Mystical Experience †
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Theory
3. Language
Before my teacher came to me, I did not know that I am. I lived in a world that was a no-world. I cannot hope to describe adequately that unconscious, yet conscious time of nothingness. I did not know that I knew aught, or that I lived or acted or desired. I had neither will nor intellect. I was carried along to objects and acts by a certain blind natural impetus. I had a mind which caused me to feel anger, satisfaction, desire. These two facts led those about me to suppose that I willed and thought. I can remember all this, not because I knew that it was so, but because I have tactual memory. It enables me to remember that I never contracted my forehead in the act of thinking. I never viewed anything beforehand or chose it. I also recall tactually the fact that never in a start of the body or a heart-beat did I feel that I loved or cared for anything. My inner life, then, was a blank without past, present, or future, without hope or anticipation, without wonder or joy or faith.
During the first year of life, a child will be quite unable to use the sign-stimulus, will not understand the directions from the experimenter and can relate to the objects only haphazardly. It is really senseless to talk of concepts at all at this stage of development. …
…for Vygotsky, being able to name an object is not evidence of having a concept of it, at least not a true concept …
A concept is a mediated relationship of a person to their environment in which a word, acting as a sign for a problem or solution encountered by the community in the past, is used to organise the individual’s actions, but which necessarily also includes immediate sensorimotor interactions with the environment. …
…In so far as [children’s] relationship to the world is mediated (for example through sensorimotor activity itself), the mediating element is their own body—grasping, crying, sucking, etc. In such a condition, a child is not able to develop concepts at all. Indeed, in their first efforts at using words, they completely fail, but, as the saying goes, in order to swim one must get into the water, and once the child throws themself into speech, they begin to learn, and the most embryonic phenomena of conceptual thinking can be said to have come into being.
So what exactly is there, in a leopard-identifying animal’s brain?
I think there is not anything in its brain that relates specifically to leopards in the way that either a thought or a word in the human brain does. All over the brain there are cohorts of neurons that respond directly to all the sights and sounds and smells that come in from the world by changing the rate at which they send out electrical impulses. Among all of these cohorts are neurons responding to sights and sounds and smells that might be made by leopards. When “enough” of these neurons (“enough” being still a black box) are triggered by a leopard appearance, the animal goes into high alerts, may issue an alarm call, may take appropriate action. But the neurons activated on any given occasion are just one subset of the complete set of potentially leopard-responding neurons. The next appearance of a leopard may trigger a quite different subset, though the result (in terms of the animal’s reactions) may be identical. Bottom line is, there’s nowhere any fixed, determined set of linked neurons that represents “leopard” and nothing else.
Differently put, concepts are the result of combining different mental contents.But once you have a word or sign for “leopard”, there has to be such a set. There has to be a fixed, permanent set of neurons that represent the sounds or gestures needed to produce the word or sign in question. But for that word or sign to have meaning, this fixed set has to link to all the different representations of leopard-bits on which the original “leopard” category was based.
I’m not saying that “concepts are words”, or “you have to have a word to have a concept”. Least of all am I saying, “You cannot think without words”. … [O]nce the brain found the trick of making concepts, it no longer needed a word as the base for a new concept. It just needed some place where all the knowledge could come together and link with other concepts.
4. Mental Absorption
The simplest variety of affect begins in the interior of a living organism. It springs up vague and diffuse, generating feelings that are not easily described or placed. The term “primordial feelings” captures the idea.
My use of the term “primordial” is conventional and meant to refer to the simple and direct nature of what I conceive of feelings as having been as they emerged in early human evolution and as they still are likely to be in many nonhuman species not to mention human infants.
5. Mystical Experience
- -
- A sense of unity or totality
- -
- A sense of timelessness
- -
- A sense of having encountered ultimate reality
- -
- A sense of sacredness
- -
- A sense that one cannot adequately describe the richness of this experience.
6. The Memory of Mental States
you could say that babies and young children have episodic memory but not autobiographical memory. Although they are very good at remembering specific events in the past, they do not put these events into a single coherent timeline, do not remember how they know about the events, and do not remember their past attitudes toward events. .. And they do not have a single “inner autobiographer”, a self who links their past and present mental states.
7. States and Statements
8. Further Implications
9. Trait and State
Dissociative absorption is a tendency to become absorbed in imagination or in an external stimulus (movie, book) to the point of obliviousness to one’s surroundings and reduced self-awareness.
10. Conclusions
According to Steven Katz62 … as well as other ‘constructivists’ … pure, unmediated experience simply does not exist. Each and every experience went through complex epistemological processes by which it was organised and shaped, and which made it communicable. Mystical experience, according to these critics, will always be prefigured and preconditioned by linguistic frameworks and the cultural context, the respective theologies and philosophies, the dogmas, social conditions, and pre-existing worldviews. What others had called interpretation was itself an ingredient of the experience.
Funding
Conflicts of Interest
1 | Wulff (2014, pp. 369–70) sketches the way mysticism and mystical experience have been used from the 18th century onward. See also (Zarrabi-Zadeh 2008). |
2 | Annette Wilke (Wilke et al. 2021, p. 5) points out “that not only in popular discourse, but even among most of early scholars of mysticism, union, unity, unification, i.e., immediate unitive experience (unio mystica, ‘mystical union’) was very prominent in characterizing and defining universal mysticism. … many of them, however, were not interested in defining mystical experience as such, … Only later, when mysticism research had shifted to the Anglo-Saxon world, the nature of mystical experience itself became a common central question.” |
3 | See, e.g., (Franco 2018). For a different point of view, see (Osto 2019). |
4 | Indeed, Turner (1995) argues that medieval European mysticism is not based on mystical experience; see also (Kügler 2004). |
5 | Wilke (Wilke et al. 2021, p. 2), with references to earlier literature. |
6 | Smart (1967), too, does not hesitate to speak of the “timeless experience” of Eckhart, Teresa of Avila, Śankara, and the Buddha that, he claims, “involves an apprehension of the transcendent”; see also Smart (1965, p. 75). |
7 | |
8 | Hood (1975) developed a measure of reported mystical experience, Hood’s M-scale (see also Streib et al. 2021). Hood’s M-scale measures a personality trait, not the depth of a mystical state. |
9 | |
10 | On the influence of memory on perception and subjective experience, see (Garner and Keller 2022; Lau et al. 2022). |
11 | Herzog et al. (2016); further Drissi-Daoudi et al. (2019). See also Manassi and Whitney (2022). On the experience of time passing more or less fast, see Wittmann (2013, 2018). |
12 | Buonomano (2017, p. 252 n. 11); with references to Scharnowski et al. (2009) and Sergent et al. (2013). |
13 | |
14 | |
15 | |
16 | One might hope to find similar testimonies in so-called feral children, children who have grown up without human contact. Unfortunately, few known feral children ever learned to speak, and the few that did (though imperfectly) have not recorded memories of their pre-linguistic experiences. See Candland (1993); https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Feral_child (accessed on 4 May 2022). |
17 | See http://markturner.org/blending.html (accessed on 10 June 2022). |
18 | |
19 | Bickerton (2009, p. 125) mentions the four great apes, Californian sea lions, Atlantic bottlenose dolphins, and African grey parrots. Barrett (2018, p. 260) points out that “words do not seem to be intrinsically part of most apes’ affective niche, as they are for typical human babies. To apes, words alone are not worth learning.” |
20 | Bickerton slightly modified his terminology in his more recent (and last) book More than Nature Needs (Bickerton 2014). Here, he uses the term “concept” also in connection with nonhuman animals, but asserts that there is a difference between nonhuman and human concepts. He still maintains “that a certain kind of thought probably limited to humans—thought that manipulates concepts of classes rather than of individual entities, that can transcend experience to create genuinely novel configurations—needed some kind of overt objects (signals or words) in order to get started.” (p. 103). Our conclusion that the experience of prelinguistic children is fundamentally different from ours remains the same. |
21 | It appears “that a unique context and selective pressure were responsible for the evolution of human language, given that no other primates have yet evolved a language-like communication system despite the fact that, arguably, they have the basic cognitive skills required and a similar genetic background to humans” (Számadó and Szathmáry 2006, p. 556). For other attempts, see, e.g., Laland (2017, chp. 8); Planer and Sterelny (2021). All accounts of the invention of words seem to fit in well with the notion of joint attention as a feature distinguishing humanity from other primates, as proclaimed by Tomasello (e.g., Tomasello 2019) and others. |
22 | See, e.g., Caldwell-Harris (2019); Lupyan et al. (2020). For a discussion of the Sapir–Whorf hypothesis, see Leavitt (2014); O’Neill (2015). Evans (2014, p. 215 ff.) summarizes experiments that show that language already influences perception at pre-conscious and non-linguistic levels. |
23 | Without necessarily subscribing to his philosophy, Wittgenstein’s famous statements—“the limits of my language mean the limits of my world” and “whereof one cannot speak, thereof one must be silent”—come to mind (Wittgenstein 1922, pp. 5–7). |
24 | Losing one’s ability to use language, aphasia, does not take one back to a pre-linguistic state. One reason may be that “[i]n 99 per cent of aphasics the processing of language is damaged, but the memory for language is retained” (Hale 2007, p. 124, quoting Richard Wise). |
25 | Note that there was not always a word to designate states of absorption: “the Oxford English Dictionary indicates that written English usage of the term [‘absorption’] to signify engrossment only became commonplace in the mid seventeenth century (along with the term ‘immersion’)” (Herbert 2019, p. 237). |
26 | Cp. Hood et al. (2009, pp. 354–55): “the wide diversity of triggers or conditions facilitating mystical experiences … may have in common the fact that an individual fascinated by any given trigger experiences a momentary loss of sense of self, being ‘absorbed’ or ‘fascinated’ by his or her object of perception.” |
27 | “Mystical experiences are absorbed states.” (Granqvist 2020, pp. 219–20). |
28 | There are many theories of consciousness. LeDoux (2015, pp. 146–79) provides a useful survey. See also Cobb (2020, chp. 15) (“Consciousness”). All these theories share a “commitment … to the proposition that the brain is the centre, cause, operating system, and seat of consciousness, mind, and mental life” (Grayling 2021, pp. 255–56). |
29 | For a recent discussion between Seth and Solms, see https://npsa-association.org/anil-seth-mark-solms-dialogue/ (accessed on 10 June 2022). |
30 | One might add Jaak Panksepp (e.g., Panksepp 1998; Davis and Panksepp 2018). |
31 | “The non-feeling, ‘precise’ contents of the mind flow with distinction, silhouetted against the affect process, a bit like acting figurines against an animated backdrop.” (Damasio 2021, pp. 78–79). |
32 | Seth (2021, p. 218) goes further: “At the very deepest layers of the self, beneath even emotions and moods, there lies a cognitively subterranean, inchoate, difficult-to-describe experience of simply being a living organism. Here, experiences of selfhood emerge in the unstructured feeling of just ‘being’.” On p. 220 he says: “the very deepest levels of experienced selfhood—the inchoate feeling of ‘just being’—seem to lack … external referents altogether. This, for me, is the true ground-state of conscious selfhood: a formless, shapeless, control-oriented perceptual prediction about the present and future physiological condition of the body itself.” Elsewhere (Webb 2022, p. 96), he describes the most basic aspect of conscious selfhood as “at the deepest level without any describable content at all.” |
33 | This suggests that brain injuries that affect such associations may result in similar experiences; see, on this, Cristofori et al. (2016). |
34 | http://www.bodysoulandspirit.net/mystical_experiences/ (accessed on 10 June 2022). |
35 | Carhart-Harris and Friston (2019, p. 319) mention “subjective phenomena associated with the psychedelic experience, including the following: ego dissolution…, … altered time perception …, a sense of the ineffable …” |
36 | This is not to deny the importance of feelings, both for the mystic and from a theoretical point of view (see above). Already, William James ([1902] 1920, p. 380) stated: “… mystical states are more like states of feeling than like states of intellect. No one can make clear to another who has never had a certain feeling, in what the quality or worth of it consists.” See also Matilal (1975, pp. 218–219); Gäb (2021, p. 235). |
37 | |
38 | E.g., (de Jong 1977), Nāgārjuna’s Mūlamadhyamakakārikā, 7.17: “If there existed anywhere something unarisen, it could arise. Since no such thing exists, what is it that arises?” |
39 | “Presumably”, because there is, of course, no way to prove that these three (Eckhart, Upanishads, Nagarjuna) had had no mystical experience. However, if not these three, there were, no doubt, others who had not. |
40 | As pointed out above, some (or even many) of these teachings can, at least in part, be explained in terms of the intellectual and cultural surroundings of their authors. However, the appeal of “mystical ideas” may have influenced the shape in which we find them in the relevant texts. |
41 | Is this what Damasio meant when he said: “In and of themselves, feelings are never memorized and thus cannot be recollected” (Damasio 2019, p. 141)? Clearly Damasio does not deny access to pure feeling: “The ebb and flow of spontaneous homeostatic feelings provides for an ever-present background, a more or less pure sense of being of the sort that those who practice meditation aspire to experience.” (cited above). |
42 | This implies that young children do not need absorption to remove the factors responsible for standard consciousness; those factors, quite simply, were not yet there. |
43 | Some scholars—most notably, Morrison and Conway (2010)—“consider a version of episodic memory (‘sensory-perceptive-affective’), which appears very early in life, and another version (‘conceptual episodic memory’), which appears later; … autobiographical is … considered by them to appear even later” (Staniloiu et al. 2020, p. 5). |
44 | It does not, to be sure, recall all the conscious experiences we ever had. |
45 | |
46 | If I am not mistaken, this opinion has not been contradicted in subsequent research; see Renoult and Rugg (2020). Rugg and Vilberg (2013) draw attention to “a content-independent network that acts in concert with cortical regions representing the contents of retrieval”. Could it be that the content-independent network remembers the experience itself? |
47 | Note that the memory we are interested in has nothing to do with remembering facts, people, things, relationships, and places, which is liable to infantile amnesia (on which see Alberini and Travaglia 2017; Peterson 2020). |
48 | Cf. Slingerland (2021, p. 97): “[A] common theme in cultures from across the world and throughout history is the idea of spiritual or moral perfection as somehow involving regaining the child’s mind. The Gospel of Matthew declares, ‘Truly I tell you, unless you change and become like little children, you will never enter the kingdom of heaven.’ An early Chinese Daoist text, the Daodejing or Laozi, compares the perfected sage to an infant or small child, perfectly open and receptive to the world.” |
49 | For a recent review of the evidence, see Zarrindat and Khakpai (2020); further Taylor (2013, pp. 1079–80). |
50 | Cf. Ott (2007, p. 262): “standards for the definition and assessment of states of absorption have not been established thus far.” See also Mohr (2018, pp. 118–19). Depth of absorption should not be confused with levels of consciousness, on which see Bayne et al. (2016). Is it possible that the correlation between pupil dilution and (depth of?) attention may open up ways to measure depth of absorption? See Zhao et al. (2019). |
51 | Hypnosis is one method and, unsurprisingly, “[f]orty per cent of hypnotized subjects describe it as an altered state of consciousness, while sixty per cent compare it to a period of focused attention” (Dietrich 2007, p. 269). |
52 | i.e., activities that are “deemed religious” (Taves 2009). |
53 | On different traditions of meditation in ancient India, see Bronkhorst (1993). On Asian traditions of meditation in general, see Eifring (2016). |
54 | Interestingly, there are reasons to think that psychedelics can help to attain far deeper states; see below. |
55 | Cf., e.g., Siegel (2005). Slingerland (2021) discusses the social consequences of the ingestion of certain drugs. |
56 | “Research reveals that mystical experiences are relatively common in the general population. Averaging across samples, nations, and methods, colleagues (Hood et al. 2009) estimate their lifetime prevalence to be roughly 35%.” (Granqvist 2020, p. 219). These numbers may easily make us overlook that mystical experiences may be more or less “deep”. |
57 | Cf. Bronkhorst (2022). Interestingly, Tellegen and Atkinson, the originators of the Tellegen Absorption Scale, already distinguish between the two, in the title of their relevant article (Tellegen and Atkinson 1974): “Openness to absorbing and self-altering experiences (‘absorption’), a trait related to hypnotic susceptibility.” Here, the trait is openness, the state is the experience. The word absorption is here used to refer to the experiences, not to the trait. Yet, “[i]n discussing imaginative involvement and absorption, for instance, … Tellegen … sometimes blurred together comments on trait and state notions of absorption” (Roche and McConkey 1990, p. 92). Note further that Mattes (2022, p. 6) speaks of “the widespread confusion between enabling conditions of flow and the flow experience itself.” |
58 | Assuming that many scholars are “normal” or “average”, this may throw light on the fact that some of them find it difficult to take mystical experience seriously. Indeed, “for a long time, extraordinary consciousness experiences have either been ignored by the mainstream natural sciences or have been explicitly denigrated as nonexistent—as the fantasies of cranks.” (Wittmann 2018, p. 2769). |
59 | More can no doubt be added. Lewis-Williams (2010, p. 143) adds auditory driving (e.g., chanting, clapping, drumming); electrical stimulation; flickering light; fatigue; hunger; sensory deprivation; stress; and extreme pain to this list. Maij and van Elk (2018, p. 1) enumerate sensory over-stimulation, sensory deprivation, mind-altering substances, magic tricks, extreme rituals, meditation practices, trying to recall memories of past mystical experiences, expectancy manipulations, and the so-called “God Helmet”. |
60 | For recent research on meditation combined with psychedelics, see, e.g., Griffiths et al. (2017); Smigielski et al. (2019); Heuschkel and Kuypers (2020); Eleftheriou and Thomas (2021). Mention should also be made of the Good Friday Experiment designed by Walter N. Pahnke and carried out in 1962. Psilocybin was administered to theology students in a prayer chapel. For details, see (Wittmann 2018, p. 2226; Baier 2021, pp. 382–88). Psychedelics have also found their way into Buddhist meditation: https://www.lionsroar.com/the-new-wave-of-psychedelics-in-buddhist-practice/ (accessed on 10 June 2022); (Osto 2016). |
61 | This “Copernican” reversal of perspectives has been preceded—in Kenneth Rose’s (2016, p. 62) terms—by the “anti-Copernican” reversal of perspectives in the study of mysticism instituted by Steven Katz more than forty years ago; see further below. |
62 | This refers to Katz’s influential article of 1978 (Katz 1978) and subsequent publications. |
63 | Katz’s claim that pure, unmediated experience does not exist, initially presented as an assumption or even a fact, became, over time, a working hypothesis and an epistemological generalization (Hammersholt 2013, p. 476). Katz does not present anything resembling proof for this claim which, in view of the theory presented in the present article, looks like a totally unwarranted assumption. |
References
- Alberini, Cristina M., and Alessio Travaglia. 2017. Infantile amnesia: A critical period of learning to learn and remember. The Journal of Neuroscience 37: 5783–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Baier, Karl. 2021. High Mysticism: On the Interplay between the Psychedelic Movement and Academic Study of Mysticism. In Constructions of Mysticism as a Universal. Roots and Interactions Across Borders. Wiesbaden: Harrassowitz, pp. 363–96. [Google Scholar]
- Barrett, Lisa Feldman. 2018. How Emotions Are Made. The Secret Life of the Brain. New York: Mariner Books. [Google Scholar]
- Bayne, Tim, Jakob Hohwy, and Adrian M. Owen. 2016. Are there levels of consciousness? Trends in Cognitive Sciences 20: 405–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Berkovich-Ohana, Aviva, Yair Dor-Ziderman, Joseph Glicksohn, and Abraham Goldstein. 2013. Alterations in the sense of time, space, and body in the mindfulness-trained brain: A neurophenomenologically-guided MEG study. Frontiers in Psychology 4: 912. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Bickerton, Derek. 2009. Adam’s Tongue. How Humans Made Language, How Language Made Humans. New York: Hill and Wang. [Google Scholar]
- Bickerton, Derek. 2014. More Than Nature Needs: Language, Mind, and Evolution. Cambridge: Harvard University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Blunden, Andy. 2012. Concepts: A Critical Approach. Leiden and Boston: Brill. [Google Scholar]
- Bronkhorst, Johannes. 1993. The Two Traditions of Meditation in Ancient India, 2nd ed. Delhi: Motilal Banarsidass, Reprint 2000. [Google Scholar]
- Bronkhorst, Johannes. 2011. Language and Reality: On an Episode in Indian Thought. Leiden and Boston: Brill. [Google Scholar]
- Bronkhorst, Johannes. 2017. Can religion be explained? The role of absorption in various religious phenomena. Method and Theory in the Study of Religion 29: 1–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Bronkhorst, Johannes. 2021. The religious predisposition. Method and Theory in the Study of Religion 33: 187–227. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bronkhorst, Johannes. 2022. The role of absorption in making God real, Commentary on Luhrmann 2020. Religion, Brain & Behavior. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Buonomano, Dean. 2017. Your Brain Is a Time Machine. The Neuroscience and Physics of Time. New York and London: W. W. Norton. [Google Scholar]
- Caldwell-Harris, Catherine L. 2019. Our language affects what we see. Scientific American. January 15. Available online: https://www.scientificamerican.com/article/our-language-affects-what-we-see/ (accessed on 10 June 2022).
- Candland, Douglas Keith. 1993. Feral Children and Clever Animals: Reflections on Human Nature. New York and Oxford: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Carhart-Harris, Robin L., and Karl J. Friston. 2019. REBUS and the anarchic brain: Toward a unified model of the brain action of psychedelics. Pharmacological Reviews 71: 316–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Carroll, Lewis. 1872. Through the Looking-Glass, and What Alice Found There. London: Macmillan. [Google Scholar]
- Christiansen, Morten H., and Nick Chater. 2022. The Language Game: How Improvisation Created Language and Changed the World. London: Bantam Press. [Google Scholar]
- Cobb, Matthew. 2020. The Idea of the Brain. A History. London: Profile Books. [Google Scholar]
- Cristofori, Irene, Bulbulia Joseph, John H. Shaver, Marc Wilson, Frank Krueger, and Jordan Grafman. 2016. Neural correlates of mystical experience. Neuropsychologia 80: 212–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Damasio, Antonio. 2019. The Strange Order of Things: Life, Feeling, and the Making of Cultures. New York: Vintage. [Google Scholar]
- Damasio, Antonio. 2021. Feeling & Knowing: Making Minds Conscious. New York: Pantheon. [Google Scholar]
- Darwin, Charles. 1872. The Expression of the Emotions in Man and Animals. London: John Murray. [Google Scholar]
- Davis, Kenneth L., and Jaak Panksepp. 2018. The Emotional Foundations of Personality. A Neurobiological and Evolutionary Approach. New York and London: W. W. Norton. [Google Scholar]
- de Jong, J. W., ed. 1977. Nāgārjuna: Mūlamadhyamakakārikā. Madras: The Adyar Library and Research Centre. [Google Scholar]
- Dietrich, Arne. 2007. Introduction to Consciousness: Neuroscience, Cognitive Science, and Philosophy. Basingstoke and New York: Palgrave Macmillan. [Google Scholar]
- Donald, Merlin. 2001. A Mind So Rare: The Evolution of Human Consciousness. New York and London: W. W. Norton. [Google Scholar]
- Drissi-Daoudi, Leila, Adrien Doerig, and Michael H. Herzog. 2019. Feature integration within discrete time windows. Nature Communications 10: 4901. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Ebbinghaus, Hermann. 1913. Über das Gedächtnis. Untersuchungen zur experimentellen Psychologie. Leipzig: Duncker und Humblot. First published 1885. [Google Scholar]
- Eifring, Halvor. 2016. Asian Traditions of Meditation. Honolulu: University of Hawaii Press. [Google Scholar]
- Eleftheriou, Maria Eleni, and Emily Thomas. 2021. Examining the potential synergistic effects between mindfulness training and psychedelic-assisted therapy. Frontiers in Psychiatry 12: 707057. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Evans, Vyvyan. 2014. The Language Myth: Why Language Is Not an Instinct. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Fauconnier, Gilles, and Mark Turner. 2002. The Way We Think: Conceptual Blending and the Mind’s Hidden Complexities. New York: Basic Books. [Google Scholar]
- Fauconnier, Gilles, and Mark Turner. 2008. The Origin of Language as a Product of the Evolution of Modern Cognition; Edited by Bernard Laks. London and Oakville: Equinox, pp. 133–56. Available online: https://ssrn.com/abstract=1556533 (accessed on 10 June 2022).
- Franco, Eli. 2018. On the Arising of Philosophical Theories from Spiritual Practice. In Saddharmāmṛtam: Festschrift für Jens-Uwe Hartmann zum 65. Geburtstag. Edited by Oliver von Criegern, Gudrun Melzer and Johannes Schneider. Wien: Arbeitskreis für Tibetische und Buddhistische Studien Universität Wien, pp. 113–26. [Google Scholar]
- Gäb, Sebastian. 2021. Mysticism without concepts. International Journal for Philosophy of Religion 90: 233–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Garner, Aleena R., and Georg B. Keller. 2022. A cortical circuit for audio-visual predictions. Nature Neuroscience 25: 98–105. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gelman, Susan A. 2003. The Essential Child: Origins of Essentialism in Everyday Thought. Oxford: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Goldin-Meadow, Susan. 2005. The Resilience of Language: What Gesture Creation in Deaf Children Can Tell Us about How All Children Learn Language. New York: Psychology Press. [Google Scholar]
- Gopnik, Alison. 2009. The Philosophical Baby. What Children’s Minds Tell Us About Truth, Love, and the Meaning of Life. New York: Farrar, Straus and Giroux. [Google Scholar]
- Granqvist, Pehr. 2020. Attachment in Religion and Spirituality: A Wider View. New York and London: Guilford. [Google Scholar]
- Grayling, A. C. 2021. The Frontiers of Knowledge. What We Know about Science, History and the Mind. New York: Viking. [Google Scholar]
- Griffiths, Roland R., Matthew W. Johnson, William A. Richards, Brian D. Richards, Robert Jesse, Katherine A. MacLean, Frederick S. Barrett, Mary P. Cosimano, and Maggie A. Klinedinst. 2017. Psilocybin-occasioned mystical-type experience in combination with meditation and other spiritual practices produces enduring positive changes in psychological functioning and in trait measures of prosocial attitudes and behaviors. Journal of Psychopharmacology 32: 49–69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Hale, Sheila. 2007. The Man Who Lost His Language. A Case of Aphasia, Revised ed. London and Philadelphia: Jessica Kingsley. [Google Scholar]
- Hammersholt, Torben. 2013. Steven T. Katz’s philosophy of mysticism revisited. Journal of the American Academy of Religion 81: 467–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Herbert, Ruth. 2019. Absorption and openness to experience: An everyday tale of traits, states, and consciousness change with music. In Music and Consciousness 2: Worlds, Practices, Modalities. Edited by Ruth Herbert, David Clarke and Eric Clarke. Oxford: University of Oxford, pp. 233–53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Herzog, Michael H., Thomas Kammer, and Frank Scharnowski. 2016. Time slices: What is the duration of a percept? PLoS Biology 14: e1002433. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Heuschkel, Kristin, and Kim P. C. Kuypers. 2020. Depression, mindfulness, and psilocybin: Possible complementary effects of mindfulness meditation and psilocybin in the treatment of depression. A review. Frontiers in Psychiatry 11: 224. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Hood, Ralph W. 1975. The construction and preliminary validation of a measure of reported mystical experience. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion 14: 29–41, Reprint 2001. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hood, Bruce. 2011. The Self Illusion. London: Constable. [Google Scholar]
- Hood, Ralph W., Peter C. Hill, and Bernard Spika. 2009. The Psychology of Religion: An Empirical Approach, 4th ed. New York: Guilford. [Google Scholar]
- James, William. 1920. The Varieties of Religious Experience: A Study in Human Nature. Thirty-Second Impression. New York: Longmans, Green, and Co. First published 1902. [Google Scholar]
- Jamieson, Graham A. 2005. The modified Tellegen absorption scale: A clearer window on the structure and meaning of absorption. Australian Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis 33: 119–39. [Google Scholar]
- Jensen, Jeppe Sinding. 2011. Framing Religious Narrative, Cognition and Culture Theoretically. Edited by Armin W. Geertz and J. Sinding Jensen. London: Equinox Publishing, pp. 31–50. [Google Scholar]
- Johansson, Sverker. 2021. The Dawn of Language: How We Came to Talk. London: MacLehose Press. [Google Scholar]
- Johnson, Matthew W., Peter S. Hendricks, Frederick S. Barrett, and Roland R. Griffiths. 2019. Classic psychedelics: An integrative review of epidemiology, therapeutics, mystical experience, and brain network function. Pharmacology & Therapeutics 197: 83–102. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Katz, Steven T. 1978. Language, epistemology, and mysticism. In Mysticism and Philosophical Analysis. Edited by Steven T. Katz. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 22–74. [Google Scholar]
- Keller, Helen. 1910. The World I Live In. New York: The Century. [Google Scholar]
- Kügler, Denys. 2004. Denys Turner’s anti-mystical mystical theology. Ars Disputandi 4: 176–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Laland, Kevin N. 2017. Darwin’s Unfinished Symphony. How Culture Made the Human Mind. Princeton and Oxford: Princeton University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Lau, Hakwan, Matthias Michel, Joseph E. LeDoux, and Stephen M. Fleming. 2022. The mnemonic basis of subjective experience. Nature Reviews Psychology. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Leavitt, John. 2014. Linguistic relativity: Precursors and transformations. In The Routledge Handbook of Language and Culture. Edited by Farzad Sharifian. London and New York: Routledge, pp. 18–30. [Google Scholar]
- LeDoux, Joseph. 2015. Anxious: The Modern Mind in the Age of Anxiety. London: One World Publications. [Google Scholar]
- Lewis-Williams, David. 2010. Conceiving God: The Cognitive Origin and Evolution of Religion. London: Thames & Hudson. [Google Scholar]
- Lifshitz, Michael, Michiel van Elk, and Tanya M. Luhrmann. 2019. Absorption and spiritual experience: A review of evidence and potential mechanisms. Consciousness and Cognition 73: 102760. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lindquist, Kristen A. 2021. Language and emotion: Introduction to the special issue. Affective Science 2: 91–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Lindquist, Kristen A., Lisa Feldman Barrett, and Liza Bliss-Moreau. 2006. Language and the perception of emotion. Emotion 6: 125–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Luhrmann, T. M. 2020. How God Becomes Real: Kindling the Presence of Invisible Others. Princeton: Princeton University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Lupyan, Gary, Rasha Abdel Rahman, Lera Boroditsky, and Andy Clark. 2020. Effects of language on visual perception. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 24: 930–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Maij, David L. R., and Michiel van Elk. 2018. Getting absorbed in experimentally induced extraordinary experiences: Effects of placebo brain stimulation on agency detection. Consciousness and Cognition 66: 1–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Manassi, Mauro, and David Whitney. 2022. Illusion of visual stability through active perceptual serial dependence. Science Advances 8: eabk2480. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Mashour, George A., Pieter Roelfsema, Jean-Pierre Changeux, and Stanislas Dehaene. 2020. Conscious processing and the global neuronal workspace hypothesis. Neuron 105: 776–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Matilal, B. 1975. Mysticism and reality: Ineffability. Journal of Indian Philosophy 3: 217–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mattes, Josef. 2022. Positive psychology and philosophy-as-usual: An unhappy match? Philosophies 7: 52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mohr, Christine. 2018. Are there varying depths in flow? Journal of Consciousness Studies 25: 115–30. [Google Scholar]
- Morrison, Catriona M., and Martin A. Conway. 2010. First words and first memories. Cognition 116: 23–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Olivelle, Patrick. 1998. The Early Upaniṣads. Annotated Text and Translation. New York and Oxford: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- O’Neill, Sean P. 2015. The Sapir-Whorf hypothesis. In The International Encyclopedia of Language and Social Interaction. Hoboken: Wiley Online Library. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Osto, Douglas. 2016. Altered States: Buddhism and Psychedelic Spirituality in America. New York: Columbia University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Osto, Douglas E. 2019. Altered states and the origins of the Mahāyāna. In Setting Out on the Great Way: Essays on Early Mahayana Buddhism. Edited by Paul Harrison. London and Oakville: Equinox, pp. 177–205. [Google Scholar]
- Ott, Ulrich. 2007. States of absorption: In search of neurobiological foundations. In Hypnosis and Conscious States: The Cognitive Neuroscience Perspective. Edited by Graham A. Jamieson. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 257–70. [Google Scholar]
- Panksepp, Jaak. 1998. Affective Neuroscience: The Foundations of Human and Animal Emotions. New York and Oxford: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Peterson, Carole. 2020. Remembering earliest childhood memories. In Autobiographical Memory Development: Theoretical and Methodological Approaches. Edited by Sami Gülgöz and Basak Sahin-Acar. London: Routledge, pp. 119–35. [Google Scholar]
- Planer, Ronald J., and Kim Sterelny. 2021. From Signal to Symbol: The Evolution of Language. Cambridge and London: The MIT Press. [Google Scholar]
- Pyers, Jennie, and Ann Senghas. 2020. Lexical iconicity is differentially favored under transmission in a new sign language: The effect of type of iconicity. Sign Language & Linguistics 23: 73–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Radulovic, Jelena, Royce Lee, and Andrew Ortony. 2018. State-dependent memory: Neurobiological advances and prospects for translation to dissociative amnesia. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience 12: 259. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Renoult, Louis, and Michael D. Rugg. 2020. An historical perspective on Endel Tulving’s episodic-semantic distinction. Neuropsychologia 139: 107366. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Roche, Suzanne M., and Kevin M. McConkey. 1990. Absorption: Nature, assessment, and correlates. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 59: 91–101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rose, Kenneth. 2016. Yoga, Meditation, and Mysticism: Contemplative Universals and Meditative Landmarks. London: Bloombury. [Google Scholar]
- Rugg, Michael D., and Kaia L. Vilberg. 2013. Brain networks underlying episodic memory retrieval. Current Opinion in Neurobiology 23: 255–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Scharnowski, Frank, Johannes Rüter, Jacob Jolij, Frouke Hermens, Thomas Kammer, and Michael H. Herzog. 2009. Long-lasting modulation of feature integration by transcranial magnetic stimulation. Journal of Vision 9: 1–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sergent, Claire, Valentin Wyart, Mariana Babo-Rebelo, Laurent Cohen, Lionel Naccache, and Catherine Tallon-Baudry. 2013. Cueing attention after the stimulus is gone can retrospectively trigger conscious perception. Current Biology 23: 150–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Seth, Anil. 2021. Being You: A New Science of Consciousness. London: Faber & Faber. [Google Scholar]
- Sharf, Robert H. 1995. Buddhist modernism and the rhetoric of meditative experience. Numen 42: 228–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Sharf, Robert. 2000. The rhetoric of experience and the study of religion. Journal of Consciousness Studies 7: 267–87. [Google Scholar]
- Siegel, Ronnald K. 2005. Intoxication: The Universal Drive for Mind-Altering Substances. Rochester: Park Street Press. [Google Scholar]
- Slingerland, Edward. 2021. Drunk: How We Sipped, Danced, and Stumbled Our Way to Civilization. New York, Boston and London: Little, Brown Spark. [Google Scholar]
- Smart, Ninian. 1965. Interpretation and mystical experience. Religious Studies 1: 75–87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Smart, Ninian. 1967. Mysticism, History of. Available online: https://www.encyclopedia.com/humanities/encyclopedias-almanacs-transcripts-and-maps/mysticism-history (accessed on 10 June 2022).
- Smigielski, Lukasz, Milan Scheidegger, Michael Kometer, and Franz X. Vollenweider. 2019. Psilocybin-assisted mindfulness training modulates self-consciousness and brain default network connectivity with lasting effects. NeuroImage 196: 207–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Soffer-Dudek, Nirit. 2019. Dissociative absorption, mind-wandering, and attention-deficit symptoms: Associations with obsessive-compulsive symptoms. British Journal of Clinical Psychology 58: 51–69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Solms, Mark. 2021. The Hidden Spring: A Journey to the Source of Consciousness. London: Profile Books. [Google Scholar]
- Stace, Walter Terence. 1960. Mysticism and Philosophy. London: MacMillan & Co. [Google Scholar]
- Staniloiu, Angelica, Andreas Kordon, and Hans J. Markowitsch. 2020. Quo vadis ‘episodic memory’?—Past, present, and perspective. Neuropsychologia 141: 107362. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Streib, Heinz, Constantin Klein, Barbara Keller, and Ralph W. Hood. 2021. The mysticism scale as measure for subjective spirituality: New results with Hood’s M-scale and the development of a short form. In Assessing Spirituality in a Diverse World. Edited by Amy L. Ai, Paul Wink, Raymond F. Paloutzian and Kevin A. Harris. New York: Springer, pp. 467–91. [Google Scholar]
- Számadó, Szabolcs, and Eörs Szathmáry. 2006. Selective scenarios for the emergence of natural languages. Trends in Ecology and Evolution 21: 555–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Taves, Ann. 2009. Religious Experience Reconsidered: A Building-Block Approach to the Study of Religion and Other Special Things. Princeton and Oxford: Princeton University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Taylor, Annette Kujawski, ed. 2013. Encyclopedia of Human Memory. 3 vols. Santa Barbara, Denver and Oxford: Greenwood. [Google Scholar]
- Tellegen, Auke, and Gilbert Atkinson. 1974. Openness to absorbing and self-altering experiences (‘absorption’), a trait related to hypnotic susceptibility. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 83: 268–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tomasello, Michael. 2019. Becoming Human: A Theory of Ontogeny. Cambridge and London: The Belknap Press of Harvard University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Tulving, Endel. 1985. Memory and consciousness. Canadian Psychology/Psychologie Canadienne 26: 1–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Turner, Denys. 1995. The Darkness of God: Negativity in Christian mysticism. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Turner, Mark. 2014. The Origin of Ideas. Blending, Creativity, and the Human Spark. Oxford: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Vygotsky, L. S. 1987–1999. Collected Works. 6 vols. Edited by Robert W. Rieber and Aaron S. Carton. New York and London: Plenum. [Google Scholar]
- Webb, Richard, ed. 2022. New Scientist Essential Guide No. 12: Consciousness. Understanding the Ghost in the Machine. London: New Scientist. [Google Scholar]
- Wilke, Annette, Robert Stephanus, and Robert Suckro, eds. 2021. Constructions of Mysticism as a Universal. Roots and Interactions Across Borders. Wiesbaden: Harrassowitz. [Google Scholar]
- Wittgenstein, Ludwig. 1922. Tractatus Logico-Philosophicus. London: Kegan Paul, Trench, Trubner. [Google Scholar]
- Wittmann, Marc. 2013. The inner sense of time: How the brain creates a representation of duration. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 14: 217–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wittmann, Marc. 2018. Altered States of Consciousness: Experiences out of Time and Self. Cambridge: MIT Press, (Original title: Wenn die Zeit stehen bleibt.). [Google Scholar]
- Wulff, David M. 2014. Mystical experiences. In Varieties of Anomalous Experience: Examining the Scientific Evidence, 2nd ed. Edited by Etzel Cardeña, Steven Jay Lynn and Stanley Krippner. Washington: American Psychological Association, pp. 369–408. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zaehner, Robert Charles. 1957. Mysticism, Sacred and Profane: An Inquiry into Some Varieties of Praeternatural Experience. Oxford: Clarendon Press. [Google Scholar]
- Zarrabi-Zadeh, Saeed. 2008. Defining mysticism, a survey of main definitions. Transcendent Philosophy 9: 77–92. [Google Scholar]
- Zarrindat, Mohammad-Reza, and Fatemen Khakpai. 2020. State-dependent memory and its modulation by different brain areas and neurotransmitters. EXCLO Journal 19: 1081–99. [Google Scholar]
- Zhao, Sijia, Gabriela Bury, Alice Milne, and Maria Chait. 2019. Pupillometry as an objective measure of sustained attention in young and older listeners. Trends in Hearing 23: 1–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. |
© 2022 by the author. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Bronkhorst, J. Mystical Experience. Religions 2022, 13, 589. https://doi.org/10.3390/rel13070589
Bronkhorst J. Mystical Experience. Religions. 2022; 13(7):589. https://doi.org/10.3390/rel13070589
Chicago/Turabian StyleBronkhorst, Johannes. 2022. "Mystical Experience" Religions 13, no. 7: 589. https://doi.org/10.3390/rel13070589
APA StyleBronkhorst, J. (2022). Mystical Experience. Religions, 13(7), 589. https://doi.org/10.3390/rel13070589