “non-religious” response.
It is important to note that the high level of atheism of the French people did not occur in the period of 2005/07 and 2017, but already in 2005/07, 17% of respondents considered themselves convinced atheists. In fact, the increase between 2005/07 and 2017 is 7 percentage points, the same as in the United States.
Spain presents a very different behavior with a slight increase (5%) in religious people and, curiously, a decrease (12 points) in the number of respondents who declare themselves non-religious. The consultation of other editions of value surveys reveals stability in the number of religious people between 1980 and 1990, and a significant decline in the last decade of the twentieth century and the first decade of 2021 (Urrutia León 2010). In fact, Figure 4 shows a level of atheism slightly higher than the United Kingdom and 4 percentage points higher than the United States, which configures Spain as the country with the second highest number of atheists in the world.
To find out how different sociodemographic variables influence the variable under study, we used a logistic regression that uses dichotomized religious denomination as the dependent term, that is, grouping non-religious and atheists into a single value, so that the variable compares “people who declare themselves to be religious” with the rest. The use of this technique will make it possible to determine the incidence of sociodemographic variables on the self-definition of oneself as a religious person. The independent variables considered are sex, age (15–30, 31–49, 50–64, and 65 and more years), level of studies (lower, middle, and upper), relationship with activity (working, unemployed, retired, student, and housewife), whether or not the respondent is part of the working population, and whether or not he/she is an immigrant7. These terms have been coded considering the last category as a reference, and their coefficients are interpreted as the effect on the dependent variable of a one-unit change in the independent variable, keeping all other variables constant.
In order to select only the variables with an influence on religious self-identification, and thus achieve a parsimonious model, a forward stepwise regression process was used to test the entry of variables based on the log-likelihood test. The advantage of regression, as opposed to the bivariate analyses used in the past, is that it analyzes the influence of each variable on the dependent term while controlling for the influence of the other variables.
The resulting model, shown in Table 4, reveals the small number of significant terms, as well as the low explanatory power of the model, which shows the low influence of the sociodemographic variables on self-identification as a religious person (dependent term), with Pseudo R values of approximately 0.11 in France, increasing to 1.5 in the United Kingdom and reaching almost 2 in Spain. The United States is the country with the worst fit to the model. Table 4 shows the significant coefficients together with their significance levels of 0.10 (*), 0.05 (**), and 0.01 (***). The coefficients of the relationship with activity are not presented because of their low contribution to self-identification as a religious person.
The analysis of the coefficients of the French respondents begins by considering that practically all the comparisons with the reference category are significant, with the exception of the working population and respondents with medium education. Native–immigrant origin is the variable with the highest coefficients, indicating that the probability (odds) of being considered religious with value 1 (born outside France) is 2.868 times higher than those born in France. As for age, it is the variable with the second highest coefficients; note the large negative values of the youngest respondents, coefficients that decrease as the age of the respondent increases, indicating that the older the person is, the greater the self-identification as a religious person.
With respect to the level of education, the logic of considering oneself a religious person is 1.825 times greater among respondents with low education than among those with university education, with the coefficient of medium education having less influence, indicating a lower self-definition as a religious person as the level of education increases. Being a woman increases the probability of self-defining oneself as a religious person by 1.46 times.
The situation in the United Kingdom is similar, except for the disappearance of the influence of educational level, and also differs in the magnitude of the coefficients—much higher than in France—as well as in the incorporation of a new variable, the fact of being part—or not—of the working population. Consideration as a religious person is 1.45 times higher among those who do not form part of the active population than among those who are active.
In Spain, the trend is similar, with the highest coefficients in the case of age, somewhat lower than in France in terms of the influence of educational level, and between the two in the case of place of birth. Sex is the second most influential variable, after being born or not in the country of residence, with women denominating themselves as religious twice as often (2.003) as men. The specificity in the United States is the null influence of educational level and origin, with age and sex behaving as in previous cases.
In short, nativeimmigrant origin is the most influential variable in self-definition as a religious person, except in the United States. The United Kingdom and Spain present the highest coefficients, which implies a greater influence. A high relationship with age can be observed—of greater magnitude in Spain—which implies greater religiosity as age increases. We do not consider this to be strictly an effect of age, but rather of the different socialization processes encountered by those born in the 1960s, 1970s, or 1980s. In fact, so-called millennials are characterized by less religious sentiment.
The level of education is the third most influential variable, presenting an inverse relationship insofar as people with less education are those who have higher levels of religiosity, present only in those interviewed in France and Spain. As for the specific characteristics of each country, in addition to the change in the coefficients, it is worth noting the greater religiosity of the inactive population in the United Kingdom and the disappearance (in this country) of the level of education to explain self-identification as a religious person. Furthermore, in the United States, only sex and age have a significant influence, with a minor level of influence of origin (native/immigrant) and level of education.
The high influence of being a native or an immigrant makes it necessary to determine if there are differences among the immigrant group, or if those of first or second generations call themselves equally religious persons; that is, if socialization in a different—and more secularized—society than the one in which the family originated shows differences in the definition of being religious or not. The data in Figure 6 confirm this hypothesis in European countries, with Cramer’s V association coefficients (between religious denomination and immigrant generation) of 0.237 in Spain (significance of 0.02), 0.212 in France (significance of 0.03), 0.148 in the United Kingdom (significance of 0.064), and no significant relationship in the case of the United States (although it is included in the figure to contextualize the differences in the four countries).
Analysis of Figure 6 shows that approximately 7 out of 10 people in the first generation consider themselves to be religious people, with 65% in the UK, a percentage that drops significantly in the second generation. There are 30 percentage points of difference in the case of Spain, 20 in France, and 14 in the United Kingdom. Thus, there is a notable change in second generations, at a time when religious wars star in the news in Western countries.
Religious denomination gives way to religious affiliation, a question asked of all respondents, regardless of the answer given in the previous question. However, it should be noted that one of the response options is ‘do not belong to a denomination’, chosen by 50% of the respondents, no doubt as a kind of “escape route” for atheists or for those who declared themselves non-religious. This average figure for half of the sample increases by nine percentage points in the French and seven in the UK when compared to the 2005/07 survey. Spaniards and North Americans show a greater upward trend (Figure 7).
In any case, it is surprising that “only” 90% of atheists and 69% of those who declare themselves to be non-religious people choose this option. That is, it is striking that 10% of atheists and 31% of those who consider themselves non-religious persons answer the question about their religion.
In the responses shown in Figure 8, Catholicism appears as the dominant religion in three of the four countries, albeit with a very differentiated following. In France, 80% of those interviewed declared themselves Catholics, and the rest of the religions—with the exception of Muslims with 11% of those interviewed—are barely followed by 2% of the population9 (6). In Spain, the level of Catholicism drops to 62%, with 3% of Muslims and a large number of religions grouped in the “other” category. The rest of the religions, with the exception of Orthodoxy (1.2%), do not reach 1%.
In the United States, the number of Catholics was again 44% of the population, barely above Protestants (38%). However, the distinguishing feature of this country is the large number of minority religions, such as other Christians (7.2%), Jews (3.5%), Buddhists (2.1%), Hindus (1.1%), Muslims (1.1%), Orthodox (1.1%), and others (2.4%). In the United Kingdom, something similar happens, undoubtedly because the predominant religion (Protestantism) is shared by less than half of the population (45.6%). This allows the proliferation of several religions, with other Christians (21%) and Catholics (20%) standing out. Other religions with a prominent presence in this country are Muslims (7.5%), Hindus (3.1%), Jews (1.0%), Buddhists (0.6%), and others (1.6%).
Compared to the wave of 2005/07, Figure 9 shows decreases in the following of major religions, with Spain being the country with the highest rate of decrease (5.4%, compared to 0.4% in the United States and 0.3% in France and England). Strikingly, as shown in Figure 9, there is a large decline of Catholics in Spain, with a decline of 37 percentage points, followed by the decline of Protestants in the USA, with a decline of 20.4 percentage points. The decline of Protestants in the United Kingdom is much smaller (8.6%), followed by the diverse grouping of religions with fewer followers such as Hindus, Jews, and Buddhists.
In contrast to this generalized loss of followers of the major religions (Douthat 2022), increases in other Christians (Evangelical/Pentecostal/Free church/etc.) are also seen in the UK. In the USA, there are two trends: On the one hand, an increase in Buddhism, Judaism, Hinduism, and Orthodoxy and, in turn, a significant increase in Catholicism. However, the low sample sizes of those declared as Buddhists, Jews, Hindus, and Orthodox (13, 22, 7, and 7 cases, respectively, in 2017, and less in the 2005/07-07 measurement) recommends being cautious in generalizing this situation.
A few paragraphs back, it was noted that belonging to religions was a question answered by all respondents, and that approximately half of the sample had indicated ‘do not belong to a denomination’. However, this is a much lower percentage than that observed in the question that began this section (Figure 4), where 41–40% of French and English respondents said they were religious people, a figure that reaches half of the population in the case of Spaniards, and 55% in the case of Americans.
This implies that, at least in the case of France and the United Kingdom, the declaration of belonging to a religion may be higher than the reality. In other words, it is a process of “religious disaffection” insofar as the respondent approached it for various reasons, but does not currently follow or, at least, does not consider himself sufficiently religious to follow the precepts of the religion in which he started. This is the phenomena analyzed by Grace Davie in Great Britain: Believing without belonging (1994). In interpreting this fact, it should always be borne in mind that 24% of the French consider themselves atheists, and that half of the residents of the United Kingdom do not consider themselves religious people (Figure 4).
These reasons lead to the construction of a new figure with the (self-considered) religious people who follow each of the faiths (Figure 10). It should be noted that the percentages do not add up to 100; that is, 72% of the French in Roman Catholic indicate that 28% (100 − 72) of those who have declared themselves to be Catholic do not consider themselves to be religious persons or are convinced atheists. In other words, the space in the figure that is “missing” up to the top is the space occupied by atheists and those who do not consider themselves religious people.
Proceeding with the interpretation, the analysis of religions in each country clearly shows the “high bar” of Roman Catholics in all countries, which indicates that approximately 28% of respondents were initiated by their parents in this creed, but do not currently follow its precepts. The United Kingdom and USA show higher percentages than Mediterranean countries. Protestantism presents a slightly better situation (fewer cases of non-religious) in the case of France and—primarily—in the USA; the situation worsens in the United Kingdom (a percentage similar to Roman Catholicism) and even more in Spain: 60% (100 − 40) of the declared Protestants are not religious people.
Muslims present the highest bars in the figure, which implies a low number of “non-religious” people among their ranks, at approximately 10% on average. This figure disappears in the case of Spain and increases notably among North Americans, where slightly less than half (47%) are non-religious.
In summary, approximately one in four respondents of the most widespread religions do not follow their precepts, a percentage that ranges from 28% in the case of declared Roman Catholics to 22% among Protestants.
As in the case of religious denomination, a sociodemographic profile will be made of the followers of the main religions in each country, i.e., Roman Catholics in the case of France, Spain, and the United States and Protestants in the United Kingdom. Protestants in the United States will also be considered because of their large presence, slightly less than the number of Catholics. It should be noted that only respondents who declare themselves to be religious persons have been considered, excluding self-declared non-religious persons and atheists (Figure 4 shows the percentages of each). The results are presented in Table 5, limited to presenting the odds ratios, which are the exponents of the coefficients11.
The analysis of the information in Table 5 shows that of the six variables considered, only two show an influence on the two religions, to which is added origin in the United States. With regard to the Catholic religion, it is clear that women and those over 64 years of age consider themselves more religious than men and young people. This is the case in all three countries considered, although the figure for sex is lower in the case of the United States and the influence of age is notably higher than in the two European countries. These values are almost double that of France, and the latter is double that of Spain, which indicates that in the latter country, the influence of age is much lower than in France and the USA. In any case, native/immigrant origin is the most important variable in the case of the United States, with immigrants having a greater affiliation to the Catholic religion.
With regard to Protestantism, the situation in the United Kingdom is very similar to that presented in Spain with the Catholic religion, with a greater belonging of women and those over 64 years of age, the latter with not very high coefficients. In the United States, the influence of sex disappears and two new variables emerge: Native/immigrant origin and educational level. Age is the variable with the highest coefficients; as it increases, Protestantism increases, in line with the educational level. With respect to origin, being born in the United States implies a greater adherence to the Protestant religion, in line with the findings of other experts (Glascock 2023).
The present section, dedicated to the self-description of the level of religiosity, reveals that 47% of the residents in the countries under study consider themselves religious, 39% as non-religious, and 15% as convinced atheists, figures that change considerably when we take into account the specific characteristics of each country: Greater religiosity in the United States and Spain, expressed by more than half of the population, a high number of non-religious people in the United Kingdom (48%), and 24% of the French population declaring themselves convinced atheists. The temporal evolution of the phenomenon reveals a slight increase in the number of religious people in the case of Spain (6%), which is an exception to the large decline in the United States (the most religious country), and a milder decline in the other two countries considered. The fall in religiosity in France leads to a greater number of atheists, with the “non-religious” response increasing among respondents in the United Kingdom. The most important sociodemographic variables in the self-definition as a religious person is the fact of having been born–or not–in the country where one resides.
Next, we proceeded to analyze religious affiliation, after finding that 50% of those interviewed do not consider themselves to be linked to any religion. Catholicism appears as the main religion in three of the four countries, a figure that rises to 62% among Spaniards and 80% in the case of the French. In the United Kingdom, Protestantism is shared by less than half of the population (45.6%), and in the United States, Catholicism, the predominant religion, is 6% higher than Protestantism. Moreover, 11% of the French are Muslims, a figure that drops to 8% in the United Kingdom and 3% in Spain. An evolutionary analysis over the last ten years reveals a decline in the major religions, primarily Catholicism in Spain. The study of sociodemographic traits that influence whether one is a member of the Catholic or Protestant religion—in each country where it is the religion with the most followers—reveals the influence of the age and sex of the respondent, as well as the native/immigrant origin in the United States.

3.3. The Sphere of Beliefs

This section is articulated around four dimensions: Belief in God, life after death, hell, and heaven. Of those interviewed, 63% said they believed in God and 3.2% did not know what to answer; 51% believed that there is life after death and 7% did not know; 35% said they believed that hell exists and 5% did not answer; and 44% believed that heaven exists with the same number of non-responses.
The country-by-country analysis shown in Figure 11 reveals, in the first place, the high levels of belief in these aspects in North American society, where eight out of ten believe in God, ten percentage points less in that there is life after death, and two out of three respondents believe that heaven and hell exist. Spain is the second country in terms of the level of belief, with seven out of ten believing in God, and slightly less than half of those interviewed (44%) believing that there is life after death. Belief in the existence of heaven and hell are lower, at 39% and 29%, respectively. In France and the United Kingdom, belief in God is reduced to half of the population, although the latter has a slightly lower belief in God and in the existence of life after death.
Let us analyze each dimension.

3.3.1. Belief in God

The survey conducted in 2017 specifically asks about the importance of God in the life of the respondents, posing to the respondents a graphical aid with a scale in which the far left shows 0 and a label stating ‘not important at all’, and on the far right appears 10 and a label with ‘very important’ (see Appendix C). We have chosen to consider the answers to this question only for those who believe in God, since it does not seem appropriate to ask about the importance of God to those who state that they do not believe in him, since their importance in life will be very low or null. Although the analysis can be carried out considering the average scores of each country (France with 6.74, the UK with 6.94, Spain with 7.07, and the USA with 8.03, always considering this scale from 0 to 10), it seems more interesting to carry out a categorization of the evaluations by joining answers 0, 1, 2, and 3 and considering them as ‘not at all important’ and answers 7, 8, 9, and 10, which define God as ‘very important’. In the middle are answers 4, 5, and 6 expressed by those who consider God in their life as ‘neither very important nor unimportant’. Thirty-eight percent of those interviewed stated that God is very important in their lives, slightly higher than those who stated that He is not (40%). The country-by-country analysis will nuance these results (Figure 12).
Three out of every four Americans (who believe in God) recognize that God is very important in their lives, a fact related to the high percentages of religious people (more than half of the population, as shown in Figure 4). In the case of Spain, this percentage drops eleven percentage points, seventeen in the case of United Kingdom, and twenty-one among French citizens, who are also the residents who express the highest intermediate scores (37%). It is surprising that approximately 10% of those who believe in God consider him to be of no importance in their lives, a percentage that increases in France and the United Kingdom and decreases in Spain.
The non-existence of these questions on beliefs in the 2005/07 measurement makes it necessary to change the order of presentation with respect to the previous headings, proceeding directly to the analysis of the sociodemographic characteristics of the respondents who believe in God, which is shown in Table 6.
As for independent terms with a significant influence on belief in God, sex, age, educational level, and the fact of not having been born in the country appear, with the latter showing the highest coefficients, and very high in the case of the United Kingdom. There are three exceptions to this general trend: No influence of educational level in the United Kingdom and being born abroad in the USA, as well as the influence of being part of the working population in the case of Spain. The interpretation of the coefficients reveals a greater belief in God in those who were born abroad (with a high influence in the United Kingdom and none in the USA), in those over 64 years of age (France presents the highest magnitude), in respondents with low levels of education (with high magnitudes in Spain and the USA), and in women, much higher in the case of the USA.
The great influence of the fact of being a native or immigrant makes it advisable to look at whether there are differences between the immigrant group, or whether first- or second-generation immigrants believe in God in the same way. Figure 13 provides an answer to this concern, always showing a greater belief in God among first-generation immigrants, with differences of 40 percentage points in the case of those interviewed in Spain, almost twenty (18%) in the French, and eleven in the case of residents of the United Kingdom. There is no significant difference in the case of residents of the United States, which would recommend eliminating this country from Figure 13, although we have opted to leave it in order to contextualize the differences in the four geographic areas under study.
Once the influence of the sociodemographic variables on belief in God has been exposed, it seems relevant to know—because of its importance—the relationship of this belief with the religious denomination and the fact of belonging or not to a religion. The information presented in Figure 14 shows the great importance of both variables. The percentage of believers in God, which, as shown in Figure 11, in France and the United Kingdom was approximately 50% (53% in France and 50% in the UK) rises to 72% among those who call themselves religious, an increase much higher than in Spain, which barely increased by 4%. The trend in the USA is disconcerting, where belief in God decreases among those who consider themselves religious and increases among those who call themselves non-religious.
It is more interesting to focus attention on the right-hand side of the Figure 14, which reveals that approximately 29% of the non-religious believe in God, a percentage that drops to 27% in the case of Spaniards.
In the lower part of Figure 14, belief in God is analyzed with the response ‘do not belong to a denomination’ in the question regarding the religion to which they belong. The situation in the UK and the USA is surprising, with a high belief in God among those who declare that they do not belong to any religion: In the UK, one-third of those who do not belong to any religion believe in God, far exceeding the average of non-believers who believe in God (28%). The trend is higher in the USA, where belief in God in this group rises to 37%.
Spain presents the opposite situation, where the percentage of believers in God who do not belong to a religion is reduced to half the average (13%), thus generating an equivalence between belonging to a religious denomination and believing in God.
It is important to note that when both concepts are included as independent terms in the regression shown in Table 6, the explanatory capacity of the model increases notably: R2 values of 0.60, 0.58, 0.7, and 0.5 in France, the United Kingdom, Spain, and the USA, respectively, with approximately 85% of cases correctly classified (reaching 90% in Spain). Another important finding is that the influence of practically all the sociodemographic variables disappears, with the exception of educational level in Spain and the USA, and sex in the latter, with religious self-identification being the variable that most explains belief in God, followed by belonging or not to a religion and, at a great distance, the fact of being born in another country. Those who call themselves religious people, were born in a different country, and belong to a religion believe in God more than the rest of the interviewees (in order to make the reading more fluid, this table has been placed as a table in Appendix D).

3.3.2. Belief in Life after Death

Regarding the belief of whether there is life after death, something shared by 51% of the interviewees, in Figure 11 we can observe the strong belief of the residents of the United States, as opposed to the skepticism of Europeans. Sixty-nine percent of those interviewed in North America believe in this idea, a number that drops to 42% among residents of the United Kingdom. The Spanish and French are close behind, 44% and 45%, respectively.
As was stated in the case of belief in God, given the impossibility of knowing the evolution of this belief in recent years (since this question was not asked in the fifth wave of the survey), we will proceed to present the sociodemographic characteristics of the respondents who believe that there is life after death, the results of which are presented in Table 7.
The analysis of these traits is in line with those shown in Table 7 regarding the low explanatory power of the models. However, the variables of influence are notably different, with a loss of influence of educational level in all countries, age in all countries except France, and the fact of belonging or not to the working population, which only shows influence in the case of Spain. In North America, only the sex of the respondent had a significant influence. In the three European countries, the importance of the immigrant/non-immigrant origin of the interviewees can be appreciated, with non-natives being those who show a greater belief in the existence of life after death, an idea also shared by women in the four countries. In France, interestingly, it is the youngest respondents who believe more that there is life after death.
Unlike what happened previously (Figure 6 and Figure 13), in this case, there is no relationship between the type of migrant—in terms of generation—and the belief in life after death.
Following the same expository logic used for belief in God, we now proceed with the analysis of the influence of religious denomination and belonging/not belonging to a religion. The first part of Figure 15 reveals a notable increase in the belief in life after death among religious Spaniards, with Americans at the average. Two out of every three French and English people who consider themselves religious believe that there is life after death, a percentage that affects three out of every four Spaniards. It is the residents of Spain who experience the greatest growth, 31 percentage points higher than the marginal percentage, as opposed to the stability of the Americans, who do not change when religious denomination is considered.
It is also surprising that 35% of English people who do not consider themselves religious believe that there is life after death, an opinion also shared by 29% of non-religious Americans. Another interesting aspect is that 11% of French atheists believe that there is life after death, which is in stark contrast to the statement of atheists. When aggregated, 37% of non-religious people in France believe that there is life after death, a percentage similar to that of the United Kingdom.
The second part of the figure, relating to religious affiliation, shows similar results in the French and Americans, increasing in UK society and decreasing notably in the Spanish. Forty-one percent of the English who declare that they do not belong to a religion believe that there is life after death, a percentage similar to that of the English and Americans. The large drop in Spanish society is surprising, 20 percentage points lower than the marginal percentage shown in Figure 11.
As happened in the case of belief in God, when these variables are introduced into the regression model as independent terms, the explanatory power of the model increases notably13, completely altering the significances shown in Table 8. In all four countries, religious denomination is the most influential variable in belief in life after death, followed by age and sex in all countries except Spain. Being a native or an immigrant and not belonging to any religion are the next most influential aspects. In Spain and England, the influence of being a native or immigrant disappears.
Those who most believe that there is life after death are self-described religious people, those under 30 years of age (belief in this aspect decreases as one grows older), women, those born outside the country where they live, and those who indicate that they do not belong to any religion (not shown).

3.3.3. Belief in Hell

Belief in the existence of hell is the third aspect considered within the sphere of beliefs. Moreover, 35% of those interviewed believe in its existence, an opinion shared by 66% of U.S. residents. Follow-up to this belief is scarce in European countries, primarily in France and the United Kingdom, where 23% of those surveyed believe in the existence of hell (Figure 11). This percentage increases slightly in the case of Spaniards, who reach 29%.
The analysis of sociodemographic traits by means of logistic regression does not present major differences from that found in Table 6 regarding belief in God. To the great importance of native–immigrant origin in European countries, not in the United States, we add the influence of the sex of the respondent (women believe in hell more than men), age in France, and the fact of not belonging to the working population in Spain. As was the case for life after death (Table 4), there is a linear relationship between age and belief in hell, where young people are the highest believers, and the same is true for those who are not active.
Again, there is a difference between first- and second-generation immigrants with respect to belief in hell in European countries, but not in the United States, where Spain presents the greatest differences: More than 53% of first-generation immigrants believe in hell, a figure that plummets to half among second-generation immigrants (Figure 16). The differences between the two groups are smaller in France and the United Kingdom, with 13 and 9 percentage points, respectively.
Religious denomination, as in the rest of the beliefs, implies large changes in terms of belief in hell, which is in the majority among those who consider themselves religious people. Three out of four religious people believe in the existence of hell, a percentage that reaches 88% in the case of Spain (Figure 17). In other words, the percentages for European countries triple when we look specifically at people who declare themselves to be very religious. On the other hand, 27% of North Americans who do not consider themselves religious people believe in the existence of hell, five points higher than those in France and the United Kingdom.
The percentages of belief in the existence of hell increase notably when one considers those who do not belong to a religion. Thirty percent of French and English residents who believe in hell without belonging to a religion rises to 36% of U.S. residents.
Introducing these variables in the regression model shown in Table 8 again produces significant increases in the explanatory power of the models (0.34 in France, 0.30 in the UK, 0.39 in Spain, and 0.382 in the USA), and important changes in the independent variables. The native/immigrant origin of the respondent loses importance, and the variable with the greatest explanatory power is religious denomination. In France and England, these are accompanied by age and the fact of not belonging to any religion, while in Spain, educational level is incorporated, and in the United States, it is educational level, age, and sex.
As with the belief in life after death, those who most believe that hell exists are self-described religious people (6.5 times more than the non-religious in France), those under 30 years of age (belief in this aspect decreases as one gets older), those born outside the country where they life, and those who indicate that they do not belong to any religion.

3.3.4. Belief in Heaven

The analysis in Figure 11 already revealed that the 44% who say they believe that heaven exists increases notably in the United States, where 65% believe in its existence, significantly surpassing European countries, whose percentage of believers ranges from 39% of Spaniards to 36% of the English and French. In any case, belief in the existence of heaven exceeds belief in hell in European countries, with a difference of thirteen points in the case of the French and English and ten points among Spaniards. There is hardly any difference in the case of North Americans, and the percentages of belief in heaven are very similar to belief in hell (65% and 66%, respectively).
The analysis of socio-demographic characteristics, which we will not present here so as not to overwhelm the reader with so much data, shows an influence of the sex of the respondent in the four countries, revealing that women believe in heaven to a greater extent than men. Age influences Spaniards and Americans, with belief in heaven increasing as the age of the interviewees increases. Educational level has an influence in all four countries, with respondents with low education showing the highest level of belief in the existence of heaven, with native–immigrant origin being the most influential variable in European countries, but not in U.S. respondents.
We can observe a significant relationship when we look specifically at first- and second-generation immigrants in France. Moreover, 67% of first-generation immigrants believe in the existence of heaven, a percentage that drops to 49% among children of French immigrants.
Figure 18 shows the great impact of religious denomination on belief in the existence of heaven, which reaches 87% of the Spanish population, remaining at almost three-quarters of the population in the rest of the countries, percentages similar to the belief in the existence of hell presented in Figure 17. In any case, more than 25% of the non-religious respondents believe in the existence of heaven, a percentage that reaches 30% in the case of residents of the United Kingdom.
As on previous occasions, the results at the bottom of Figure 18 are more surprising since 35% of U.S. residents who do not belong to a religion believe in the existence of heaven, slightly lower percentages than residents in the United Kingdom (34%) and France (32%). We do not refer to the changes in the regression model when the variables in Figure 18 are introduced because of the coincidence of the situation shown by the belief in hell.
To summarize, of the four beliefs considered, belief in God is the one with the most followers, at approximately 63% of the population, reaching 79% and 70% in the United States and Spain, respectively. Half of those interviewed believe that there is life after death, a figure that reaches 69% of North Americans, and approximately 44% in European countries. Forty-four percent say they believe in heaven, a figure that rises to 65% in the case of U.S. residents and drops to 36-339% in European countries. The existence of hell is the least reported, by approximately one-third of the population, dropping to 23% for residents of France and the United Kingdom.
Gender, age, educational level, and not being born in the country are the independent terms with significant influence on belief in God. The influential variables in the belief in life after death show some differences, with a loss of influence of educational level in all countries, age in all countries except France, and the fact of belonging or not to the working population, which only shows influence in the case of Spain. In North America, only the sex of the respondent had a significant influence.
As for the explanatory traits in believing in hell, to the great importance of native–immigrant origin in European countries, but not in the United States, we add the influence of the sex of the respondent, age in France, and the fact of belonging to the labor force in Spain. Finally, the analysis of the sociodemographic characteristics of those who believe in heaven shows an influence of the sex of the respondent in the four countries, age in Spain and the United States, educational level in the four countries, and native–immigrant origin in the European countries. In any case, the variables with the greatest influence on the four beliefs are self-identification as a religious person and belonging or not to a religion.

3.4. Scope of Practices

It should be recalled that the organization of this article follows the classification scheme known as “the three B’s”, which allude to (religious) belongings, (religious) belief, and (religious) behavior; that is, belonging to a religious denomination, belief in shared dogmas, and common behaviors. Having dealt with the first two, it is time for the third, which specifically focuses on the habitual frequency with which religious services are attended, that is, excluding “special” situations such as weddings and funerals, as well as the frequency with which prayers are said.
This question is posed to the respondent showing a figure aid with the possible answers, which are ‘Several times a day’, ‘More than once a week’, ‘Once a week’, ‘Once a month’, ‘Only on special holy days’, ‘Other special holy days’, ‘Once a year’, ‘Less often’, and ‘Never, practically never’. Due to the low number of responses to the categories ‘Other special holy days’ and ‘Once a year’, they have been grouped with their contiguous category, thus forming two categories called ‘Only on holy days’—the result of adding ‘Only on special holy days’ and ‘Other special holy days’—and ‘Once a year & Less often’. This situation, coupled with the fact that no one answered ‘Several times a day’, leads to reducing the question from nine categories to six, which makes it more manageable.
Half of the respondents never attend religious services, and 16% attend once a year or less. The responses shown in Figure 19 qualify these responses by country, indicating that two-thirds of French respondents never attend religious services, a percentage that is reduced by 29 percentage points for U.S. residents. Almost one in five (exactly 18%) UK residents attend once a year and less frequently, and Spaniards stand out as having the highest rate of church attendance on “holy days” (Christmas, Easter, etc.).
Also noteworthy is the weekly frequency, reported by 15% Spaniards and 19% of North Americans, tripling the frequency of residents of the United Kingdom (7%) and even more so of the French (5%).
This low attendance at religious events is not new, as those who never attend increased just 5 percentage points between 2005/07 and 2017, amounting to 11% in the UK (and 8% in US residents). Figure 20 shows the decrease in weekly attendance and more than one day a week in Americans, with six and four percentage points, as well as the decrease on public holidays (holy days), primarily in French respondents (6%).
It could be the case that religious practice was different according to religious denomination, something that is not ascertained by performing the consequent relationship between variables (not shown). For profiling, in line with what is shown in Table 4, Table 5, Table 6, Table 7 and Table 8, we used a logistic regression to study the traits associated with low participation in religious services, which provides a very low adjustment, but also a consistency between countries. Non-attendance at religious services is higher among natives, those with high levels of education, and those in the labor force, although Spain also adds age, which indicates that young people have the lowest attendance at religious services.
We do not refer to the changes in the regression model when the variables in Figure 18 are introduced because of the coincidence of the situation shown by the belief in hell. Within the group of those not born in the country of residence, there is a large difference between first- and second-generation immigrants, with the latter attending fewer religious services, as shown in Figure 21. In Europe, more than half of second-generation immigrants do not attend religious services, reaching 61% in Spain. Respondents from the United States again show no difference in this regard.
As in the previous section, we analyze the relationship between non-attendance at religious services (never and practically never) with religious denomination and whether or not one belongs to a religion (Figure 22). Surprisingly, we can observe low attendance at religious services among those who consider themselves religious at 24%, which rises notably to 31% among respondents in the United Kingdom and 36% among residents of France, dropping to 15% in the middle among North American respondents.
As for those who say they belong to a religion, at least one in four Europeans hardly attends religious services at all, a figure that rises to 38% in the case of France. North American respondents, as usual, are the ones who attend the most, and only 15% of those who declare a religious denomination do not attend services. The percentages in the second part of Figure 20 are surprising, but it should be borne in mind that within the category of “belonging to a religion”, there are people who—as we have already seen—have abandoned their precepts.
Within the group of those not born in the country of residence, there is a great difference between first- and second-generation immigrants, with the latter attending fewer religious services, as shown in Figure 23. Respondents from the United States again show no difference in this regard.
Within the group of those not born in the country of residence, there is a large difference between first- and second-generation immigrants, with the latter attending fewer religious services, as shown in Figure 23. Respondents from the United States again show no difference in this regard.
The second element included in the practices is the frequency of prayer, which reveals that 48% never do it, 12% less than ‘several times a year’, and 16% every day, representing the highest results. Figure 24 presents the differences in the frequency of prayer among European countries since this question was not included in the North American survey and therefore it is not possible to know its distribution. This figure shows the increase in the number of people who never pray in France, which reaches 57% of those interviewed, as well as in the United Kingdom, where the percentage of people who never pray is just over half of the population. In the analysis of the columns on the left, where those who pray every day appear, it is again the French who pray the least, followed by the residents of the United Kingdom. Approximately 20% of Spaniards pray every day.
It is not possible to compare these results with the 2005/07 wave because, at that time, it was presented in a dichotomous form, whether or not they pray (59% and 41%, respectively) and, moreover, it was only asked in Spain and the United States.
The results of those who pray the least are shown in Figure 24, which reveals that approximately 20–25% of religious people hardly pray at all, that is, they pray several times a year, less often, and never. The percentages in the second part of Figure 25 are surprising, although it should be kept in mind that within the category of “belonging (to a religion)” there are people who, as we have already seen, have abandoned their precepts.
In summary, more than half of the sample consulted never attend religious services, percentages that, in France and the United Kingdom, reach 64% and 59%, respectively. Figure 21 shows that this is not a new situation insofar as it has only increased by five percentage points in the ten years considered. Grace Davie defined this phenomenon as “believing without belonging” in 1994, in her case, referring to the United Kingdom, but it seems that could be extensible to the four countries considered in present day. In fact, Grace Davie herself warned in 2015 of the persistence of this logic of religious action in the United Kingdom. The study of the profiles associated with low participation in religious offices is related to living or not in the country of birth and educational level. If attendance at religious services is low, the habit of prayer is even scarcer in the European countries. Some 48% never do so, and barely 16% pray every day, with Spaniards being the ones who pray the most.

3.5. A Group in Progressive Numerical Ascent: The “Nones”

For the precise definition of this group, we use religious denomination, where 1838 of the 4761 respondents declare themselves non-religious persons and 698 are convinced atheists. The non-religious, as shown in Figure 4, account for 53% of those interviewed. There is another variable that provides information on this issue, as is the question on religious denominations, considering those who answered ‘do not belong to a denomination’. This option was chosen by 2391 respondents, or 49.8% of the sample.
An exhaustive analysis comparing the two questions (Figure 26) reveals that a large number of people who consider themselves non-religious or atheists do not indicate ‘do not belong to a denomination’, but rather choose a specific denomination (Catholicism, in most cases). It is possible that these non-religious persons (and atheists) indicated the religious denomination in which their parents and guardians initiated them, and which they have not followed.
The left side of Figure 25 also “explains” a situation alluded to in the previous section when we noted the large number of people who were called religious and did not participate in practices. Note how, on the left side of the figure, there are 20% of religious people who do not belong to any religious creed.
The above-mentioned three paragraphs are situations that complicate an adequate delimitation of the group of nones. For this reason, it seems more appropriate to define the “nones” with the question on religious denomination; defining as SOFT nones those who indicate that they are not religious and HARD those who respond that they are atheists. To the first group belong 1836 respondents, 39% of the sample, and to the second group belong 698 respondents, 15% of the sample. When aggregated, we are talking about a group that represents 54% of the population.
Figure 27 shows a high presence of nones in France and Spain, affecting two out of three respondents. In spite of the similarity in the number, the situation is very different on average: In France, 25% of those interviewed declare themselves atheists (hard nones), a situation that drops to 12 % in Spain. There is also a low presence—comparatively speaking—of nones in North American society, precisely the country where the important work The Nones: Where there Came from, Who They Are, and Where They Are Going appeared, providing account of this trend and even defining the term (Burge 2021).
The sociodemographic profile of these groups will be examined insofar as it can shed light on their progressive increase or decrease, without differentiating between soft and hard nones, considering them together. The information provided in Figure 28 shows a greater presence of men, people under 50 years of age, and those with a high level of education. Half of the “none” group is made up of people under 50 years of age, which will imply a possible future increase as they will educate their children in the values they now profess. It is important to note that there are more nones in the group between 31 and 49 years of age than in younger groups, so it will be necessary to proceed with caution with this idea of greater growth of the group in the future.
Other characteristic features (Exemplified in Figure 28) are that the majority are working and were born in the country where they live. These last factors affects not only those interviewees who are the children of residents of these countries but also the children of emigrants born in the countries considered, although to a lesser extent.
To summarize, in this section, we have analyzed the least religious group, those defined by Burge (2021) as nones, differentiating between SOFT nones, those who—in the question on religious self-description—answer that they are not religious, and HARD nones, those who answer that they are atheists. To the first group belong 1836 respondents, 39% of the sample, and to the second group belong 698 respondents, 15% of the sample. When aggregated, we are talking about a group that represents 54% of the population.

4. Discussion

Having presented the most important data relative to the in-depth survey carried out, our objective in this section is to analyze a series of avenues of theoretical and reflective analyses that open up from this survey and which allow us to engage in dialogue with some proposals made by prominent authors in the field of the sociology of religion and secularization that will provide us with a series of keys to understand the religious situation of these countries in a scenario of secularization.
We will present them briefly and then analyze each of them in detail in the following pages:
  • The crisis? of Christianity.
  • The thesis of European exceptionalism.
  • The tension between belief and religious practice.
  • The rise of the nones.

4.1. The Crisis? of Christianity

We commented in the introduction to this paper that the hegemonic thesis of secularization was based on a basic assertion: “The more modern a society, the more secular, and the more secular the less religious” (Casanova 2009, pp. 1056–57). We also pointed out at the beginning of this work that this assertion did not stand up to an in-depth examination of modern secular religiosity and, therefore, did not serve as a general tool for analyzing the religious situation in times of secularization. Now, if we apply this logic to the concrete situation of Christianity, would it be valid, in what sense and in what terms? Thus, assuming that it does not serve as a general slogan for analysis, it would not be unreasonable to ask whether there are concrete scenarios of religiosity in which something such as a religious crisis or a loss of weight and importance of religion in daily life is clearly perceived.
In order to continue advancing in this line of analysis, we must introduce into the debate the three sub-theses on which the hegemonic thesis of secularization is based. For this, we turn to the proposal made by José Casanova: “Strictu sensu, the core and central thesis of the theory of secularization is given by the conceptualization of a historical process of social modernization as a process of functional differentiation and emancipation of the secular spheres-in the first place, the State, the economy and science-from religion, as well as the concomitantly specialized differentiation of religion within its own, newly created, religious sphere. The other sub-theses, the decline and privatization of religion, were added as pre-tendentiously structural consequences of the secularization process.” (Casanova 2012, p. 23).
In this section, we will focus on the ‘decline’ sub-thesis, knowing that in Section 4.3, we will return to the privatization thesis and its impact on religious belief and practice. The theoretical analyses converge with the data analyzed in pointing out that the thesis of the decline of the religious is applicable in the context of historical religions (Bellah 1969), but, in particular, in the specific scenario of Christian-European religiosity. On the one hand, it must be said that not all historical religions are suffering a setback. For example, in European societies—and this is closely linked to the immigration variable and the creation of mechanisms of integration and adaptation to new environments through religion, as we pointed out in Section 3, an issue to which we will return in Section 4.3—there is growth in the Islamic religion or Protestant creeds such as evangelicalism.
On the other hand, this decline in Christianity is perceived, above all, among groups that have been socialized in a scenario of differentiation between secular and religious spheres. This implies that, in this specific case, secularization understood as functional differentiation has had a negative impact on religious affiliation. This leads us to point out from Guillaume Cuchet (2018) that a process of ‘de-Christianization’ has taken place. Cuchet identifies the Second Vatican Council as the turning point with regard to this process on French territory, as the moment when, above all, religious practice becomes a major crisis. It is interesting to see how what we have defined elsewhere as a milestone—the Second Vatican Council—in the process of aggiornamento of the Catholic Church to modernity (Gil-Gimeno 2017b), can also be articulated in terms of disaffection, in this case, with regard to the impact of Christian religious practice in France.
Although we have located the religious crisis of Christianity—fundamentally in its Catholic and Protestant varieties—in the European context, the truth is that, with its nuances, there are studies that point to the same thing happening in the American context. Jim Davies, Michael Graham, and the aforementioned Burge comment in The Great Dechurching: Who’s Leaving, Why Are They Going, and What Will It Take to Bring Them Back? (Davis et al. 2023) that we are facing a turning point in terms of American religiosity, one that is closely connected, as we will see in depth in Section 4.4. Jessica Grose elaborates on this idea in an article published in The New York Times: “There are far more Christians in America than there are people of any other faith background. But the book also has an aim that I don’t have: It argues for bringing dechurched Americans back to regular worship. (The three men who worked on the book are all pastors). The data they shared with me suggests that “dechurching” is particularly prevalent among Buddhists and Jews, with nearly half not attending worship services regularly, and around 30 percent of most Christian denominations and around 20 percent of Mormons and Orthodox Christians of Grose (2023, p. 1)”.

4.2. The European Exceptionalism Thesis

In Heinrich (2020), Joseph Heinrich undertakes a thorough study in which he analyzes how the modern Western perspective became hegemonic, that is, how it became the pattern or model from which other modern cultural programs were to be developed. Heinrich’s analysis is, undoubtedly, a critical analysis of the ‘blindness’ and ‘arrogance’ of the modern Western understanding of social interactions in this phase of human history. Behind the pretended hegemonic position of Western modernity over the rest of modern cultural programs are hidden ‘biases’ that directly affect the researcher’s gaze and, therefore, the quality of his interpretations regarding the impact of secularization on current reality and on the phenomenon of religion in general, and Christianity in particular.
The one that interests us at this point of the work is the one that is articulated through a dynamic of the pars pro toto type from which the particular (the situation of dechurching in the European case) is considered the general norm (the religious situation in the context of secularization). In this sense, in recent years, numerous authors have spoken of European exceptionalism (Casanova 2003, 2012; Gil-Gimeno 2020) with regard to the analysis of the religious situation if we approach it from a global perspective. In this sense, European weirdness (exceptionalism) has tended to be seen as the norm, as the pattern of interaction between the secular and the religious, when this has been shown not to be the case. Heinrich’s logic of rarity acts in this case as a bias that prevents us from becoming aware that the European religious situation is exceptional and rare if we compare it with other areas of the world. The data analyzed tell us that the religious decline of Christianity (although it exists as we have seen in the previous section) is much less in the USA than in the European countries analyzed. Similarly, if we were to study extra-Western realities, we would realize that, in other cultural programs of secularization, the impact of historical religions is also greater than in Europe.
Therefore, the thesis of European exceptionalism has to make us aware that, just as we can speak of ‘multiple modernities’ (Eisenstadt 2000), we can also speak of ‘multiple secularizations’ (Gil-Gimeno 2017a), that is, of multiple cultural programs linked to secularization, and that this multiplicity of programs implies a great diversity of forms of articulation of the religious and the secular, and also the need to pay attention to the specific contexts in which these formulas are developed.
In short, when analyzing the Western program of secularization (if we can speak of a homogeneous program as the data and previous reflections have made us aware), we have to take into account both the socio-cultural particularities of each context and the fact that what the hegemonic thesis of secularization understood as a paradigm has turned out to be an exception or a rarity (Heinrich 2020). Thus, the data analyzed in terms of de-Christianization must make us aware that this is something that occurs fundamentally in the European context, and that is beginning to occur, with nuances or socio-cultural particularities, in the American context.

4.3. The Tension between Belief and Religious Practice

For Durkheim, religion is a systematized set of beliefs and practices related to the sacred (1995). In fact, in its historical development, Christianity (fundamentally Catholicism, but also Protestantism) managed to establish a great synchrony between the corpus of beliefs and the corpus of practices. The Church—in the case of Catholicism—or the sect—in the case of the first generation of Protestant denominations—acted as generating instruments of cohesion and social control-oriented ad majorem Dei gloriam. The principle Extra ecclesiam nulla salus sums up this idea perfectly.
The arrival of modernity and the development of secularization processes undoubtedly broke this harmony and balance. The data analyzed above tell us that, in the tension between belief and practice, the most damaged element is the latter, especially with regard to its communitarian-collective dimension. The data seem to support the thesis articulated by Davie in the 1990s (1994) in Great Britain, which he has brought back to the table in the second decade of the 21st century (2015): The religious situation in the societies analyzed responds to the formula of believing without belonging. That is, in the scenario of decline of religious-Christian belief and practice (Casanova 2012) or de-Christianization (Cuchet 2018), practice—especially collective practice—is more undermined than belief. With regard to the latter, it should be noted that the belief that remains with a certain level of representativeness or social impact is, fundamentally, belief in God since the other forms of belief analyzed—in life after death, in hell, and in paradise—also experience a clear decline or do not achieve the same degree of impact as the former.
Davie’s thesis has shared space with another that has had less impact in the field of the sociology of religion, which considers that what is in decline is belief and not so much practice. This thesis was defended by Hervieu-Léger and Champion (1986). For them, religion acted as a kind of integrating institution at a time when the collective bond was compromised by the impact of individualization processes, that is, as a form of response to the undesired consequences of the process of emancipation of the individual with respect to early modern institutions. Although the data do not support the thesis that places the belonging component above all else, it is true that this logic of community action works to a great extent if we cross the variable ‘religious practice’ with the variable ‘in-gration’. Religious creeds—not exclusively, but in a significant way—for example, the Islamic communities in France with immigrants from the Maghreb or the evangelical community among South American immigrants in Spain, have acted—especially among first-generation immigrants—as fundamental elements of inclusion in the processes of arrival and reception of these people; they have functioned as mechanisms that offered roots and links in a scenario of lack of movement or anomie caused by the migratory processes. In fact, there are studies (Pérez-Agote 2015) that tell us that parts of these groups recognize that, in their countries of origin, they did not develop very religious behaviors, and that these have been reinforced by the support of the religious community in their processes of arrival in the country of destination, and by the fact of introducing an element of familiarity—something known, close—in a different scenario. At times, these religious communities have acted as mediators for integration, and at others, as creators of enclave cultures within the host societies. This action of religious communities, as we can see, has an impact on the effective integration of communities and is at the basis of some current cultural tensions such as those linked to the recognition of the rights and freedoms of religious or cultural minorities. In short, the thesis of Hervieu-Léger and Champion (1986) is sustained when we introduce the immigration variable when explaining the relationship between religious belief and practice.

4.4. The Rise of the Nones

The last element on which we will dwell in this discussion is the growth of the collective known as ‘the nones’. The decline of religious belief and practice is associated with the emergence, or rather, the acquisition of strength of a social category without religious affiliation. As Burge points out, in the case of the USA: “The percentage of religiously unaffiliated people had steadily risen since the early 1990s. Previously, the “nones” had zoomed past 10 percent of the population by 1996, crossed the 15 percent threshold just a decade later, and managed to reach 20 percent by 2014. That rise had not abated in 2018. It had finally happened: the nones were now the same size as both Roman Catholics and evangelical Protestants. That meant that the religiously unaffiliated were statistically the same size as the largest religious groups in the United States of Burge (2021, pp. 19–20)”.
This social category has ceased to have a residual character, as we can see from the data. Its presence and social and statistical impact is perhaps the clearest indication of the loss of hegemony of Christianity over social life. It is a phenomenon that has occurred first in Europe and, subsequently, in the USA. In the case of the European countries studied, the data tell us that there have been two waves: The first in which France and Great Britain are included and the second in which Spain is included. To understand the reasons why each of the countries is situated in one wave or the other, we have to consider economic, political, cultural, or religious factors, among others. In this sense, and focusing on the Spanish case, we can see how, for example, the position of the Spanish Church in maintaining power in both the dictatorial (Franco) and democratic regimes, acting as an element of confrontation in the face of the differentiation between religious and secular spheres, has possibly had an effect on the fact that the development of the social category ‘nones’ has been delayed with respect to what has occurred in other countries. However, the data tell us that the growth of this category in recent years in Spain is really considerable.
In the case of the USA, Burge offers us a series of keys to understand the reasons for the appearance and growth of ‘the nones’: “If I were to have to point to a few factors, I think that secularization, politics, and the internet are the major causal factors that have given rise to the nones. It seems foolhardy to believe that Europe, a continent that is very similar to the United States in terms of educational attainment and economic growth, could see a massive decline in religious affiliation and that somehow the United States would avoid that same fate, at least to some degree. I do believe that America is a stubbornly religious country in ways that Europeans cannot adequately understand and social science can’t completely explain. What that means is that I don’t believe we will ever see a time where huge majorities of Americans are unchurched. However, to try to pinpoint where the march of the nones will end is a fool’s errand.” (Burge 2021, p. 136)
As a conclusion to this paragraph, we would like to dwell briefly on the political factor. For Burge, the appropriation of the religious discourse by the Republican party or the most conservative political proposals—let us remember that the mobilization of the white evangelist vote was key in the electoral victory of Donald Trump (Gorski and Perry 2022; Whitehead and Perry 2020)—and the acceptance of this state of affairs by the progressive parties—primarily by the Democratic party—has had a direct impact on the rise of the nones. This new distribution of forces and the role played by the religious element in it conditions the positioning of the collectives on issues of debate and cultural tension such as, for example, abortion.

5. Conclusions

The aim of this work was to carry out a comparative exercise of the religious situation of Christianity in four countries—the USA, France, Great Britain, and Spain—in the scenario of secularized societies. The journey undertaken has tried to contextualize, in the first introductory moment, the current religious scenario, which is characterized not so much by the decline of religion, but by the development of a pluralism of forms of sacralization and access to transcendence. In the current secular context, there coexist—among others—historical creeds or religions (Christianity, Islam, Judaism, Hinduism, and Buddhism), other forms of sacralization that are clearly secular (the human person, revolutionary cults, and civil society), and others that seek a refusion with nature or with spirituality.
This plurality, this pluralism (Berger 2014), as we say, is the main characteristic of the current secular religious situation. Now, when carrying out our analysis, we have focused on investigating data linked to historical religions—primarily Christian cults as a consequence of their majority character in the selected societies. Therefore, our proposal has comparatively analyzed the religious situation of these faiths in the current context. This means that the results can only be applicable to the realities measured by the data.
We have arranged the data obtained around the following categories of analysis: Contextualization: The importance of religion within different aspects of life; the level of religiosity and membership in religious denominations; the sphere of beliefs: Belief in God, belief in life after death, belief in hell, and belief in heaven; the scope of practices; and the nones. Finally, from among the exhaustively commented data, we have selected four elements of analysis with which we have dialogued in the discussion, and which have provided us with the necessary materials to reach a series of conclusions.
The first of these has focused on studying whether, on the basis of the data, we can affirm that Christianity is in crisis in the countries analyzed. The research tells us that we are witnessing what Cuchet (2018) calls a process of de-Christianization or Davis et al. (2023) calls dechurching. Therefore, we can indeed speak of a regression or decline of these cults, although not of their tendency to disappear.
The second element focused on remarking the exceptional character of the European religious situation—of Christianity in Europe—if we compare it with the situation of other historical creeds—Islam, for example—in other secularization programs or in European societies themselves. To paraphrase Heinrich (2020), we Europeans (even Westerners) would be the weirdest people as far as the religious situation is concerned. This statement has two main consequences: The first is the recognition of the existence of different secularization programs and, therefore, the existence of multiple secularities (Gil-Gimeno 2017a), and the second is the recognition that the European secularizing program is neither the model to follow nor the advance guard to be joined later by other secularized programs that are currently not so advanced, but rather, as we commented, is the exception (Casanova 2003, 2012).
The third element is linked to the decline in religious practice and its disadvantageous situation with respect to religious belief, fundamentally in God. People in the countries analyzed seem to be governed more by the logic of believing without belonging (Davie 1994, 2015) than the opposite. Now, the second logic (Hervieu-Léger and Champion 1986) works correctly if we apply the immigration variable to the analysis.
Finally, and once the phenomenon of dechurching has been ascertained, we thought it would be interesting to briefly present the social group that has emerged as a consequence of the crisis of these historical religious forms. We have briefly highlighted two aspects in this regard: The constant increase in ‘the nones’ (we have been able to see it in the data and in Burge’s work) and certain reasons that allow us to understand the reason for this boom. One of these reasons could clearly be secularization understood from the perspective of functional differentiation of secular and religious spheres, which is undoubtedly the strong thesis of secularization theory (Casanova 2012).
In summary, our work has attempted to analyze the relationship between the secular and the religious in the context of four countries with a Judeo-Christian tradition such as the USA, France, Great Britain, and Spain, based on data provided by the World Values Survey. The data provide us with a concrete measure of the vitality of the religious phenomenon in these societies in the context of the “Secular Age” as defined by Taylor (2007). In the future, it would be interesting to be able to compare these data to other countries in which the dominant religion or religious culture is not Judeo-Christian.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, V.D.d.R. and J.G.-G.; methodology, V.D.d.R.; software, V.D.d.R. and J.G.-G.; validation, V.D.d.R. and J.G.-G.; formal analysis, V.D.d.R.; investigation, V.D.d.R. and J.G.-G.; resources, V.D.d.R. and J.G.-G.; data curation, V.D.d.R.; writing—original draft preparation V.D.d.R.; writing—review and editing, V.D.d.R. and J.G.-G.; visualization, V.D.d.R. and J.G.-G.; supervision, V.D.d.R. and J.G.-G.; project administration, V.D.d.R. and J.G.-G. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

Not applicable.

Informed Consent Statement

Not applicable.

Data Availability Statement

Not applicable.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Appendix A. Emigration Status

CasesPercentage
FIRST GENERATION4649.70%
Mother, father and I are immigrant: 9.7% (464 cases)  
Second Generation3928.20%
Mahter immigrant, I not: 5.5% (260 cases)
Father immigrant, I not: 2.8% (132 cases)
BORN OUTSIDE OF NATIVE PARENTS 851.80%
I immigrant, Mahter not: 1.3% (62 cases)
I immigrant, Father not: 0.5% (22 cases)
NATIVES 382080.20%
Respondent father & Mother
have been born in country: 80.2% (3820 cases)
Total 4760100
Non response 75

Appendix B. Questions Used (as Formulated). Note: Reproduced from “2017–2021 World Values Survey Wave 7 Master Survey Questionnaire”

                   RELIGIOUS VALUES                   
  • (SHOW CARD 17)
  • Q164. How important is God in your life? Please use this scale to indicate. 10 means “very important” and 1 means “not at all important.” (Code one number):
    Not at all important            Very important
    12345678910
Which, if any, of the following do you believe in?
Yes No
Q165 God12
Q166Life after death 12
Q167Hell12
Q168Heaven12
(SHOW CARD 18)
Q171.
Apart from weddings and funerals, about how often do you attend religious services these days? (One answer):
1
Several times a day
2
More than once a week
3
Once a week
4
Once a month
5
Only on special holy days
6
Once a year
7
Less often
8
Never, practically never
Q172.
Apart from weddings and funerals, about how often do you pray? (Code one answer):
1
Several times a day
2
Once a day
3
Several times each week
4
Only when attending religious services
5
Only on special holy days
6
Once a year
7
Less often
8
Never, practically never
Q173.
Independently of whether you attend religious services or not, would you say you are…? (read out and code one answer):
1
A religious person
2
Not a religious person
3
An atheist
Q289.
Do you belong to a religion or religious denomination? If yes, which one? (Code answer due to list below. Code 0 if the respondent answering “no denomination”)
No: do not belong to a denomination 0
Yes:Roman Catholic1
Protestant2
Orthodox (Russian/Greek/etc.)3
Jew4
Muslim5
Hindu6
Buddhist7
Other (write in)8
NOTE: If your own society does not fit into this codign system, please devise an alternative, following this as closely as possible; for example, in Islamic countries, ask about Sunni, Shia, etc. Send a list of the categories used hear along with your data).

Appendix C. Supplementary Charts

Figure A1. Importance of religion, according to sociodemographic characteristics.
Figure A1. Importance of religion, according to sociodemographic characteristics.
Religions 14 01005 g0a1aReligions 14 01005 g0a1bReligions 14 01005 g0a1cReligions 14 01005 g0a1d

Appendix D

Table A1. Sociodemographic traits of those who believe in God, including religious denomination and non-membership of a religious creed.
Table A1. Sociodemographic traits of those who believe in God, including religious denomination and non-membership of a religious creed.
FranceUnited KingdomSpainUSA
Odds Ratio Odds Ratio Odds Ratio Odds Ratio
Predictors
Sexo: mujeresN.S. N.S. N.S. 2.043***
(Ref.: varones)
Study
  LowerN.S. N.S. 1.955***5.946
  MiddleN.S. N.S. 2.081**1.904***
(Ref.: Upper)
Part of the working populationN.S. 1.435**N.S. N.S.
(Ref.: no)
Inmigration
Inmigrants1.670**2.624***N.S. N.S.
(Ref.: Natives)
Persona religiosa14.158***17.917***32.513***36.189
(Ref.: no religioso y ateo)
No confesión religi0.136***0.273***0.067**0.187
(Ref.: confesión relig)
Constant1.377***0.424***2.020***1.119
R20.605 0.581 0.696 0.521
N1.596 1.627 1.085 2.252
Notes: N.S.: No significant relationship. Source: (European Values Study 2022). ** Level of significance <0.05. *** Level of significance <0.01.

Notes

1
15–30, 31–49, 50–64, 65 and more years.
2
Sampling fraction is the ratio of the sample size (n) to population size (N).
3
Gender, age (15–30, 31–49, 50–64, 65 and more years), educational level (lower, middle, upper), relationship with activity (working, unemployed, retired, student, housewife), whether or not the respondent is part of the working population, and whether or not the respondent is an immigrant are considered.
4
Despite the interest of being a first or second generation immigrant, it is not included in the model due to the low sample size: 856 cases, 18% of the sample. Including it would have conditioned the number of cases included in the rest of the variables. This aspect will be addressed later, by means of a cross-tabulation with the variable under study.
5
In order to improve the visualization of the graph, these are mean scores, calculated by multiplying ‘very important’ by 4, ‘rather’ by 3, ‘not very’ by 2 and ‘not at all important’ by 1.
6
Statistic used to measure the relationship between variables, see Section 4.
7
Despite the interest of being a first or second generation migrant, it is not included in the model due to the low sample size: 856 cases, 18% of the sample. Including it would have conditioned the number of cases included in the rest of the variables. This aspect will be addressed later, by means of a cross-tabulation with the variable under study.
8
Ratio of reasons, i.e., the ratio between the odds of considering oneself a religious person being an emigrant (religious emigrants/non-religious emigrants and atheists) and the odds of considering oneself a religious person being a native (religious natives/non-religious natives and atheists).
9
Protestant 2.1%, Other Christian (Evangelical/Pentecostal/Free church/etc.) 1.5%, Orthodox 1.2%, Jew 1%, Buddhist 0.8%, Hindu 0.2% and other 2.1%. Only the faiths reported by 1% of the sample were considered, with the rest grouped in the ‘other’ category.
10
Interpretative note: the percentages do not add up to 100 because atheists and those defined as “non-religious persons” are not considered. These would occupy the upper part of each bar, i.e., the space above 72% in the case of Roman Catholics in France, 20% (100–20) of French Protestants….
11
The coefficients are the neperian logarithm of these values. Thus, for example, considering the odds ratio of French women in Table 4, 1.465, its neperian logarithm is 0.382, the coefficient; so with the information shown in Table 5 it is very simple to arrive at the coefficients. In order to make it easier to follow only these coefficients will be used from now on.
12
In order to facilitate interpretation, these graphs have a row at the base for the marginal, given the large distance from graph 11, which presents belief responses.
13
R2 values of 0.319, 0.229, 0.282 and 0.272 in France, UK, Spain and USA respectively, and more than 75% of cases correctly classified.

References

  1. Beck, Ulrich. 2009. El Dios Personal. Barcelona: Paidos. [Google Scholar]
  2. Bellah, Robert N. 1967. Civil Religion in America. Dædalus, Journal of the American Academy of Arts and Sciences 96: 1–21. [Google Scholar]
  3. Bellah, Robert N. 1969. Religious Evolution. In Sociology and Religion: A Book of Readings. Edited by Birnbaum Norman and Lenzer Gertrud. Upper Saddle River: Prentice-Hall, pp. 67–83. [Google Scholar]
  4. Bellah, Robert N. 2011. Religion in Human Evolution. From the Paleolithic to the Axial Age. Cambridge: The Belknap Press of Harvard University Press. [Google Scholar]
  5. Berger, Peter L. 2014. The Many Altars of Modernity: Toward a Paradigm for Religion in a Pluralist Age. London: De Gruyter. [Google Scholar]
  6. Burge, Ryan P. 2021. The Nones: Where They Came From, Who They Are, and Where They Are Going. Minneapolis: Fortress Press. [Google Scholar]
  7. Casanova, José. 1994. Public Religions in the Modern World. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. [Google Scholar]
  8. Casanova, José. 2003. Beyond European and American Excepcionalisms: Towards a Global Perspective. In Predicting Religion: Christian, Secular and Alternative Futures. Edited by Davie Grace, Heelas Paul and Woodhead Linda. New York: Aldershot, pp. 17–29. [Google Scholar]
  9. Casanova, José. 2009. The secular and secularisms. Social Research 76: 1049–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Casanova, José. 2012. Genealogías de la Secularización. Barcelona: Anthropos. [Google Scholar]
  11. Cuchet, Guillaume. 2018. Comment Notre Monde a cessé d’être Chrétien. Anatomie d’un Effondrement. Paris: Editions du Seuil. [Google Scholar]
  12. Davie, Grace. 1994. Religion in Britain since 1945: Believing without Belonging. Oxford: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
  13. Davie, Grace. 2015. Religion in Britain: A Persistent Paradox. London: Wiley. [Google Scholar]
  14. Davis, Jim, Graham Michael, and Ryan P. Burge. 2023. The Great Dechurching: Who’s Leaving, Why Are They Going, and What Will It Take to Bring Them Back? New York: Zondervand. [Google Scholar]
  15. Donald, Merlin. 1991. Origins of the Modern Mind: Three Stages in the Evolution of the Culture and Cognition. Cambridge: Harvard University Press. [Google Scholar]
  16. Douthat, Ross. 2022. The Americanization of religion. New York Times, December 21. [Google Scholar]
  17. Durkheim, Émile. 1973. Individualism and the Intellectuals. In Émile Durkheim on Morality and Society. Edited by R. N. Bellah. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press. [Google Scholar]
  18. Durkheim, Émile. 1995. The Elementary Forms of Religious Life. New York: Free Press. [Google Scholar]
  19. Eisenstadt, Shmuel N. 2000. Multiple modernities. Daedalus 129: 1–29. [Google Scholar]
  20. European Social Survey. 2022. About the European Social Survey European Research Infrastructure (ESS ERIC). Available online: https://www.europeansocialsurvey.org/about/ (accessed on 12 December 2022).
  21. European Values Study. 2020. European Values Study (EVS) 2017: Methodological Guidelines (GESIS, Papers, 2020/13). Köln: European Values Study. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. European Values Study. 2022. Join EVS/WVS 2017–2022. Available online: https://europeanvaluesstudy.eu/methodology-data-documentation/survey-2017/joint-evs-wvs/ (accessed on 22 December 2022).
  23. Gil-Gimeno, Javier. 2017a. Secularizaciones múltiples. Sociología Histórica 7: 291–319. [Google Scholar]
  24. Gil-Gimeno, Javier. 2017b. De la confrontación al aggiornamento en las relaciones entre Iglesia Católica y Modernidad. Sociológica 91: 1–37. [Google Scholar]
  25. Gil-Gimeno, Javier. 2020. Repensando la relación entre lo secular y lo religioso. Análisis de dos puntos ciegos asociados a la teoría de la secularización. Revista ILU 25: 57–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Glascock, Taylor. 2023. The resentment fueling the Republican Party is not coming from the suburbs. New York Times, January 25. [Google Scholar]
  27. Gorski, Philip, and Samuel L. Perry. 2022. The Flag and the Cross. White Christian Nationalism and the Threath to American Democracy. Oxford: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
  28. Grose, Jessica. 2023. The Largest and Fastest Religious Shift in America Is Well Underway. New York Times. Available online: https://www.nytimes.com/2023/06/21/opinion/religion-dechurching.html (accessed on 28 June 2023).
  29. Habermas, Jürgen. 1998. Teoría de la Acción Comunicativa. Madrid: Taurus, vol. II. [Google Scholar]
  30. Harkness, Janet A., Michael Braun, Brad Edwards, Timothy P. Johnson, Lars E. Lyberg, Peter Ph. Mohler, Beth-Ellen Pennell, and Tom W. Smith. 2010. Survey Methods in Multinational, Multiregional, and Multicultural Contexts. Hoboken: Wiley. [Google Scholar]
  31. Harkness, Janet A., Van de Vijver, J. R. Fons, and Peter Ph. Mohler, eds. 2003. Cross-Cultural Survey Methods. Hoboken: Wiley. [Google Scholar]
  32. Heinrich, Joseph. 2020. The Weirdest People in the World: How the West Became Psychologically Peculiar and Particularly Prosperous. New York: Penguin. [Google Scholar]
  33. Hénaff, Marcel. 2010. The Price of Truth. Standford: Standford University Press. [Google Scholar]
  34. Hervieu-Léger, Danièle, and Françoise Champion. 1986. Vers un Nouveau Christianisme? Introduction à la Sociologie du Christianisme Occidentel. Paris: Les editions du Cerf. [Google Scholar]
  35. James, William. 1917. The Varieties of Religious Experience: A Study in Human Nature. New York: Longmans, Green and Co. [Google Scholar]
  36. Joas, Hans. 2014. The Sacredness of the Person. Georgetown: Georgetown University Press. [Google Scholar]
  37. Johnson, Timothy P., Beth-Ellen Pennell, Ineke Stoop, and Brita Dorer. 2019. The promise and challenge of 3MC Research. In Advances in Comparative Survey Methods: Multinational, Multiregional, and Multicultural Contexts (3MC). Edited by Timothy P. Johnson, Beth-Ellen Pennell, Ineke Stoop and Brita Dorer. Hoboken: Wiley, pp. 3–12. [Google Scholar]
  38. Lenoir, Frédéric. 2005. Las Metamorfosis de Dios. Madrid: Alianza. [Google Scholar]
  39. Luckmann, Thomas. 1967. The Invisible Religion: The Problem of Religion in Modern Society. New York: Macmillan. [Google Scholar]
  40. Mathiez, Albert. 2012. Los Orígenes de los Cultos Revolucionarios. Madrid: CIS. [Google Scholar]
  41. Pérez-Agote, Alfonso. 2015. Las dinámicas de la evolución religiosa. In España 2015: Situación Social. Edited by Torres Albero. Cristobal Madrid: CIS, pp. 1433–61. [Google Scholar]
  42. Scheaffer, Richard L., William Mendenhall III, R. Ott Lyman, and Kenneth G. Gerow. 2006. Elementary Survey Sampling. Australia: Brooks/Cole. [Google Scholar]
  43. Simmel, Georg. 2012. La Religión. Barcelona: Gedisa. [Google Scholar]
  44. Taylor, Charles. 2007. A Secular Age. Harvard: The Belknap Press of Harvard University Press. [Google Scholar]
  45. The International Social Survey Programme. 2022. About ISSP. Available online: https://issp.org/ (accessed on 12 December 2022).
  46. Urrutia León, Manuel Mª. 2010. Religión, creencias, prácticas e iglesias. In Un Individualismo Placentero y Protegido. Edited by Javier Elzo and María Silvestre. Bilbao: Universidad de Deusto, pp. 127–64. [Google Scholar]
  47. Weber, Max. 1978. Economy and Society. An Outline of Interpretive Sociology. Los Angeles: University of California Press. [Google Scholar]
  48. Weber, Max. 1983. Ensayos Sobre Sociología de la Religión. Madrid: Taurus, vol. 1. [Google Scholar]
  49. Whitehead, Andrew L., and Samuel L. Perry. 2020. Taking America Back for God: Christian Nationalism in the United States. Oxford: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
  50. World Values Survey. 2008. WV5_Results_By_Country Study # v20180912. Available online: https://www.worldvaluessurvey.org/WVSDocumentationWV5.jsp (accessed on 12 December 2022).
  51. World Values Survey. 2022. How We Are. Available online: https://www.worldvaluessurvey.org/WVSContents.jsp (accessed on 12 December 2022).
Figure 1. Degree of importance of different aspects of life (in France, the United Kingdom, Spain, and the United States of America).
Figure 1. Degree of importance of different aspects of life (in France, the United Kingdom, Spain, and the United States of America).
Religions 14 01005 g001
Figure 2. Degree of importance to different aspects of life, by country.
Figure 2. Degree of importance to different aspects of life, by country.
Religions 14 01005 g002
Figure 3. Evolution of the degree of importance of religion in the four countries under consideration.
Figure 3. Evolution of the degree of importance of religion in the four countries under consideration.
Religions 14 01005 g003
Figure 4. Religious denomination in the four countries considered.
Figure 4. Religious denomination in the four countries considered.
Religions 14 01005 g004
Figure 5. Evolution of the religious denomination in the four countries under consideration.
Figure 5. Evolution of the religious denomination in the four countries under consideration.
Religions 14 01005 g005
Figure 6. Religious denomination according to first- or second-generation immigrants in the four countries considered (ordered by degree of influence).
Figure 6. Religious denomination according to first- or second-generation immigrants in the four countries considered (ordered by degree of influence).
Religions 14 01005 g006
Figure 7. Do not belong to a denomination, 2017 vs. 2005/07.
Figure 7. Do not belong to a denomination, 2017 vs. 2005/07.
Religions 14 01005 g007
Figure 8. Responses to the question do you belong to a religion or religious denomination in the four countries.
Figure 8. Responses to the question do you belong to a religion or religious denomination in the four countries.
Religions 14 01005 g008
Figure 9. Differences (2017–2005/7) in the monitoring of religions in the four countries considered.
Figure 9. Differences (2017–2005/7) in the monitoring of religions in the four countries considered.
Religions 14 01005 g009
Figure 10. Religious disaffection: Percentage of followers of religions who consider themselves “religious people” in the four countries considered10.
Figure 10. Religious disaffection: Percentage of followers of religions who consider themselves “religious people” in the four countries considered10.
Religions 14 01005 g010
Figure 11. Belief in God, life after death, heaven, and hell in the four countries considered.
Figure 11. Belief in God, life after death, heaven, and hell in the four countries considered.
Religions 14 01005 g011
Figure 12. Importance of God in participants’ lives in the four countries under consideration.
Figure 12. Importance of God in participants’ lives in the four countries under consideration.
Religions 14 01005 g012
Figure 13. Belief in God according to whether one is a first- or secon—generation immigrant, in the four countries considered.
Figure 13. Belief in God according to whether one is a first- or secon—generation immigrant, in the four countries considered.
Religions 14 01005 g013
Figure 14. Belief in God according to religious denomination and belonging or not to a religion12.
Figure 14. Belief in God according to religious denomination and belonging or not to a religion12.
Religions 14 01005 g014
Figure 15. There is life after death, according to religious denomination and belonging or not to a religion.
Figure 15. There is life after death, according to religious denomination and belonging or not to a religion.
Religions 14 01005 g015aReligions 14 01005 g015b
Figure 16. Belief in hell by first- or second-generation immigrants in countries with significant relationships.
Figure 16. Belief in hell by first- or second-generation immigrants in countries with significant relationships.
Religions 14 01005 g016
Figure 17. Belief in the existence of hell, according to religious denomination and religious or non-religious affiliation.
Figure 17. Belief in the existence of hell, according to religious denomination and religious or non-religious affiliation.
Religions 14 01005 g017
Figure 18. Belief in the existence of heaven, by religious denomination and religious or non-religious affiliation.
Figure 18. Belief in the existence of heaven, by religious denomination and religious or non-religious affiliation.
Religions 14 01005 g018aReligions 14 01005 g018b
Figure 19. Attendance at religious services in the four countries under consideration.
Figure 19. Attendance at religious services in the four countries under consideration.
Religions 14 01005 g019
Figure 20. Differences (2017–2005/07) in attendance at religious services in the four countries considered.
Figure 20. Differences (2017–2005/07) in attendance at religious services in the four countries considered.
Religions 14 01005 g020
Figure 21. Non-attendance at religious services, by first- or second-generation immigrant, in the countries considered.
Figure 21. Non-attendance at religious services, by first- or second-generation immigrant, in the countries considered.
Religions 14 01005 g021
Figure 22. Non-attendance at religious services *, by religious denomination and religious affiliation or non-affiliation.
Figure 22. Non-attendance at religious services *, by religious denomination and religious affiliation or non-affiliation.
Religions 14 01005 g022
Figure 23. Non-attendance at religious services, by first- or second-generation immigrants, in the countries considered.
Figure 23. Non-attendance at religious services, by first- or second-generation immigrants, in the countries considered.
Religions 14 01005 g023
Figure 24. Frequency of prayer, limited to European countries.
Figure 24. Frequency of prayer, limited to European countries.
Religions 14 01005 g024
Figure 25. They pray ‘several times a year’, ‘less often’ and ‘never’ according to religious denomination and belonging or not to a religion.
Figure 25. They pray ‘several times a year’, ‘less often’ and ‘never’ according to religious denomination and belonging or not to a religion.
Religions 14 01005 g025aReligions 14 01005 g025b
Figure 26. Ratio of self-identified religious people and those who do not belong to any religion.
Figure 26. Ratio of self-identified religious people and those who do not belong to any religion.
Religions 14 01005 g026
Figure 27. Presence of “nones” in the four countries considered.
Figure 27. Presence of “nones” in the four countries considered.
Religions 14 01005 g027
Figure 28. Demographic profile of nones and non-nones.
Figure 28. Demographic profile of nones and non-nones.
Religions 14 01005 g028
Table 1. Sample sizes and errors, dates of data collection, and mode of data collection used in this paper.
Table 1. Sample sizes and errors, dates of data collection, and mode of data collection used in this paper.
CountrySize SamplingError Sampling *Dates of Collection of InformationMode of Collection
France1870±2.31%2 March 2018–16 August 2018CAPI
United Kingdom1788±2.36%12 February 2018–16 July 2018CAPI
Spain1209±2.88%28 November 2017–22 January 2018CAPI
USA2596±1.96%28 April 2017–31 May 2017CAWI y CATI
* P and Q = 0.5, confidence level 95.5%, under the assumption of simple random sampling; Source: (European Social Survey 2022).
Table 2. Sample sizes, weighting coefficients, and sample considered.
Table 2. Sample sizes, weighting coefficients, and sample considered.
CountrySize SamplingWeighting CoefficientsSampling in Each Country
France18700.6465241209
United Kingdom17880.6761741209
Spain120911209
USA25960.4653311209
Source: Prepared by the authors based on the data shown in Table 6.
Table 3. Sample sizes and errors, weighting coefficients, and dates of data collection.
Table 3. Sample sizes and errors, weighting coefficients, and dates of data collection.
Country Size SamplingError Sampling *Weighting CoefficientsDates of Collection of Information
France 1001±3.2%31 January 2006–24 February 2006
United Kingdom1041±3.1%0.9615751 December 2005–18 December 2005
Spain1200±2.9%0.83416710 July 2007–24 July 2007
USA1249±2.8%0.80144119 September 2006–29 September 2006
* P and Q = 0.5, confidence level 95.5%, under the assumption of simple random sampling; Source: (World Values Survey 2008).
Table 4. Influential variables in religious denomination.
Table 4. Influential variables in religious denomination.
FranceUKSpainUSA
Coef. Standard ErrorOdds RatioCoef. Standard ErrorOdds RatioCoef. Standard ErrorOdds RatioCoef. Standard ErrorOdds Ratio
Predictors
Sex: woman0.382***0.1301.4650.536***0.1331.7100.739***0.132.0930.48***0.1311.615
(Ref.: man)
Age
  18 to 30−0.611***0.2050.543−1.175***0.2640.309−1.585***0.2290.205−1.342***0.2240.261
  31 to 48−0.553***0.1760.575−0.876***0.2290.417−1.361***0.1850.256−0.885***0.2130.413
  50 to 64−0.392**0.1800.675−0.472**0.2180.623−1.240***0.1900.289−0.570**0.2310.566
(Ref.: 65 & older)
Estudy
  Lower0.602***0.1841.825N.S. N.S.N.S.0.578***0.1691.783N.S. N.S.N.S.
  Middle0.148 0.1531.160N.S. N.S.N.S.0.418**0.1911.520N.S. N.S.N.S.
(Ref.: Upper)
Part of the working populationN.S. N.S.N.S.0.38**0.1911.462N.S. N.S.N.S.N.S. N.S.N.S.
(Ref.: no)
Inmigrants1.052***0.1522.8631.348***0.1643.8501.226 0.2043.409N.S. N.S.N.S.
(Ref.: Natives)
Constant−1.120***0.2600.326−1.558***0.4090.120−0.647**0.2800.524N.S. N.S.N.S.
R20112 0.150 0.192 0.058
N1.711 1.649 1.139 2.304
%decasoscorrec-
Notes: N.S.: No significant relationship. The relationship with activity does not appear because there is no significant term. Source: (European Values Study 2022). ** Level of significance <0.05. *** Level of significance <0.01.
Table 5. Sociodemographic characteristics of the followers of the Roman Catholic religion in France, Spain and the United States, and of the Protestant religion in the United Kingdom and the United States.
Table 5. Sociodemographic characteristics of the followers of the Roman Catholic religion in France, Spain and the United States, and of the Protestant religion in the United Kingdom and the United States.
Catholic RomanProtestants
FranceSpainUSAUKUSA
Odds Ratio Odds Ratio Odds Ratio Odds Ratio Odds Ratio
Predictors
Sexo: woman1.622***1.631***1.438**1.633***N.S.***
(Ref.: man)
Age
  18 to 300.234***0.141***0.408***0.110***0.233***
  31 to 480.317***0.264***0.518**0.166***0.282***
  50 to 640.516***0.303***0.818 0.499***0.396***
(Ref.: 65 & older)
Study
  LowerN.S. N.S. N.S. N.S. 0.404
  MiddleN.S. N.S. N.S.**N.S. 0.629**
(Ref.: Highier)
InmigrantsN.S. N.S. 1.853***N.S. 0.468**
(Ref.: Natives)
Constant0.285***0.49***0.172***0.162***0.733***
R20.093 0.141 0.035 0.128 0.088
N1.749 1.208 2.311 1.788 2.596
Notes: N.S.: No significant relationship. The relationship with activity does not appear because there is no significant term. Source: (European Values Study 2022). ** Level of significance <0.05. *** Level of significance <0.01.
Table 6. Sociodemographic characteristics of those who believe in God.
Table 6. Sociodemographic characteristics of those who believe in God.
FranceUnited KingdomSpainUSA
Odds Ratio Odds Ratio Odds Ratio Odds Ratio
Predictors
Sexo: mujeres1.514***1.628***1.571***2.253***
(Ref.: varones)
Age
  18 to 300.567**0.259***0.245***0.261***
  31 to 480.734*0.343***0.357***0.443**
  50 to 640.893 0.454***0.398***0.624
(Ref.: 65 & older)
Study
  Lower1.639**N.S. 1.986***4.376
  Middle1.074 N.S. 1.555**1.538***
(Ref.: upper)
Part of the working populationN.S. N.S. 1.579**N.S.
(Ref.: no)
Inmigrants2.685***4.805***3.263***N.S.
(Ref.: Natives)
Constant0.494**0.684*0.861 2.255***
R20.920 0.158 0.168 0.081
N1.628 1.646 1.109 2.288
Notes: N.S.: No significant relationship. The relationship with activity does not appear because there is no significant term. Source: (European Values Study 2022). * Level of significance <0.1.** Level of significance <0.05. *** Level of significance <0.01.
Table 7. Sociodemographic characteristics of those who believe that there is life after death.
Table 7. Sociodemographic characteristics of those who believe that there is life after death.
FranceUnited KingdomSpainUSA
Odds Ratio Odds Ratio Odds Ratio Odds Ratio
Predictors
Sexo: woman1.909***1.867***1.581**1.844***
(Ref.: man)
Age
  18 to 301.981***N.S. N.S. N.S.
  31 to 481.499**N.S. N.S. N.S.
  50 to 641.052 N.S. N.S. N.S.
(Ref.: 65 & older)
Part of the working populationN.S. N.S. 1.363**N.S.
(Ref.: no)
Inmigrats2.136***1.782***2.058***N.S.
(Ref.: Natives)
Constant0.193***0.239***0.233***0.966
R20.086 0.049 0.042 0.027
N1.555 1.646 1.019 2.260
Notes: N.S.: No significant relationship. The relationship and level of studies with activity does not appear because there is no significant term. Source: (European Values Study 2022). ** Level of significance <0.05. *** Level of significance <0.01.
Table 8. Sociodemographic characteristics of those who believe that hell exists.
Table 8. Sociodemographic characteristics of those who believe that hell exists.
FranceUnited KingdomSpainUSA
Odds Ratio Odds Ratio Odds Ratio Odds Ratio
Predictors
Sexo: woman1.909***1.867***1.581**1.844***
(Ref.: man)
Age
  18 to 301.981***N.S. N.S. N.S.
  31 to 481.499**N.S. N.S. N.S.
  50 to 641.052 N.S. N.S. N.S.
(Ref.: 65 & older)
Study
  LowerN.S.***N.S. N.S. N.S.
  MiddleN.S. N.S. N.S. N.S.
(Ref.: Upper)
Activity
  EmployedN.S. N.S. N.S. N.S.
  UnenployedN.S. N.S. N.S. N.S.
  RetiredN.S. N.S. N.S. N.S.
  StudentN.S. N.S. N.S. N.S.
(Ref.: Hosewife)
Part of the working populationN.S. N.S. 1.363**N.S.
(Ref.: no)
Inmigrats2.136***1.782***2.058***N.S.
(Ref.: Natives)
Constant0.193***0.239***0.233***0.966**
R20.086 0.049 0.042 0.027
N1.555 1.646 1.019 2.260
Notes: N.S.: No significant relationship. Source: (European Values Study 2022). ** Level of significance <0.05. *** Level of significance <0.01.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Díaz de Rada, V.; Gil-Gimeno, J. Have the Inhabitants of France, Great Britain, Spain, and the US Been Secularized? An Analysis Comparing the Religious Data in These Countries. Religions 2023, 14, 1005. https://doi.org/10.3390/rel14081005

AMA Style

Díaz de Rada V, Gil-Gimeno J. Have the Inhabitants of France, Great Britain, Spain, and the US Been Secularized? An Analysis Comparing the Religious Data in These Countries. Religions. 2023; 14(8):1005. https://doi.org/10.3390/rel14081005

Chicago/Turabian Style

Díaz de Rada, Vidal, and Javier Gil-Gimeno. 2023. "Have the Inhabitants of France, Great Britain, Spain, and the US Been Secularized? An Analysis Comparing the Religious Data in These Countries" Religions 14, no. 8: 1005. https://doi.org/10.3390/rel14081005

APA Style

Díaz de Rada, V., & Gil-Gimeno, J. (2023). Have the Inhabitants of France, Great Britain, Spain, and the US Been Secularized? An Analysis Comparing the Religious Data in These Countries. Religions, 14(8), 1005. https://doi.org/10.3390/rel14081005

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop