Next Article in Journal
Between Rocks and ‘High Places’: On Religious Architecture in the Iron Age Southern Levant
Previous Article in Journal
Saints, Sacred Trees, and Snakes: Popular Religion, Hierotopy, Byzantine Culture, and Insularity in Cyprus during the Long Middle Ages
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Insecure Attachment to God and Interpersonal Conflict

Department of Psychology, Indiana State University, Terre Haute, IN 47809, USA
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Religions 2021, 12(9), 739; https://doi.org/10.3390/rel12090739
Submission received: 28 July 2021 / Revised: 2 September 2021 / Accepted: 3 September 2021 / Published: 9 September 2021

Abstract

:
Research has expanded the notion of attachment to caregivers to other figures such as God, and there is now literature supporting positive effects of attachment to God with various psychosocial outcomes. The dimensions of attachment to God—anxiety and avoidance—reflect varying ways that people see God as supportive and reliable versus unsupportive and inconsistent. As a stable aspect of the individual, attachment to God results in recurring patterns of interpersonal behavior that can maintain and support self-control or disrupt it. No studies have examined the moderating effect of attachment to God on the relationship between self-control and negative social exchanges. To fill this gap, a sample of 1049 adults across the United States completed measures on attachment to God, self-control, and interpersonal outcomes. First, results showed that insecure attachment to God is associated with a hostile-dominant interpersonal style. Second, it was found that the highest level of negative social exchanges occurred in individuals low in self-control and high in attachment avoidance and anxiety. Results are discussed in terms of self-regulation, stress exposure, and situation selection. An implication of the current study is that secure attachment to God may foster less stress exposure by influencing a person’s situation selection.

1. Introduction

Secure attachment to caregivers and romantic partners has a host of benefits for psychosocial outcomes and health (Hankin et al. 2005; Rosenstein and Horowitz 1996). Developmentally, attachment-related processes influence the capacity to self-regulate, including enhancing self-control and limiting impulsivity. According to the interpersonal perspective of personality, social, and clinical psychology, attachment is an individual difference variable that not only resides in the person but is also an individual difference variable that results in recurring patterns of social exchanges that benefit or harm the individual. More recently, the research literature has recognized that attachment can also occur with God or a higher power. Similar to its secular variants, attachment to God is also associated with a number of health-promoting outcomes (Granqvist and Kirkpatrick 2013).
As it relates to its role in promoting self-regulation, there have been quite a few studies demonstrating a significant relationship between insecure attachment and low self-control in children (Burgess et al. 2003; DeKlyen et al. 1998; Gilliom et al. 2002), and a few such studies in adults (Orehek et al. 2017; Valikhani et al. 2018). Trait self-control is associated with many outcomes, including interpersonal success, academic achievement, prosocial behavior, well-being, and longevity. Similarly, aspects of religion such as (most notably) church attendance have been associated with positive outcomes including academic achievement, volunteering, social support, and longevity (Hill et al. 2019; McCullough et al. 2000; Petrovic et al. 2021; Phillips and Kitchens 2016). Religiousness is associated with self-control, perhaps due to behavioral factors such as religious rituals that develop the self-control “muscle” (Baumeister and Alquist 2009) and cognitive factors such as sanctifying goals to promote “waiting, tolerating, and cooperating” (McCullough and Carter 2011, p. 423) to successfully monitor and change one’s behavior toward greater conformity with a desired end state or goal. One way that this might occur is through establishing recurring patterns of interpersonal exchanges that place one on a trajectory of interpersonal success associated with positive outcomes. Our research has shown that various components of religiousness and spirituality have relevant interpersonal correlates with known health benefits (Jordan et al. 2014).
Despite the conceptual likelihood that attachment to God influences self-regulation and specifically self-control, there have been few studies directly focused on this (McCullough and Carter 2013). As a resource that can be depleted, it is important to know what factors may play a protective role in maintaining the positive effects of self-control. Attachment to God may do this by differentially impacting the relationship between self-control and interpersonal dysfunction. In the current paper, we first examine the characteristic interpersonal style associated with attachment to God. We then examine whether the two dimensions of attachment to God, anxiety and avoidance, influence the association between trait self-control and negative social exchanges.

1.1. An Interpersonal Conceptualization of Religiousness

While there have been many conceptualizations of religiousness that are more individually focused (perhaps best represented by the quote of Whitehead (1926, p. 47): “Religion is what the individual does with his solitariness”), greater attention—or re-recognition of what has always been known—has been paid to the social aspects of religiousness. The interpersonal perspective of personality, social, and clinical psychology includes a structural model of interpersonal behavior (i.e., the Interpersonal Circumplex) that conceptualizes stable differences in a person’s interpersonal style (Pincus and Ansell 2013). Social behavior varies along the dimensions of affiliation (friendly versus hostile) and control (dominant versus submissive). Coinciding with this component is a process model of social transactions (i.e., dominant behavior pulls for submissive behavior and friendly behavior pulls for friendly behavior). The implications for social outcomes are obvious in that social behaviors on the right side of the Interpersonal Circumplex (IPC) are generally associated with salubrious outcomes, while those on the left have deleterious consequences. This happens because transactional cycles occur over time, and to the extent that these are positive, it is likely that self-regulatory reserves are maintained.
One variable that might promote transactional cycles leading to health is attachment to God. Attachment theory is often utilized as a framework for studying emotion and self-regulation and mental health in children (Mikulincer and Shaver 2012a, 2012b). In Bowlby’s early writings, he stated that it is essential for infants to experience a warm, consistent, and satisfying relationship with a mother, or caregiver substitute, to develop good mental health (Bowlby 1951). According to Bowlby, an infant’s future mental health, social functioning, and interpersonal relationships will likely be negatively impacted by the absence of this type of care (Bowlby 1951, 1982). Paralleling the extension of childhood attachment to adult intimate relationships, attachment theorists have further broadened the scope of attachment by applying it to supernatural figures (Granqvist and Kirkpatrick 2013). Kirkpatrick (1992) pioneered this extension, and argued that God functions as an attachment figure because people seek proximity to God, people experience distress when God seems distant, and people turn to God in times of trouble. In addition, he argued that God functions as a secure base and “felt security” for people. Attachment to God has been receiving more research attention, with studies suggesting that for many people God does serve as a safe haven and a secure base (Beck 2006; Limke and Mayfield 2011). For adults lacking a significant other or friend, attachment to a spiritual figure, like God, may be particularly important. Research provides support that God or a higher power may play a unique and healthy function in terms of helping the person develop differentiated attachment-related internal working models of self and other (Kimball et al. 2013).
Kirkpatrick’s original theory has been used to inform research efforts to develop questionnaires to assess attachment to God. Beck and McDonald (2004) and Rowatt and Kirkpatrick (2002) independently developed Attachment to God questionnaires to help operationalize the attachment to God construct. Research by Rowatt and Kirkpatrick (2002) and their development of an Attachment to God Scale suggests that two dimensions—anxiety and avoidance—underlie attachment to God. According to Rowatt and Kirkpatrick, individuals high in attachment anxiety view God as inconsistent in meeting their needs, and individuals high in attachment avoidance are distrusting of God as an attachment figure. Rowatt and Kirkpatrick found that anxious attachment to God in particular was associated with neuroticism and negative affect. Avoidant attachment, on the other hand, had less consistent findings. Ultimately, if individuals have a secure attachment to God, they will turn to God as a safe haven when distressed and will use God as a secure base through whom they can navigate life’s difficulties (Granqvist and Kirkpatrick 2013).
Though compared to childhood attachment to caregivers there is less research on the association between attachment to God and specific mental health issues, there are several studies that show that secure attachment to God is associated with positive affect and religious well-being (Beck and McDonald 2004; Limke and Mayfield 2011; Rowatt and Kirkpatrick 2002). Miner (2009) found that attachment to God remains associated with psychological well-being after controlling for caregiver attachment. In addition, attachment to God appears to be a better predictor of well-being than other religious measures, including church attendance, God image, or prayer frequency (Bradshaw et al. 2010; Leman et al. 2018). The foregoing research has primarily focused on the role of attachment to God from a Westernized, individual perspective. While beyond the scope of the current study, a line of research is developing on the role of attachment in settings that can be more collectivistic as it relates to attachment to God or a higher power (see Sahdra and Shaver (2013) for a comparison between attachment theory and Buddhist psychology, and see Ghobary Bonab et al. (2013) for attachment to God in Islamic spirituality).
Our previous research has demonstrated that some maladaptive aspects of religiousness (e.g., a belief in a punishing God) are associated with the left side of the IPC with deleterious social correlates such as low social support. Given its conceptual similarity with the maladaptive aspects of religiousness we previously “mapped” onto the IPC, we expected that the dimensions of insecure attachment to God would also be associated with the left side of the IPC. No prediction was rendered for whether these dimensions of attachment to God would be associated with dominance or submissiveness.

1.2. Trait Self-Control and Interpersonal Outcomes

Individual differences in self-control are reflected in a person’s ability to adjust or adapt to internal or external circumstances (Tangney et al. 2004). Self-control is a general component of self-regulation, and it refers to the ability to regulate or manage one’s emotions, behaviors, and thoughts for the purpose of pursuing goals. An individual possessing a high degree of this personality construct has the ability to modulate and override his or her thoughts, emotions, and action tendencies for the purpose of behaving in a manner conducive to his or her corresponding goals, demands, laws, or standards, even when experiencing an intense urge or proclivity to carry out an opposite behavior (Baumeister et al. 2006). In essence, self-control reflects an individual’s ability to change his or her inner response if it is not helpful to overall wellbeing and avoid carrying out behaviors that are potentially or actually harmful.
Individuals possessing high levels of dispositional self-control exhibit discipline, reliability, and hard work. They are thought to possess more adaptive functioning, particularly in the form of academic achievement, self-esteem, interpersonal skills, satisfying relationships, secure attachment, and overall positive emotions (Uzun et al. 2020). In particular, high scores of dispositional self-control have been associated with higher grade-point averages and self-esteem, more secure attachment, more favorable emotional responses, and less binge eating and alcohol abuse. Additionally, low scores of dispositional self-control are thought to contribute to personal and interpersonal problems, and even depression and anxiety (de Ridder et al. 2012; May et al. 2017; Stavrova et al. 2020).
As others have noted, there is value in better understanding the intrapersonal outcomes associated with trait self-control (Burkley et al. 2011). However, self-control is associated with adaptive functioning with others (Boman et al. 2012; Finkel et al. 2016; McDermott 2018; Vohs et al. 2011); therefore, considering self-control from an interpersonal perspective can make useful contributions to the literature. For example, situation selection may be one correlate of self-control that is interpersonally based. In other words, birds of a feather flock together; high self-control pulls for a social environment that leads to better, more adaptive friendships (Boman 2017). Exposure to negative social exchanges is associated with distress and poor health (Kenny et al. 2013; Wickrama et al. 2010). Limiting one’s exposure to these events through situation selection can set up transactional cycles over time that facilitate emotional equanimity, interpersonal stability, and fewer encounters with adverse social situations.

1.3. The Present Study

As the foregoing suggests, self-control is associated with many positive outcomes, but it is a limited resource (Baumeister and Alquist 2009). Though it has recently been seen as a potentially important variable in terms of self-regulation (McCullough and Carter 2013), attachment to God has not been examined as a potential moderator of the relationship between self-control and adverse interpersonal outcomes. The ability to monitor oneself so as to meet social- and self-standards has been associated with parental attachment (Nie et al. 2016) as well as attachment to romantic partners (Slotter et al. 2020). Attachment to God may be a protective factor and aid a person in self-regulation, such that more secure attachment to God enhances the healthy effect of self-control in limiting one’s exposure to negative social exchanges. The goal of the current study is to examine whether the relationship between self-control and aversive social experiences is separately moderated by the dimensions of attachment to God.

2. Method

2.1. Participants and Procedures

A total of 1049 participants from the United States completed this study, and the data of 997 of these participants were used for analysis after adjusting for straight-line responses. A panel aggregator, Qualtrics, was used to collect the online sample that came from actively managed market research panels. Qualtrics provides high-quality research samples by checking every IP address, using deduplication technology, and using sophisticated digital fingerprinting for bot mitigation purposes. To enroll in a survey, potential respondents are sent an email invitation inviting them to complete a survey for research purposes and informing them about the length of time it takes to complete the survey and incentives for doing so. Incentives may include cash, airline miles, gift cards, redeemable points, sweepstake entry, and vouchers.
Qualtrics creates a panel base that is proportioned to the general population as well as the requirements of the specific survey. Screening criteria can also be used based on the particular samples that are desired. For the current study, only individuals who self-reported belief in a higher power and were at least 18 years of age met the screening criteria to complete the survey. Participant ages ranged between 18 and 82 years (M = 43; SD = 16.5). This population was 51% female, 49% male; 59.9% were non-Hispanic white, 17.7% were Hispanic, 12.1% were non-Hispanic black, 5.4% were Asian, and 3.1% were American Indian/Alaskan Native. Regional breakdown included 21.33% Midwest, 18.03% Northeast, 37.27% South, and 23.38% West. The religious affiliations of the participants are shown in Table 1. The time length to complete the online survey was 20 min or less. Data quality was assessed and screened by short time completion, straight line responses, and incoherent responses. Missing data were completed by calculating the means of the participants’ scores on each subscale of the individual questionnaires.
The current study was approved by the Indiana State University institutional review board. Deciding to take the online survey was considered consent to participate in this research, and Qualtrics employed a double opt-in registration process to ensure that participants had made a conscious decision to participate in online surveys. Qualtrics uses several methodologies to recruit respondents. For example, participants may be recruited via email, e-newsletter campaigns, traditional banner placements, and social media. Multiple sources of participants are used to reduce bias that could result from using only one or a few recruiting sources. No identifying information was collected.

2.2. Measures

The Attachment to God Scale (AGS) (Rowatt and Kirkpatrick 2002) is a 9-item measure that assesses individual differences along the dimensions of attachment anxiety (e.g., God’s reactions to me seem to be inconsistent) and attachment avoidance (e.g., God seems impersonal to me) in relation to God using a 7-point scale. The AGS has 6 items assessing attachment avoidance and 3 items assessing attachment to anxiety. Higher scores for the items assessing avoidance indicate higher avoidant attachment to God and higher scores for the items assessing anxiety indicate higher anxious attachment to God. Good reliability (attachment anxiety: α = 0.73, attachment avoidance: α = 0.91) and validity of the AGS have been demonstrated in past studies (Fergus and Rowatt 2014).
The Brief Self-Control Scale (BSCS) is a 13-item self-report questionnaire designed to measure the construct of self-control. The creation of the BSCS resulted from condensing the Total Self-Control Scale. Tangney and colleagues (2004) performed 2 studies to establish that the BSCS remained an adequate measure of self-control after its reduction from the Total Self-Control Scale. The BSCS highly correlated with the Total Self-Control Scale in study 1 (0.93) and study 2 (0.92). The BSCS established good internal consistency and retest reliability. Additionally, the BSCS was found to measure the same domains and content of self-control as the Total Self-Control Scale (Tangney et al. 2004). A recent psychometric study demonstrated further good properties of the BSCS, including support for a unidimensional structure (Manapat et al. 2021)
The International Personality Item Pool—Interpersonal Circumplex (IPIP-IPC), a 32-item measure, was used as a brief assessment of the dimensions of the IPC (Markey and Markey 2009). Participants rate themselves with regard to the accuracy of various descriptions (e.g., “Let others finish what they are saying” and “Snap at people”) using a 5-point Likert scale. IPIP octants were calculated via the summation of the 4 distinct items loading on each octant; these octants were standardized, and then the 2 broad dimensions of social behavior—control and affiliation—were calculated from circumplex weighting of the IPIP octants (Wiggins and Broughton 1991). The IPIP-IPC was used to determine the “interpersonal style” associated with the attachment to God dimensions (i.e., anxiety and avoidance). The IPIP-IPC maintains the good psychometric properties and circular structure of longer measures such as the Interpersonal Adjective Scale (Markey and Markey 2009). In the present study, the control dimension (α = 0.82) and affiliation dimension (α = 0.80) had adequate internal consistency.
The Test of Negative Social Exchanges (TENSE), an 18-item measure, was used to assess exposure to aversive social experiences such as insensitivity, hostility, rejection, and ridicule. It has good internal consistency and test–retest reliability (Jordan et al. 2015; Ruehlman and Karoly 1991). In the current study, participants were asked how often the people in their lives engaged in particular social exchanges over the previous month on a 5-point Likert scale.

3. Results

Correlations, means, standard deviations, and internal consistencies are presented in Table 2. The avoidance dimension of attachment to God was significantly correlated with the anxious dimension (r = 0.45, p < 0.001), self-control (r = −0.32, p < 0.001), and negative social exchanges (r = 0.23, p < 0.001). The anxious dimension of attachment to God was significantly correlated with self-control (r = −0.31, p < 0.001) and negative social exchanges (r = 0.35, p < 0.001). Self-control was negatively correlated with negative social exchanges (r = −0.39, p < 0.001).
To determine the interpersonal style associated with attachment to God, each dimension (i.e., anxiety and avoidance) score was regressed on the control and affiliation interpersonal style scores from the IPIP-IPC. The multiple R indicates the “interpersonalness” of the construct, and the regression coefficients provide the specific location of the scale within the IPC (Gurtman 1991). Results for the regressions of each dimension of attachment to God on the IPIP-IPC dimensions of affiliation and control are presented in Table 3. Both dimensions of attachment to God had a significant association with the cold/hostile pole of the IPC, and to a lesser extent (though still significant), the dominant pole of the IPC. Taken together, both avoidance and anxiety—as it relates to attachment to God—are marked by a hostile-dominant interpersonal style.
For the moderation analyses, the PROCESS macro developed by Hayes (2018) was used to test for interactions. For the following analyses, the independent variable and moderator variable were mean centered. The overall model of self-control, avoidance, and their interaction predicting negative social exchanges was significant (R = 0.41, F(3, 951) = 64.07, p < 0.001). Self-control significantly predicted negative social exchanges (β = −0.37, t = −11.51, p < 0.001). Avoidance significantly predicted negative social exchanges (β = 0.11, t = 3.52, p < 0.001). The interaction between self-control and avoidance was also significant (β = −0.08, t = −2.72, p < 0.01). A simple slope analysis between self-control and negative social exchanges was conducted at low (−1 SD below the mean), moderate (mean), and high (+1 SD above the mean). At 1 standard deviation below the mean of avoidant attachment to God, the relationship between self-control and negative social exchanges was significant (β = −0.29, t = −7.35, p = 0.001). At the mean value, the relationship between self-control and negative social exchanges was significant (β = −0.37, t = −11.51, p < 0.001). At 1 standard deviation above the mean of avoidant attachment, the relationship between self-control and negative social exchanges was significant (β = −0.45, t = −9.40, p < 0.001). Figure 1 plots the simple slopes for the interaction.
The overall model of self-control, anxiety, and their interaction predicting negative social exchanges was significant (R = 0.47, F(3, 951) = 90.93, p < 0.001). Self-control was significantly associated with negative social exchanges (β = −0.34, t = −10.99, p = < 0.001). Anxiety significantly predicted negative social exchanges (β = 0.24, t = 8.01, p < 0.001). The interaction between self-control and anxiety was also significant (β = −0.14, t = −4.93, p < 0.001). A simple slope analysis between self-control and negative social exchanges was conducted at low (−1 SD below the mean), moderate (mean), and high (+1 SD above the mean). At 1 standard deviation below the mean of anxious attachment to God, the relationship between self-control and negative social exchanges was significant (β = −0.20, t = −5.43, p = < 0.001). At the mean value, the relationship between self-control and negative social exchanges was significant (β = −0.34, t = −10.99, p = < 0.001). At 1 standard deviation above the mean of anxious attachment, the relationship between self-control and negative social exchanges was significant (β = −0.47, t = −10.55, p < 0.001). Figure 2 plots the simple slopes for the interaction.

4. Discussion

The current study determined the interpersonal style associated with the dimensions of attachment to God (i.e., anxiety and avoidance) and then examined their moderating effect on the relationship between self-control and negative social exchanges. First, it was hypothesized that both dimensions of attachment to God would be associated with the unfriendliness dimension of the IPC. Second, it was hypothesized that the relationship between self-control and negative social exchanges would vary depending upon the anxiety and avoidance dimensions of attachment to God. The results of the current study supported these hypotheses.
Our study found that the anxiety and avoidance dimensions of attachment to God were associated with unfriendliness, and to a lesser degree, interpersonal dominance. This is known as a hostile-dominant interpersonal style, and based on the principle of complementarity (Pincus and Ansell 2013), this style will pull for, or elicit, hostile and submissive responses from others. As repeated interpersonal transactions happen over time, social exchanges are likely to develop that qualitatively differ from interpersonal transactions marked by reciprocated friendliness. The findings related to the first hypothesis are consistent with our previous research (Jordan et al. 2014).
Our study also found that self-control is associated with fewer negative social exchanges, but the strength of this association varies depending upon the anxiety and avoidance dimensions of attachment to God. For the avoidance dimension of attachment to God, the most negative social exchanges occurred in individuals with low self-control and high avoidance. A similar finding was also found for the anxiety dimension of attachment to God. Put differently, at low levels of self-control, more and more negative social exchanges were reported by participants, as they had higher levels of attachment-related avoidance and anxiety.
As it relates to self-control and its association with interpersonal outcomes, the moderating effect of the dimensions of attachment to God is consistent with a resource depletion model. If as the current findings suggest, the anxiety and avoidance dimensions of attachment to God are associated with a hostile-dominant interpersonal style, then the resulting transactional cycles will be marked by interpersonal distress, which has been found to be associated with a depletion of self-control. Low levels of attachment-related avoidance and anxiety toward God seem to play a protective role in terms of less exposure to negative social exchanges, especially for individuals who rate themselves as low in trait levels of self-control. The implication of the current findings is that insecure attachment to God may be a relevant factor to consider in religious individuals in therapy settings where matters related to self-regulation and interpersonal outcomes are frequently discussed.
Another implication has to do with stress regulation (Williams et al. 2009). Attachment to God may influence stress regulation at the level of stress exposure, reactivity, recovery, and/or restoration. McCullough and Carter (2011) aptly describe how “simply avoiding” certain situations may be one way that religion leads to self-monitoring that renders one’s interpersonal environment less turbulent and mired in conflict. In other words, limiting one’s level of stress exposure through situation selection is an important component of preserving one’s capacity to implement and maintain self-control.
The current study is not without its weaknesses. First and foremost, this is a cross-sectional study and the temporal sequence of the variables included in this study cannot be ascertained. While attachment to one’s caregiver is clearly an early occurring process, attachment to God occurs later and it is an unresolved question as to whether religiousness leads to self-control or vice versa (for a longitudinal example of personality variables associated with later religiousness, see McCullough et al. 2003). Second, as a non-experimental study, cause cannot be determined, which is a very important consideration when examining a resource depletion model of self-control and its effect on interpersonal outcomes. Finally, the sample is derived from a predominantly Westernized, individualistic culture that may not be generalizable to other cultures (see Sahdra and Shaver (2013) for attachment in Buddhist psychology, and see Ghobary Bonab et al. (2013) for attachment to God in Islamic spirituality).
Nevertheless, the current study has several strengths. First, the study included a large, national sample, which may have led to more reliable findings. Second, the study only included individuals who had a belief in God or a higher power. There has been criticism about past studies within the psychology of religion in terms of their samples and the lack of clarity regarding whether individuals low in religiousness, for example, were atheists, agnostic, or something else (Sloan and Bagiella 2002). Third, to our knowledge, no other research has specifically examined the relationship between trait self-control and attachment to God. While there have been studies that are clearly associated with attachment-related processes and behaviors associated with self-control (Exline et al. 2016), none have addressed the avoidance and anxiety dimensions of attachment to God. As such, the current findings uniquely contribute to the research literature. Finally, the current findings extend previous research on the interpersonal style of components of religiousness (Jordan et al. 2014).
In conclusion, the current study replicates past research demonstrating a relationship between self-control and interpersonal outcomes. It offers a new finding on the role that attachment to God plays in the relationship between trait levels of self-control and negative social exchanges. The interpersonal perspective of personality, social, and clinical psychology suggests that attachment to God is an individual difference variable that results in recurring patterns of social exchanges that benefit or harm the individual. Future research can build upon the current study’s findings by assessing other relevant variables (e.g., intrinsic/extrinsic religiousness, devoutness) and including other components of the methodological framework of the interpersonal perspective (e.g., interpersonal values) to comprehensively examine the effects of attachment to God on self-regulation.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, K.D.J., K.L.N. and A.M.F.; methodology, K.D.J. and K.L.N.; software, K.D.J.; validation, K.D.J. and K.L.N.; formal analysis, K.D.J.; investigation, K.D.J.; resources, K.D.J., K.L.N. and A.M.F.; data curation, K.D.J. and K.L.N.; writing—original draft preparation, K.D.J., K.L.N. and A.M.F.; writing—review and editing, K.D.J.; visualization, K.D.J.; supervision, K.D.J.; project administration, K.D.J.; funding acquisition, K.D.J. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by the Metanexus Institute.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted according to the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki, and approved by the Institutional Review Board of Indiana State University (protocol code 1522998-1, date of approval 11 December 2019).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The data presented in this study are available on request from the corresponding author.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Baumeister, Roy F., and Jessica L. Alquist. 2009. Self-regulation as a limited resource: Strength model of control and depletion. In Psychology of Self-Regulation: Cognitive, Affective, and Motivational Processes. Edited by Joseph P. Forgas, Roy F. Baumeister and Dianne M. Tice. New York: Psychology Press, vol. 11, pp. 21–33. [Google Scholar]
  2. Baumeister, Roy F., Matthew Gailliot, C. Nathan DeWall, and Megan Oaten. 2006. Self-Regulation and Personality: How Interventions Increase Regulatory Success, and How Depletion Moderates the Effects of Traits on Behavior. Journal of Personality 74: 1773–801. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Beck, Richard. 2006. God as a secure base: Attachment to God and theological exploration. Journal of Psychology and Theology 34: 125–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Beck, Richard, and Angie McDonald. 2004. Attachment to God: The Attachment to God Inventory, tests of working model correspondence, and an exploration of faith group differences. Journal of Psychology and Theology 32: 92–103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Boman, John H. I. V. 2017. Do birds of a feather really flock together? Friendships, self-control similarity and deviant behaviour. British Journal of Criminology 57: 1208–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Boman, John H. I. V., Marvin D. Krohn, Chris L. Gibson, and John M. Stogner. 2012. Investigating friendship quality: An exploration of self-control and social control theories’ friendship hypotheses. Journal of Youth and Adolescence 41: 1526–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Bowlby, John. 1951. Maternal care and mental health. Bulletin of the World Health Organization 3: 355–533. [Google Scholar]
  8. Bowlby, John. 1982. Attachment and loss: Retrospect and prospect. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry 52: 664–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  9. Bradshaw, Matt, Christopher G. Ellison, and Jack P. Marcum. 2010. Attachment to God, images of God, and psychological distress in a nationwide sample of Presbyterians. International Journal for the Psychology of Religion 20: 130–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  10. Burgess, Kim B., Peter J. Marshall, Kenneth H. Rubin, and Nathan A. Fox. 2003. Infant attachment and temperament as predictors of subsequent externalizing problems and cardiac physiology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 44: 819–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Burkley, Edward, Darshon Anderson, and Jessica Curtis. 2011. You wore me down: Self-control strength and social influence. Social and Personality Psychology Compass 5: 487–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. de Ridder, Denise T. D., Gerty Lensvelt-Mulders, Catrin Finkenauer, F. Marijn Stok, and Roy F. Baumeister. 2012. Taking stock of self-control: A meta-analysis of how trait self-control relates to a wide range of behaviors. Personality and Social Psychology Review 16: 76–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  13. DeKlyen, Michelle, Mathhew L. Speltz, and Mark T. Greenberg. 1998. Fathering and early onset conduct problems: Positive and negative parenting, father–son attachment, and the marital context. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review 1: 3–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Exline, Julie J., Steffany J. Homolka, and Valencia A. Harriott. 2016. Divine struggles: Links with body image concerns, binging, and compensatory behaviours around eating. Mental Health, Religion & Culture 19: 8–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Fergus, Thomas A., and Wade C. Rowatt. 2014. Examining a purported association between attachment to God and scrupulosity. Psychology of Religion and Spirituality 6: 230–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  16. Finkel, Eli J., Grainne M. Fitzsimons, and Michelle R. van Dellen. 2016. Self-regulation as a transactive process: Reconceptualizing the unit of analysis for goal setting, pursuit, and outcomes. In Handbook of Self-Regulation: Research, Theory, and Applications, 3rd ed. Edited by Kathleen D. Vohs and Roy F. Baumeister. New York: The Guilford Press, pp. 264–82. [Google Scholar]
  17. Ghobary Bonab, Bonab, Maureen Miner, and Marie-Therese Proctor. 2013. Attachment to God in Islamic spirituality. Journal of Muslim Mental Health 7: 77–104. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  18. Gilliom, Miles, Daniel S. Shaw, Joy E. Beck, Michael A. Schonberg, and JoElla L. Lukon. 2002. Anger regulation in disadvantaged preschool boys: Strategies, antecedents, and the development of self-control. Developmental Psychology 38: 222–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Granqvist, Pehr, and Lee A. Kirkpatrick. 2013. Religion, spirituality, and attachment. In APA Handbook of Psychology, Religion, and Spirituality (Vol 1): Context, Theory, and Research. Edited by In Kenneth I. Pargament, Julie J. Exline and James W. Jones. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association, pp. 139–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Gurtman, Michael B. 1991. Evaluating the interpersonalness of personality scales. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 17: 670–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Hankin, Benjamin L., Jon D. Kassel, and John R. Z. Abela. 2005. Adult Attachment Dimensions and Specificity of Emotional Distress Symptoms: Prospective Investigations of Cognitive Risk and Interpersonal Stress Generation as Mediating Mechanisms. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 31: 136–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  22. Hayes, Andrew F. 2018. Introduction to Mediation, Moderation, and Conditional Process Analysis, 2nd ed. New York: The Guilford Press. [Google Scholar]
  23. Hill, Terrence D., Christopher S. Bradley, Benjamin Dowd-Arrow, and Amy M. Burdette. 2019. Religious attendance and the social support trajectories of older Mexican Americans. Journal of Cross-Cultural Gerontology 34: 403–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. Jordan, Kevin D., Kevin S. Masters, Stephanie A. Hooker, John. M. Ruiz, and Timothy W. Smith. 2014. An interpersonal approach to religiousness and spirituality: Implications for health and well-being. Journal of Personality 82: 418–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Jordan, Kevin D., Paula G. Williams, and Timothy W. Smith. 2015. Interpersonal distinctions among hypochondriacal trait components: Styles, goals, vulnerabilities, and perceptions of health care providers. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology 34: 459–75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Kenny, Rachel, Barbara Dooley, and Amanda Fitzgerald. 2013. Interpersonal relationships and emotional distress in adolescence. Journal of Adolescence 36: 351–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  27. Kimball, Cynthia N., Chris J. Boyatzis, and Kaye V. Cook. 2013. Attachment to God: A qualitative exploration of emerging adults’ spiritual relationship with God. Journal of Psychology and Theology 41: 175–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Kirkpatrick, Lee A. 1992. An attachment-theory approach to the psychology of religion. International Journal for the Psychology of Religion 2: 3–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Leman, Joseph, III, Will Hunter, Thomas Fergus, and Wade Rowatt. 2018. Secure attachment to God uniquely linked to psychological health in a national, random sample of American adults. International Journal for the Psychology of Religion 28: 162–73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Limke, Alicia, and Patrick B. Mayfield. 2011. Attachment to God: Differentiating the contributions of fathers and mothers using the Experiences in Parental Relationships Scale. Journal of Psychology and Theology 39: 122–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Manapat, Patrick D., Michael C. Edwards, David P. MacKinnon, Russell A. Poldrack, and Lisa A. Marsch. 2021. A psychometric analysis of the Brief Self-Control Scale. Assessment 28: 395–412. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Markey, Patrick M., and Charlotte N. Markey. 2009. A brief assessment of the interpersonal circumplex: The IPIP-IPC. Assessment 16: 352–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  33. May, Ross W., Gregory S. Seibert, Marcos A. Sanchez-Gonzalez, Michael C. Fitzgerald, and Frank D. Fincham. 2017. Dispositional self-control: Relationships with aerobic capacity and morning surge in blood pressure. Stress: The International Journal on the Biology of Stress 20: 29–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  34. McCullough, Michael E., and Evan C. Carter. 2011. Waiting, tolerating, and cooperating: Did religion evolve to prop up humans’ self-control abilities? In Handbook of Self-Regulation: Research, Theory, and Applications, 2nd ed. Edited by Kathleen D. Vohs and Roy F. Baumeister. New York: The Guilford Press, pp. 422–37. [Google Scholar]
  35. McCullough, Michael E., and Evan C. Carter. 2013. Religion, self-control, and self-regulation: How and why are they related? In APA Handbook of Psychology, Religion, and Spirituality (Vol 1): Context, Theory, and Research. Edited by Kenneth I. Pargament, Julie J. Exline and James W. Jones. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association, pp. 123–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. McCullough, Michael E., Jo-Ann Tsang, and Sharon Brion. 2003. Personality traits in adolescence as predictors of religiousness in early adulthood: Findings from the Terman longitudinal study. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 29: 980–91. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. McCullough, Michael E., William T. Hoyt, David B. Larson, Harold G. Koenig, and Carl Thoresen. 2000. Religious involvement and mortality: A meta-analytic review. Health Psychology 19: 211–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. McDermott, Elana R. 2018. Reproducing economic inequality: Longitudinal relations of self-control, social support, and maternal education. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology 56: 79–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Mikulincer, Mario, and Philip R. Shaver. 2012a. Adult attachment orientations and relationship processes. Journal of Family Theory & Review 4: 259–74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Mikulincer, Mario, and Philip R. Shaver. 2012b. Attachment theory expanded: A behavioral systems approach. In The Oxford Handbook of Personality and Social Psychology. Edited by Kay Deaux and Mark Snyder. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 467–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Miner, Maureen. 2009. The impact of child-parent attachment, attachment to God and religious orientation on psychological adjustment. Journal of Psychology and Theology 37: 114–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Nie, Yan-Gang, Jian-Bin Li, and Alexander T. Vazsonyi. 2016. Self-control mediates the associations between parental attachment and prosocial behavior among Chinese adolescents. Personality and Individual Differences 96: 36–39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Orehek, Edward, Anna Vazeou-Nieuwenhuis, Ellen Quick, and Casey C. Weaverling. 2017. Attachment and self-regulation. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 43: 365–80. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  44. Petrovic, Katja, Cassandra M. Chapman, and Timothy P. Schofield. 2021. Religiosity and volunteering over time: Religious service attendance is associated with the likelihood of volunteering, and religious importance with time spent volunteering. Psychology of Religion and Spirituality 13: 136–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Phillips, Russell E., III, and Michael B. Kitchens. 2016. Augustine or Philistine? College students’ sanctification of learning and its implications. International Journal for the Psychology of Religion 26: 80–94. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Pincus, Aaron L., and Emily B. Ansell. 2013. Interpersonal theory of personality. In Handbook of psychology: Personality and Social Psychology, 2nd ed. Edited by Howard Tennen, Jerry Suls and Irving B. Weiner. New York: John Wiley & Sons, Inc., vol. 5, pp. 141–59. [Google Scholar]
  47. Rosenstein, Diana S., and Harvey A. Horowitz. 1996. Adolescent attachment and psychopathology. (Attachment and Psychopathology, Part 2). Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 64: 244–53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Rowatt, Wade C., and Lee A. Kirkpatrick. 2002. Two dimensions of attachment to God and their relation to affect, religiosity, and personality constructs. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion 41: 637–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Ruehlman, Linda S., and Paul Karoly. 1991. With a little flak from my friends: Development and preliminary validation of the Test of Negative Social Exchange (TENSE). Psychological Assessment: A Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 3: 97–104. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Sahdra, Baljinder K., and Philip R. Shaver. 2013. Comparing attachment theory and Buddhist psychology. International Journal for the Psychology of Religion 23: 282–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Sloan, Richard P., and Emilia Bagiella. 2002. Claims about religious involvement and health outcomes. Annals of Behavioral Medicine 24: 14–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Slotter, Erica B., Jessica L. Grom, and Brenden Tervo-Clemmens. 2020. Don’t take it out on me: Displaced aggression after provocation by a romantic partner as a function of attachment anxiety and self-control. Psychology of Violence 10: 232–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Stavrova, Olga, Tila Pronk, and Michail D. Kokkoris. 2020. Finding meaning in self-control: The effect of self-control on the perception of meaning in life. Self and Identity 19: 201–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  54. Tangney, June P., Roy F. Baumeister, and Angie L. Boone. 2004. High Self-Control Predicts Good Adjustment, Less Pathology, Better Grades, and Interpersonal Success. Journal of Personality 72: 271–322. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Uzun, Bilge, Sera LeBlanc, and Joseph R. Ferrari. 2020. Relationship between academic procrastinaton and self-control: The mediational role of self-esteem. College Student Journal 54: 309–16. [Google Scholar]
  56. Valikhani, Ahmad, Mehdi R. Sarafraz, and Pooneh Moghimi. 2018. Examining the role of attachment styles and self-control in suicide ideation and death anxiety for patients receiving chemotherapy in Iran. Psycho-Oncology 27: 1057–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Vohs, Kathleen D., Catrin Finkenauer, and Roy F. Baumeister. 2011. The sum of friends’ and lovers’ self-control scores predicts relationship quality. Social Psychological and Personality Science 2: 138–45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  58. Whitehead, Alfred N. 1926. Religion in the making: Lowell lectures. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
  59. Wickrama, Kandauda A. S., Chalandra M. Bryant, and Thulitha K. A. Wickrama. 2010. Perceived community disorder, hostile marital interactions, and self-reported health of African American couples: An interdyadic process. Personal Relationships 17: 515–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Wiggins, Jerry S., and Ross Broughton. 1991. A geometric taxonomy of personality scales. European Journal of Personality 5: 343–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Williams, Paula G., Holly K. Rau, Matthew R. Cribbet, and Heather E. Gunn. 2009. Openness to experience and stress regulation. Journal of Research in Personality 43: 777–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. The relationship between self-control and negative social exchanges moderated by avoidant attachment to God.
Figure 1. The relationship between self-control and negative social exchanges moderated by avoidant attachment to God.
Religions 12 00739 g001
Figure 2. The relationship between self-control and negative social exchanges moderated by anxious attachment to God.
Figure 2. The relationship between self-control and negative social exchanges moderated by anxious attachment to God.
Religions 12 00739 g002
Table 1. Descriptive statistics of religion.
Table 1. Descriptive statistics of religion.
ReligionFrequencyPercent
Atheist40.4
Buddhist181.8
Hindu80.8
Jehovah’s Witness80.8
Jewish262.6
LDS101.0
Muslim505.0
New age40.4
Lutheran252.5
Roman Catholic21922.0
Episcopalian90.9
Methodist282.8
Presbyterian191.9
Christian39539.6
Baptist757.5
Pentecostal282.8
Adventist30.3
Taoist10.1
Unitarian20.2
Other or none of the above656.5
Table 2. Means, standard deviations, Cronbach’s alphas, and correlations for the Attachment to God scale, Brief Self-Control Scale, and Test of Negative Social Exchanges scale.
Table 2. Means, standard deviations, Cronbach’s alphas, and correlations for the Attachment to God scale, Brief Self-Control Scale, and Test of Negative Social Exchanges scale.
AGS AvoidanceAGS AnxietyBSCSTENSE
AGS Avoidance
AGS Anxiety0.45 *
BSCS−0.32 *−0.31 *
TENSE0.23 *0.35 *−0.39 *
Sample (n = 997)
Mean16.5111.8342.2237.58
SD7.824.759.6516.47
α0.810.790.820.96
Note. AGS = Attachment to God scale; BSCS = Brief Self-Control Scale; Test of Negative Social Exchanges scale = TENSE. * p < 0.001.
Table 3. Multiple regression of dimensions of attachment to God on affiliation and control measures of interpersonal style (IPIP-IPC).
Table 3. Multiple regression of dimensions of attachment to God on affiliation and control measures of interpersonal style (IPIP-IPC).
RF(2, 953)β Affiliationβ Control
AGS avoidance0.4090.56 *−0.37 *0.14 *
AGS anxiety0.2944.78 *−0.24 *0.16 *
Note. AGS = Attachment to God scale. * p < 0.001
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Jordan, K.D.; Niehus, K.L.; Feinstein, A.M. Insecure Attachment to God and Interpersonal Conflict. Religions 2021, 12, 739. https://doi.org/10.3390/rel12090739

AMA Style

Jordan KD, Niehus KL, Feinstein AM. Insecure Attachment to God and Interpersonal Conflict. Religions. 2021; 12(9):739. https://doi.org/10.3390/rel12090739

Chicago/Turabian Style

Jordan, Kevin D., Katie L. Niehus, and Ari M. Feinstein. 2021. "Insecure Attachment to God and Interpersonal Conflict" Religions 12, no. 9: 739. https://doi.org/10.3390/rel12090739

APA Style

Jordan, K. D., Niehus, K. L., & Feinstein, A. M. (2021). Insecure Attachment to God and Interpersonal Conflict. Religions, 12(9), 739. https://doi.org/10.3390/rel12090739

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop