Next Article in Journal
Special Issue “International Conference of Spirituality in Healthcare. Creating Space for Spirituality in Healthcare,”—Trinity College Dublin 2017
Next Article in Special Issue
Association between Religion and Health in China: Using Propensity Score Matching Method
Previous Article in Journal
Gods, Gurus, Prophets and the Poor: Exploring Informal, Interfaith Exchanges among Working Class Female Workers in an Indian City
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

One Religion, Two Tales: Religion and Happiness in Urban and Rural Areas of China

School of Business, Macao University of Science and Technology, Taipa, Macau 999078, China
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Religions 2019, 10(9), 532; https://doi.org/10.3390/rel10090532
Submission received: 3 July 2019 / Revised: 25 August 2019 / Accepted: 9 September 2019 / Published: 17 September 2019
(This article belongs to the Special Issue The Relationship between Religiosity and Mental Health)

Abstract

:
Most previous studies performed in Western social contexts have revealed that religion can influence an individual’s sense of happiness. Few studies have sought to clarify the influence of religion in a Chinese social context, however, and there has been no study specifically about the potential differences in the dichotomous social environments of urban and rural areas in China. Via the nationwide survey data of the 2007 Spiritual Life Study of Chinese Residents (SLSC), this study examines the association between religion and happiness among urban and rural residents of China. The results reveal that there is a generally positive association between religion and happiness among those with religious affiliations in China. Regardless of affiliation with Buddhism or Protestantism, there is a strong positive association among rural respondents, an insignificant association among urban respondents, and mixed results among town residents.

1. Introduction

Happiness, a feeling that is desired by every human being (Rizvi and Hossain 2017), is a “psychological outcome that encompasses an individual’s cognitive judgments of satisfaction and the affective appraisals of emotions” (Kesebir and Diener 2008, p. 118). A lot of studies have determined a long list of factors relating to the how and why of happiness. Factors such as health, marital status, income, social status, psychological status, social networks, and social capital can influence the happiness of an individual (Chen and Williams 2016; Cheung 2016). Among these factors, religion has been one of the unique factors found to produce the benefits of mental health, physical well-being, social support, and internal peace that ultimately lead to an individual’s happiness (Rizvi and Hossain 2017).
Meanwhile, scholars have proposed that among different cultural contexts, such as those found in East Asia, South Asia, and the Middle East (e.g., Lim et al. 2019; Devine et al. 2019; Fariddanesh and Rezaei 2019), there has only been a very small volume of literature focusing on countries where atheism is the social norm (McCullough and Willoughby 2009; Potter 2003; Wielander 2017; Thege et al. 2013; Yang 2006). China is considered to be much less religious than many Western countries (Overmyer and Edmonds 2003; Wang et al. 2015). However, China has a long history of religion, and there has been a religious awakening in China during the past decades (Chau 2010; Liu et al. 2011; Wei and Liu 2013; Ying et al. 2017). Existing studies on the relationship between religion and happiness in mainland China have presented some mixed results (Deng et al. 2019; Liu and Koenig 2013; Chen and Williams 2016; He et al. 2016; Lu and Zhang 2016; Zhang et al. 2016).
Further, the secularization hypothesis of religion (Norris and Inglehart 2011) argues that conditions of socioeconomic inequality may influence religiosity, and that social environment may strengthen or attenuate associations between religion and happiness (Büssing and Koenig 2010; Diener et al. 2011; Lun and Bond 2013). China has long been a place of social dichotomies, with significant disparities between social economic development levels and individual well-being between urban and rural residents (Knight and Gunatilaka 2010; Knight et al. 2009; Liang and Wang 2014; Treiman 2012; Zhao 2012). Constituting more than 50% of the whole population, rural residents (peasants) are usually considered to be of lower social status in China due to their much lower income level and poor living conditions. Urban and rural residents may also have different individual or social motivations to practice religion, and despite the hostile rural social environment, the religious residents in China might be more likely to report higher levels of happiness than their peers living in urban areas. Using the nationwide survey data of the 2007 Spiritual Life Study of Chinese Residents (SLSC), this study examines the association between religious involvement and self-reported happiness among the sampled residents of China, and further examines whether the religious urban and rural residents are equally likely to report being happy, controlling for their demographic and socioeconomic characteristics.

2. Religion and Happiness

2.1. The Dichotomy of China: Urban and Rural Areas

Socioeconomic environment. China is a dichotomous society and this societal issue is not only demonstrated in the high level of unequal mean income between rural residents and urban residents, but also in the serious lack of services such as education, social security benefits, and medical care in the rural areas. Despite the great improvements of living standards relative to decades ago, many rural areas in China are still lacking health care services, social security benefits, pension funds, and social capital (Chen and Williams 2016).
Social values, life style and social status. Urban residents emphasize an urbanized life style, equal social status, and greater liberty. Conversely, rural residents emphasize traditional life values and life styles (Davey et al. 2009). Compared to urban residents, the social status of rural residents (peasants) is considered to be at the lower level(s) in the social hierarchy of China (Davey et al. 2009).
Gap of happiness. The socioeconomic dichotomy of urban–rural areas has generated a large gap of happiness among residents, too (Knight et al. 2009; Liang and Wang 2014). On the one hand, in urban areas, “progress paradox” documents the phenomenon that people within an environment of rapid economic growth encounter insecurity (life environment changes) and inequality (winners and losers in the changing process) (Graham and Pettinato 2002). As per these results, urban educated residents in China, compared with rural residents, are more likely to report life stresses, depression, poor mental health, or lower life-satisfaction (Graham et al. 2017).
Based on the unique characteristics of the Chinese social context, it is reasonable to predict that low income and life quality will result in rural residents having comparatively lower levels of happiness than urban residents. However, some scholars argue that as long as a rural resident’s basic needs are satisfied and they prefer a materialistically simpler life, then their happiness levels will not be significantly different from those of urban residents (Davey et al. 2009; Diener and Biswas-Diener 2002; Biswas-Diener et al. 2005).

2.2. Motivations for Religious Involvement in Urban and Rural China

The ratio of the population who are regularly engaged in religious activities has increased from 7% in 2001 (Stark and Liu 2011), to 10% in 2012, and to 26.4% in 2014 (Lu and Zhang 2016; Wenzel-Teuber 2017). Within the dichotomy of the social environment in China, residents in urban and rural areas of China have heterogeneous motivations for participation in religious activities.
In urban areas of China, the motivation for religious conversion may largely be due to the psychological response to social anomies, inequality (Chen and Williams 2016; Lu 2012), or living stresses (Graham et al. 2017). By emphasizing non-material aspects of life, religion may help to improve happiness by providing stress coping skills, as well as by providing a strong sense of purpose and acceptance over and above one’s economic conditions or social status (Chen and Williams 2016).
In rural China, the major reason for religious conversion apart from the religious tradition may be due to family members becoming seriously sick (Liang and Qi 2015; Zheng and Wang 2014) or suffering from some chronic disease (Zhou and Yu 2017). Given the limited reimbursement under the rural health insurance system in China, chronic diseases result in big economic and time costs to rural families. Those facing the hardship of a health crisis may turn to religion, especially Christian involvement in rural China, as an alternative healing method, as they search for social or psychological supportive resources (Zhou and Yu 2017).

2.3. Association and Mechanism of Religion and Happiness

Happiness refers to “a person’s cognitive and affective evaluations of his or her life” (Diener et al. 2002, p. 63). The benchmark of happiness is rooted in the mind of the individual and determines whether he or she is happy (Lim et al. 2019; Lu et al. 2001). Researchers argue that both objective (e.g., income) and subjective factors (e.g., values and religion) can influence the happiness of an individual. Furthermore, whether one objective factor can influence an individual’s happiness depends on whether or not it can change the individual’s subjective state of mind in response to it (Lim et al. 2019; Reyes-García et al. 2016).
Religion is a subjective belief (or set of beliefs) that influences the desired life values, life styles, and life purposes of religious people (Cohen and Johnson 2017; Devine et al. 2019; Fariddanesh and Rezaei 2019; Lun and Bond 2013). Thus, religion influences religious peoples’ subjective responses to objective factors.
The previous study achieved fruitful results suggesting that religion has a positive effect on happiness for people who are religious, providing better physical and mental health and thus greater happiness (Büssing and Koenig 2010; Cohen and Johnson 2017; Krause and Wulff 2005; Lewis and Cruise 2006; Sander 2017; Rizvi and Hossain 2017). For instance, the intrapersonal aspects of religion, such as prayer, meditation, and self-reflection, associate positively with happiness (Greeley and Hout 2006; Krause 2003). Furthermore, religious faith enhances the happiness of individuals via the provision of a comprehensive framework for the interpretation of world events and life challenges, and thus a sense of meaning and purpose in life (Inglehart 2010; Lim and Putnam 2010).
Religion may impact an individual’s happiness through various mechanisms such as social capital (Chen and Williams 2016), social networks (Lim and Putnam 2010), lifestyle or health-related behaviors (Diener and Seligman 2002), and positive psychological status (e.g., faith, inner peace) (Myers 2000; Stark and Maier 2008). Some studies also reveal that being a member of a religious community helps an individual develop longstanding close relationships with other members. These relationships will lead the individual to experience a sense of attachment, commitment, social support, and happiness (Krause 2003). Individuals who turn to religion will obtain social support (e.g., friendship and community), psychological resources (e.g., self-identification, self-esteem, and self-efficacy), coping strategies for dealing with stress or trauma (e.g., illness, unemployment, divorce), a sense of meaning and purpose for life in an unpredictable world, and an expected utility in the afterlife (Brown and Tierney 2009; Greeley and Hout 2006; Inglehart 2010; Krause 2003; Lim and Putnam 2010).

2.4. Association of Religion and Happiness in Urban and Rural China

The secularization hypothesis of religion (Norris and Inglehart 2011) argues that patterns of human security and, in particular, conditions of socioeconomic inequality drive religiosity. People are religious in countries where there are low quality and/or ineffective public services, incomes are low, and personal insecurity is high. In these hostile environments, “religion provides a partial refuge, from disease and distress when it can, and a promise of relief in the next world when it cannot” (Deaton and Stone 2013). Further, when the socioeconomic environment has improved and the citizens can obtain better economic and personal security, religiosity often declines (Norris and Inglehart 2011).
Based on the secularization hypothesis of religion, we can infer that rural religious residents in China probably report higher levels of happiness than those of the urban residents, because religion can better help them in the hostile rural social environment of China. Meanwhile, religiosity among urban residents is not as strong as predicted by the secularization hypothesis (Norris and Inglehart 2011).
In summary, we propose the hypothesis of this study as follows:
Hypothesis 1. 
Religious affiliation is generally positively associated with the self-reported happiness of Chinese urban and rural residents.
Hypothesis 2. 
The association between religion and happiness among rural residents may be stronger than those of urban residents.

3. Data and Sampling

Data comes from the 2007 Spiritual Life Study of Chinese Residents (SLSC), a national multi-stage probability sample of 7021 Chinese in mainland China. A high-quality team of international scholars, with Dr. Fenggang Yang as the principal investigator, designed the questionnaire and led the project. HorizonKey, a Chinese research firm, collected the data through face-to-face interview surveys.
Until now, the SLSC dataset has been the only nationally representative survey with rich information regarding the religious and spiritual lives of residents in mainland China. No new comprehensive survey about religious life in China has been conducted since the SLSC in 2007, and no new survey is planned for the near future. Despite the rapid economic and social changes during the past decade, there have been no remarkable changes with respect to the roles of religion in China since 2007 (Lu and Gao 2017). Therefore, the SLSC dataset should still provide relevant information for understanding religious life in contemporary China (Lu and Gao 2017). The Chinese General Social Surveys (CGSSs) provide more updated samples of 2012 to 2015; however, they only contain three general questions about the religion of a respondent and the frequency of religious attendance.
Respondents had to be 16 years of age or older, have lived at their current residence for three months, and not have done a survey within the past six months. Also excluded were people with jobs related to surveys, marketing investigation, public relations, or the media. Respondents were selected using a multi-stage method from select metropolitan cities, towns, and administrative villages. The final survey was administered in 56 locales throughout China, including three municipal cities (Beijing, Shanghai, Chongqing), six provincial capital cities (Guangzhou, Nanjing, Wuhan, Hefei, Xi’an, and Chengdu) 11 regional-level cities, 16 small towns, and 20 administrative villages. Within each locale, households were sampled within neighborhoods, and neighborhoods were sampled within administratively defined total neighborhood committees (government-defined collections of neighborhoods). A KISH grid procedure was used to randomly select one respondent from each household for a face-to-face in-home interview. The dataset contained sampling weights to reflect the general population parameters in the 2006 Statistical Yearbook of China.
Starting with 7021 observations in the original dataset, we dropped the observations from respondents who “had difficulty in understanding the survey questions”, respondents who refused to answer or felt it hard to speak about their religious beliefs, as well as observations with missing values for key variables. The final dataset used in this study contained 6267 valid responses, accounting for 89.26% of the original dataset.

4. Statistical Analysis Method and Variables

4.1. Dependent Variable: Self-Reported Happiness (SRH)

The dependent variable in this study was self-reported happiness. The survey measured the subjective happiness by asking respondents “Do you feel happy about your life overall? Are you very happy, somewhat happy, somewhat unhappy, or very unhappy?” Responses to this questions were ranked on a five-point scale from 1 (very unhappy) to 5 (very happy). This global measure of happiness as subjective well-being has been shown to be valid in large sample surveys (Brockmann et al. 2009; Layard 2011; Reyes-García et al. 2016). The same measure of subjective well-being has also been adopted by the CGSS database of China and has yielded broadly consistent results in the literature (Chen and Williams 2016).

4.2. Explanatory Variables

Religious affiliation. The religious affiliation of the respondents was identified by a question “Regardless of whether you have been to churches or temples, do you believe in a religion?” More than 20% of the respondents reported a religion they believed in. If the answer was “Yes”, the respondent was further asked to indicate the specific affiliation. Buddhism, Protestantism, Islam, Catholicism, Daoism, and Confucianism were recorded in the original dataset of the SLSC. As expected, Buddhism was the largest group, and accounted for about 17% of the final sample population. Protestantism was the second largest, and accounted for about 2.41%. Together, Buddhists and Protestants accounted for about 94.4% of individuals with religion affiliations. Each of the other religious affiliations (Islam, Daoism, Catholicism, and Confucianism) accounted for less than 0.5% of the sample population. Because the observation numbers of these religious affiliations were insufficient for conducting valid regression analyses (Chen and Williams 2016), we did not identify religious affiliations other than Buddhism and Protestantism. Two dummy variables indicated a respondent claiming to be a believer of “Buddhism” or “Protestantism”.
Importance of religion. An interview question asked respondents “Please tell me the importance of the following items in your life. Is it very important, somewhat important, somewhat unimportant, or not at all important?” Religion is listed together with other items such as family, friends, entertainment, politics, and career. The importance of religion is recorded with a four-point scale from 1 (very important) to 4 (not important at all). To capture the potential effect clearly, we constructed a dummy variable to identify respondents who considered religion “very important”.
Frequent religious attendance. An interview question first asked respondents “Did you worship in a conventional religious setting (regardless of it being a temple or a church building) during past 12 months?”, then asked about the frequency as “regularly or occasionally”. We only identified respondents who had regular religious attendance. The base group is “have no or occasional religious attendance”.
Ever practice praying. An interview question asked “Do you ever pray? Have you ever tried to communicate with God or a certain supernatural power, asking for blessings and protection?” A dummy variable took the value of 1 if the answer was “yes”.
Residency. This survey dataset had a variable indicating if the respondents lived in an urban, town, or rural setting at the time of the survey. A qualified respondent should have continuously occupied the residency during the past three months. According to the Rural Urban Classification of China (National Bureau of Statistics of China 2010)1, urban areas (code 100) include cities, counties and townships established according to the Administrative Division System of China. Town (code 210) is a central area in rural environment, serving as a regional economic, cultural, and service center without an administrative establishment. Rural (code 220) refers to the villages and rest areas in the non-urban environment. Since town areas are the transition areas between the typical urban and rural environments, we adopted a dummy variable of “town area” to control the potential mixed effects in these areas, so that the net effects in typical urban and rural areas could be clearly captured.

4.3. Control Variables

Socio-demographic variables were included to control for their potential confounding effects on the self-reported happiness. The survey dataset contained information about respondents’ genders, ages (measured in years), ethnic groups, marital statuses, education levels, occupations (unemployed status included), household economic statuses, and self-rated health statuses.
This study adopted self-rated health as a measure of general health status. A respondent’s self-rated health status is widely considered as a valid proxy for health and a reliable predictor of future health outcomes such as morbidity and mortality (Idler and Benyamini 1997; Stavrova 2015).
To control for the impacts of social capital on the well-being of respondents, we constructed a dummy variable of a social capital indicator based on the survey question “Did you attend any of the following events in the past twelve months?” The events are listed as attending other people’s weddings, charity events, various social events, vacations, or entertainments events with family or friends and so on. If the respondent said “yes” to any of the events listed above, the social capital indicator variable takes the value of 1.

4.4. Statistical Analysis Method

Ordered logistic regression may be applied when a dependent variable has more than two categories and the values of each category have a meaningful sequential order where a value is indeed “higher” than the previous one. In this study, the dependent variable was the five-point variable “happiness”. We performed model tests in STATA, and an insignificant test statistic indicated that the final model did not violate the proportional odds/parallel lines assumption. Therefore, we could apply the command svy: ologit in STATA 14 statistical package (Stata Corp LP, College Station, TX, USA) to analyze the final sample.
First, the baseline model of our empirical analysis examined the relationship between happiness and general religious beliefs, controlling for potential demographic and socioeconomic confounding factors. We also examined how self-reported happiness varied among different religiously affiliated groups.
Second, to examine how an association with happiness may vary among respondents in urban and rural areas, we included the interaction terms of religious affiliation and residential status (urban and rural).
As a robustness check, we replaced religious affiliations with variables measuring inner religiosity (“importance of religion”) and religious practice (“frequency of religious attendance”, “practice praying”). The interaction terms of these religiosity measurements and residential statuses (urban and rural) were also constructed, respectively.

5. Results and Analysis

Table 1 reports the descriptive statistics of the dataset. The subsamples of with/without religions are divided according to the religious affiliations reported. Across the various demographic and socioeconomic variables, there was no significant difference between the groups with religion and without religion. This is despite the fact that the religious group was a population with a 3% higher elementary education level and contained 3.78% fewer respondents with a college-and-above education. The results also show that the religious group was more likely to have social capital. Respondents in East China, South China, or Shanghai City were more likely to report themselves to have religious beliefs than those from the rest of the areas in China. This difference was due in part to the historical fact that Protestantism originally spread into China from eastern and southern coastal cities.
Table 2 reports the results of the basic model of ordered logistic regression analysis, testing the association between SRH and religious affiliations. Column (1) reports the results analyzing the basic factors of reporting happiness among the respondents when no religiosity variables are included. It is noticeable that the odds of reporting a higher level of happiness among rural residents were 0.718 of (or 28.2% lower than) the odds of urban residents (p < 0.05), while the odds of a town resident were insignificant. Similar patterns of the odds ratios between urban and rural residents exist in Column (2) and (3) when religiosity variables are included. Column (2) indicates that the odds of reporting a higher level of happiness among people with a religious affiliation were 1.227 of (or 22.7% higher than) the odds of those who reported to have no religion (p < 0.01). Column (3) reports the results when the specific religious affiliations, Buddhism and Protestantism, were disentangled. The odds ratios of Buddhism and Protestantism were 1.175 (p < 0.05) and 1.399 (p < 0.05) respectively.
Table 3 reports the results examining how religiosity and religious practices are significantly associated with a higher level of happiness. “Religion being very important” has a highly significant association with a higher level of happiness (odds ratio = 1.630, p < 0.01). Other control variables are the same as in the basic model reported in Table 2.
Table 4 reports how the association of religiosity and happiness varied among respondent groups in urban, town, and rural residents. The base group includes the urban residents without religion. Despite affiliations with Buddhism and Protestantism, no significantly higher level of self-reported happiness was identified among urban respondents with religious beliefs. However, rural respondents with religious beliefs had a significantly much higher likelihood of reporting a higher level of happiness (odds ratio = 1.646, p < 0.01). The effects were significant among both rural Buddhists (odds ratio = 1.324, p < 0.1) and rural Protestants (odds ratio = 1.709, p < 0.1).
We further did a robustness check by including interaction terms of residential statuses and religious practice indicators such as “religion very important”, “regular religious attendance”, and “ever praying”. The results are reported in Table 5 and the base group refers to those considering religion as not being important. Again, our results indicate that among the respondents with urban residency, none of the religiosity or religious practice activities were associated with a higher level of happiness. Among the town respondents, only the odds ratio of “religion very important” was as large as 2.336 (p < 0.01). However, highly significant and strong associations were again identified among rural religious respondents.

6. Discussion and Conclusions

General association. The results of this study suggest that religious activities (measured by self-identified religious affiliation, religious attendance, or practice) are significantly associated with a higher level of happiness among respondents in China, despite an atheistic social environment and regulations. These findings are generally consistent with the findings in Western societies where religion is a social norm, as well as in peer studies using Chinese national representative samples (Chen and Williams 2016; He et al. 2016; Lu and Zhang 2016; Zhang et al. 2016; Lu and Gao 2017).
Urban versus rural. We found no significant association between religious activities and happiness among urban respondents, but found a strong positive association among rural respondents generally. This finding is consistent with our prediction based on the secularization hypothesis of religion (Norris and Inglehart 2011). Several specific working channels of religion may be considered under the context of China:
(1)
The social network effect of religion could be applied (Lim and Putnam 2010; Krause 2003). An individual’s social network can provide social support, social identification, and social capital to him or her, hence helping to maintain psychological well-being (Halbesleben 2006; Hobfoll et al. 1990). Urban residents in China, especially those in central cities, enjoy a much more advanced social network and opportunities than rural residents (Meng and Chen 2014; Guang et al. 2010). Therefore, the marginal benefits of a social network brought by religious affiliation could be insignificant among urban residents. Meanwhile, rural residents develop their own social relationships based on a kinship network (Steinhardt 2012; Xu et al. 2010) within a narrow range and with limited resources (Meng and Chen 2014; Kipnis 1997). Therefore, religious involvement may help to break the limitation of a kinship network, providing new personal networks and social ties among rural residents (Lim and Putnam 2010).
(2)
Stress coping skills could be obtained. Rural residents in China have a low level of education and few continuous education opportunities, but through religious participation and social activities they might learn better coping skills and psychological supports (Kesebir and Diener 2008). A religious rural resident might be more likely to be satisfied with a materialistically simple life advocated by religious belief, even though the material resources are not comparable to those of an urban resident. Coping skills may be especially helpful among rural residents with chronic diseases, alleviating the psychological burdens of disease.
(3)
Promoting believers’ self-worth and identification (Haslam et al. 2009; Krause and Wulff 2005). Since the social statuses of rural residents in China re generally considered to be at a lower level in the social hierarchy of China, and individuals have long suffered social discrimination (Davey et al. 2009), religion—either Buddhism or Christianity—could help to promote self-worth and self-esteem among rural followers by advocating social equality, inner peace, and forgiveness.
Comparison with the findings of existing studies. Utilizing the same SLSC dataset and logistic models, Lu and Gao (2017) provided the closest existing study to this one. In Lu and Gao (2017), religious identity is measured by whether respondents self-identify as believers of any of the five officially recognized religions, and the authors find that having a religious identity in general is not significantly associated with happiness. This result contradicts our findings, that people identifying as Protestants or Buddhists are more likely to report a higher level of happiness. Despite using the same dataset, the data management in our study is different from Lu and Gao (2017) in several ways. First, we dropped the observations if a respondent was marked as “Had difficulty understanding the survey questions”. Second, we included area fixed effects in all regressions. It is well known that there are very large differences in economic development levels and diversities in the local social cultures among nationwide areas in China.

7. Implications

In each modern society there are rising demands for better health. In China in particular, there is a trend of residents pursuing higher levels of happiness or peak performance of subjective well-being after their basic living needs have been met. In a rapidly ageing society like China, due to the challenges from chronic disease burdens, psychological and/or spiritual needs or coping skills are also in high demanded. Religion can transcend the cognition of patients through the provision of hope, meaning, and life purpose, thus mitigating the difficulty and psychic discomfort of uncertainty regarding the outcomes of certain illnesses (Mueller et al. 2001).
It is expected that the socioeconomic inequality between the urban and rural areas in China will continue to exist. Since rural residents in China can obtain a significantly larger positive effect of happiness enhancement through religious affiliation, it will be necessary for Chinese society to allow free spaces to engage in religious activities, especially for the rural residents, before the social welfare in rural areas can be improved sufficiently.

8. Limitations and Future Study

First, there was a small sample issue among the religious respondents. Despite that the final sample containing 6267 observations, only about 20% of the respondents reported “religious affiliation” and only about 2.41% reported as Christian. The observation numbers of the rural religious believers were also relatively small.
Second, the SLSC dataset contained no information about respondents’ personality characteristics or about the inclinations or motivations of a respondent to practice religion. It provided no information about lifestyle, such as eating habits, risky health behaviors (e.g., drinking or smoking), or exercise. Therefore, the results estimated may be subject to potential unobservable omitted variable biases.
Third, like many peer studies using observatory data to analyze religion and happiness, our analysis also was limited to an associative relationship, rather than being determined by causal effects. There are concerns about the endogeneity or reverse causal effect between religiosity and self-reported happiness (Chiswick and Mirtcheva 2013).
Fourth, there may have be measurement errors and nonrandom missing values in the dataset. Some respondents may have tended not to report their religious beliefs under the general atheistic social environment in China where religion is not a social norm (Lu and Zhang 2016; Wenzel-Teuber 2017), or people may not have had a correct understanding about the terminology “religion” (Wei and Liu 2013). Additionally, some scholars have argued that the varied conclusions of the relationship between religion and happiness depend on the measurement of happiness. Therefore, future studies should concern social background and develop a more highly valid and reliable measurement of religion and happiness for people in a Chinese social context.

Author Contributions

J.H.Z.: Writing—original draft, Data curation. W.C.Z.: Funding acquisition, Writing—original draft. X.Y.J.: Writing—review & editing.

Funding

The Project Supported by Faculty Research Grants of Macau University of Science and Technology (Grant No. FRG-17-011-MSB).

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Biswas-Diener, Robert, Joar Vittersø, and Ed Diener. 2005. Most people are pretty happy, but there is cultural variation: The Inughuit, the Amish, and the Maasai. Journal of Happiness Studies 6: 205–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Brockmann, Hilke, Jan Delhey, Christian Welzel, and Hao Yuan. 2009. The china puzzle: Falling happiness in a rising economy. Journal of Happiness Studies 10: 387–405. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Brown, Philip H., and Brian Tierney. 2009. Religion and subjective well-being among the elderly in china. Journal of Socio-Economics 38: 310–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Büssing, Arndt, and Harold G. Koenig. 2010. Spiritual needs of patients with chronic diseases. Religions 1: 18–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Chau, Adam Yuet. 2010. Religion in Contemporary China: Revitalization and Innovation. Abingdon: Routledge. [Google Scholar]
  6. Chen, Yunsong, and Mark Williams. 2016. Subjective well-being in the new China: Religion, social capital, and social status. The British Journal of Sociology 67: 719–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Cheung, Felix. 2016. Can income inequality be associated with positive outcomes? Hope mediates the positive inequality–happiness link in rural China. Social Psychological and Personality Science 7: 320–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Chiswick, Barry R., and Donka M. Mirtcheva. 2013. Religion and child health: Religious affiliation, importance, and attendance and health status among American youth. Journal of Family and Economic Issues 34: 120–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Cohen, Adam B., and Kathryn A. Johnson. 2017. The relation between religion and well-being. Applied Research in Quality of Life 12: 533–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Davey, Gareth, Zhenghui Chen, and Anna Lau. 2009. ‘Peace in a thatched hut—That is happiness’: Subjective wellbeing among peasants in rural China. Journal of Happiness Studies 10: 239–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Deaton, Angus, and Arthur A. Stone. 2013. Two happiness puzzles. American Economic Review 103: 591–97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Deng, Jianjun, Tsingan Li, Jingya Wang, and Ruiping Zhang. 2019. Optimistically Accepting Suffering Boosts Happiness: Associations Between Buddhism Patience, Selflessness, and Subjective Authentic-Durable Happiness. Journal of Happiness Studies, 1–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Devine, Joe, Timothy Hinks, and Arif Naveed. 2019. Happiness in Bangladesh: The role of religion and connectedness. Journal of Happiness Studies 20: 351–71. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Diener, Ed, and Robert Biswas-Diener. 2002. Will money increase subjective well-being? Social Indicators Research 57: 119–69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Diener, Ed, and Martin E. P. Seligman. 2002. Very happy people. Psychological Science 13: 81–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Diener, Ed, Richard E. Lucas, and Shigehiro Oishi. 2002. Subjective well-being: The science of happiness and life satisfaction. In Handbook of Positive Psychology. New York: Oxford University Press, vol. 2, pp. 63–73. [Google Scholar]
  17. Diener, Ed, Louis Tay, and David Gershom Myers. 2011. The religion paradox: If religion makes people happy, why are so many dropping out? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 101: 1278–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  18. Fariddanesh, Marjan, and Ali Mohammad Rezaei. 2019. Predicting the Happiness of Adolescents Based on Coping Styles and Religious Attitudes. Journal of Religion and Health 58: 537–53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  19. Graham, Carol, and Stefano Pettinato. 2002. Frustrated achievers: Winners, losers and subjective well-being in new market economies. Journal of Development Studies 38: 100–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Graham, Carol, Shaojie Zhou, and Junyi Zhang. 2017. Happiness and health in China: The Paradox of progress. World Development 96: 231–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Greeley, Andrew, and Michael Hout. 2006. Happiness and lifestyle among conservative Christians. The Truth about Conservative Christians 1: 150–61. [Google Scholar]
  22. Guang, Lei, Jennifer Adams, Arianne Gaetano, Martin King Whyte, Björn A. Gustafsson, Emily Hannum, and Si-Ming Li. 2010. One Country, Two Societies: Rural-Urban Inequality in Contemporary China. Cambridge: Harvard University Press, vol. 16. [Google Scholar]
  23. Halbesleben, Jonathon R. B. 2006. Sources of social support and burnout: A meta-analytic test of the conservation of resources model. Journal of Applied Psychology 91: 1134–45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Haslam, S. Alexander, Jolanda Jetten, Tom Postmes, and Catherine Haslam. 2009. Social identity, health and well-being: An emerging agenda for applied psychology. Applied Psychology: An International Review 58: 1–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. He, Jianhui, Sawitri Assanangkornchai, Le Cai, and Edward McNeil. 2016. Disparities in drinking patterns and risks among ethnic majority and minority groups in China: The roles of acculturation, religion, family and friends. Drug and Alcohol Dependence 159: 198–206. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Hobfoll, Stevan E., Jolanda Jetten, Tom Postmes, and Catherine Haslam. 1990. Conservation of social resources: Social support resource theory. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships 7: 465–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Idler, Ellen L., and Yael Benyamini. 1997. Self-rated health and mortality: A review of twenty-seven community studies. Journal of Health and Social Behavior 38: 21–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Inglehart, Ronald F. 2010. Faith and freedom: Traditional and modern ways to happiness. In International Differences in Well-Being. New York: Oxford University Press, pp. 351–97. [Google Scholar]
  29. Kesebir, Pelin, and Ed Diener. 2008. In pursuit of happiness: Empirical answers to philosophical questions. Perspectives on Psychological Science 3: 117–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Kipnis, Andrew B. 1997. Producing Guanxi: Sentiment, Self, and Subculture in a North China Village. Durham: Duke University Press. [Google Scholar]
  31. Knight, John, and Ramani Gunatilaka. 2010. Great expectations? The subjective well-being of rural-urban migrants in China. World Development 38: 113–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Knight, John, Lina Song, and Ramani Gunatilaka. 2009. Subjective well-being and its determinants in rural China. China Economic Review 20: 635–49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Krause, Neal. 2003. Religious meaning and subjective well-being in late life. The Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences 58: 160–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Krause, Neal, and Keith M. Wulff. 2005. Church-Based Social Ties, A Sense of Belonging in a Congregation, and Physical Health Status. The International Journal for the Psychology of Religion 15: 73–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Layard, Richard. 2011. Happiness: Lessons from a New Science. London: Penguin UK. [Google Scholar]
  36. Lewis, Christopher Alan, and Sharon Mary Cruise. 2006. Religion and happiness: Consensus, contradictions, comments and concerns. Mental Health, Religion and Culture 9: 213–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Liang, Zhenhua, and Gubo Qi. 2015. The religious construction of illness: Understanding the behavior and motivation of farmers’ illness-caused religious conversion: Evidence from a Christian church in rural Henan. China Agricultural University Journal of Social Sciences Edition 32: 39–47. [Google Scholar]
  38. Liang, Ying, and Peigang Wang. 2014. Influence of prudential value on the subjective well-being of Chinese urban–rural residents. Social Indicators Research 118: 1249–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Lim, Chaeyoon, and Robert D. Putnam. 2010. Religion, Social Networks, and Life Satisfaction. American Sociological Review 75: 914–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Lim, Hock-Eam, Daigee Shaw, Pei-Shan Liao, and Hongbo Duan. 2019. The Effects of Income on Happiness in East and South Asia: Societal Values Matter? Journal of Happiness Studies, 1–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Liu, Eric Yang, and Harold Koenig. 2013. Measuring Intrinsic Religiosity: Scales for use in mental health studies in China—A research report. Mental Health, Religion & Culture 16: 215–24. [Google Scholar]
  42. Liu, Eric Y., Harold Koenig, and Dedong Wei. 2011. Discovering a blissful island: Religious involvement and happiness in Taiwan. Sociology of Religion 73: 46–68. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Lu, Yunfeng. 2012. Understanding the Rise of Religion in China. Chinese Sociological Review 45: 3–7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Lu, Jun, and Qin Gao. 2017. Faith and happiness in China: Roles of religious identity, beliefs, and practice. Social Indicators Research 132: 273–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Lu, Yun Feng, and Chun Ni Zhang. 2016. An observation of Christianity in contemporary China: Based on the data of CGSS and CFPS. World Religious Cultures 1: 34–46. [Google Scholar]
  46. Lu, Luo, Robin Gilmour, and Shu-Fang Kao. 2001. Cultural values and happiness: An East-West dialogue. Journal of Social Psychology 141: 477–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Lun, Vivian Miu-Chi, and Michael Harris Bond. 2013. Examining the relation of religion and spirituality to subjective well-being across national cultures. Psychology of Religion and Spirituality 5: 304–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. McCullough, Michael E., and Brian L. B. Willoughby. 2009. Religion, self-regulation, and self-control: Associations, explanations, and implications. Psychological Bulletin 135: 69–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Meng, Tianguang, and He Chen. 2014. A multilevel analysis of social capital and self-rated health: Evidence from China. Health & Place 27: 38–44. [Google Scholar]
  50. Mueller, Paul S., David J. Plevak, and Teresa A. Rummans. 2001. Religious in volvement, spirituality, and medicine: Implications for clinical practice. Mayo Clinic Proceedings 76: 1225–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Myers, David G. 2000. The funds, friends, and faith of happy people. American Psychologist 55: 56–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. National Bureau of Statistics of China. 2010. China Statistical Year Book 2010; Beijing: China Statistics Press.
  53. Norris, Pippa, and Ronald Inglehart. 2011. Sacred and Secular: Religion and Politics Worldwide. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
  54. Overmyer, Daniel L., and Richard Edmonds, eds. 2003. Religion in China Today (No. 3). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
  55. Potter, Pitman B. 2003. Belief in control: Regulation of religion in China. The China Quarterly 174: 317–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Reyes-García, Victoria, Ronnie Babigumira, Aili Pyhälä, Sven Wunder, Francisco Zorondo-Rodríguez, and Arild Angelsen. 2016. Subjective wellbeing and income: Empirical patterns in the rural developing world. Journal of Happiness Studies 17: 773–91. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Rizvi, Mohd Ahsan Kabir, and Mohammad Zakir Hossain. 2017. Relationship between religious belief and happiness: A systematic literature review. Journal of Religion and Health 56: 1561–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Sander, William. 2017. Religion, religiosity, and happiness. Review of Religious Research 59: 251–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Stark, Rodney, and Eric Y. Liu. 2011. The religious awakening in China. Review of Religious Research 52: 282–89. [Google Scholar]
  60. Stark, Rodney, and Jared Maier. 2008. Faith and happiness. Review of Religious Research 50: 120–25. [Google Scholar]
  61. Stavrova, Olga. 2015. Religion, self-rated health, and mortality: Whether religiosity delays death depends on the cultural context. Social Psychological and Personality Science 6: 911–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Steinhardt, H. Christoph. 2012. How is high trust in China possible? Comparing the origins of generalized trust in three Chinese societies. Political Studies 60: 434–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Thege, Barna K., János Pilling, András Székely, and Mária S. Kopp. 2013. Relationship between religiosity and health: Evidence from a post-communist country. International Journal of Behavioral Medicine 20: 477–86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Treiman, Donald J. 2012. The “difference between heaven and earth”: Urban–rural disparities in well-being in China. Research in Social Stratification and Mobility 30: 33–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Wang, Zhizhong, Harold G. Koenig, Yuhong Zhang, Wanrui Ma, and Yueqin Huang. 2015. Religious involvement and mental disorders in mainland China. PLoS ONE 10: e0128800. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Wei, Dedong, and Eric Y. Liu. 2013. Religious involvement and depression: Evidence for curvilinear and stress-moderating effects among young women in rural China. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion 52: 349–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Wenzel-Teuber, Katharina. 2017. Statistics on religions and churches in the people’s republic of China: Update for the year 2016. Religions and Christianity in Today’s China 7: 26–53. [Google Scholar]
  68. Wielander, Gerda. 2017. Christianity and Positive Psychology-Are ‘Western’ Spiritual Practices Conquering the Chinese Spirit? In Eastspirit: Transnational Spirituality and Religious Circulation in East and West. Aylesbury: Brill, pp. 172–91. [Google Scholar]
  69. Xu, Qingwen, Douglas D. Perkins, and Julian Chun-Chung Chow. 2010. Sense of community, neighboring, and social capital as predictors of local political participation in China. American Journal of Community Psychology 45: 259–71. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Yang, Fenggang. 2006. The red, black, and gray markets of religion in China. The Sociological Quarterly 47: 93–122. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Ying, Zheng, Shibao Liu, Shuming Bao, and Jianbo Zhou. 2017. Religious diversity and regional development in China. China Economic Review 46: 1–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Zhang, Hong, Zhiqin Sang, Darius K.-S. Chan, Fei Teng, Man Liu, Shuo Yu, and Yuan Tian. 2016. Sources of meaning in life among Chinese university students. Journal of Happiness Studies 17: 1473–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  73. Zhao, Wei. 2012. Economic inequality, status perceptions, and subjective well-being in china’s transitional economy. Research in Social Stratification and Mobility 30: 433–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  74. Zheng, Hong’e, and Wei Wang. 2014. Chinese rural Christian concept of disease and medical treatment: A case study of a villages in Shandong province. The World Religious Cultures 1: 98–101. [Google Scholar]
  75. Zhou, Shaojie, and Qiuyun Sun. 2017. The psychology of peasant religious conversion for the purpose of disease control: The role of “belief” in understanding Chinese rural religious practices. Chinese Journal of Sociology 37: 1–31. [Google Scholar]
1
National Bureau of Statistics of China, The Rural Urban Classification, 2011, (available at http://www.stats.gov.cn/tjsj/pcsj/rkpc/5rp/, last access on 24 August 2019).
Table 1. Descriptive Statistics.
Table 1. Descriptive Statistics.
VariablesFull SampleUrbanTownRural
N%N%N%N%
HappinessVery unhappy240.38130.4230.1880.52
Unhappy1983.16892.89372.24724.69
So-so96115.3349616.1021813.2024716.09
Happy298047.55158351.4072744.0167043.65
Very happy210433.5789929.1966740.3853835.05
Self-reported HealthVery unhealthy330.53150.4950.30130.85
Unhealthy2383.80963.12462.78966.25
So-so63210.0835311.461338.051469.51
Healthy210933.65117638.1846628.2146730.42
Very healthy325551.94144046.75100260.6581352.96
With religionYes129120.6261219.8735821.6732120.91
ReligiousBuddhism106817.0450016.2329918.1026917.52
affiliationProtestantism1512.41732.37342.06442.87
Islam300.48130.42171.0300.00
Daoism190.30110.3630.1850.33
Catholicism120.1980.2620.1220.13
Confucianism110.1870.2330.1810.07
Importance of religionVery important1752.79682.21623.75452.93
Somewhat important5869.353059.901599.621227.95
Somewhat unimportant129620.6869222.473472125716.74
Not important at all/Don’t know421067.18201565.42108479.00111172.38
Have you ever prayed?Yes71011.3340413.1218511.201217.88
Worship regularlyYes1852.95792.56754.54312.02
Demographic and socioeconomic characteristics
GenderMale300547.95147948.0280048.4372647.30
Age * 39.87(13.60)40.12(13.74)38.10(12.78)41.28(14.05)
Marriage statusMarried/cohabiting508581.14243479.03133981.05131285.47
Divorced/separated/widowed2644.211474.77603.63573.71
Unmarried91814.6549916.2025315.3116610.81
Ethnic groupHan (Yes)600995.88
Education levelNo schooling2363.77722.34362.181288.34
Elementary71711.441745.651438.6640026.06
Junior middle school208233.2283927.2458335.2966043
High school207433.09120038.9658335.2929118.96
College and above115818.4879525.8130718.58563.65
Economic StatusLower72811.6235311.461458.7823014.98
Middle394462.93198764.51101961.6893861.11
Middle-high147723.5769522.5644827.1233421.76
High1181.88451.46402.42332.15
UnemployedYes3445.492217.181237.4500
Social capitalYes441370.4296431.3040024.2149031.92
Regions in ChinaEast China3195.091524.94985.93694.50
South China101116.131534.9732119.4353734.98
Central China124619.8855317.9531819.2537524.43
North China99615.8972523.5419111.56805.21
Northwest China2684.281605.191086.5430419.80
Southwest China102016.2843113.9928517.2517011.07
Northeast China89714.3139612.8633120.04--
Beijing Area2574.102578.34----
Shanghai Area2534.042538.21----
Total 6267100308010016521001535100
*: Age is a continuous variable with the means reported and the standard deviation in parentheses.
Table 2. Association between happiness and religion (basic model; ordered logistic model, odds ratio reported).
Table 2. Association between happiness and religion (basic model; ordered logistic model, odds ratio reported).
(1)(2)(3)
VariablesHappinessHappinessHappiness
Ref. GroupUrban residentsUrban residents w/t religionUrban residents w/t religion + other affiliations
With Religious affiliation 1.227 ***
(0.0870)
Buddhism 1.175 **
(0.0824)
Protestantism 1.399 **
(0.231)
Town resident0.9900.9820.991
(0.164)(0.163)(0.164)
Rural resident0.718 **0.714 **0.715 **
(0.0998)(0.0993)(0.0994)
Health1.972 ***1.967 ***1.970 ***
(0.0649)(0.0648)(0.0649)
Social Capital1.117 *1.103 *1.102 *
(0.0648)(0.0642)(0.0643)
Middle economic status1.707 ***1.698 ***1.700 ***
(0.141)(0.140)(0.140)
Middle-high economic status2.654 ***2.639 ***2.644 ***
(0.257)(0.255)(0.256)
High economic status3.747 ***3.731 ***3.760 ***
(0.797)(0.794)(0.800)
Unemployed0.9050.9060.906
(0.102)(0.102)(0.102)
Age1.0021.0021.002
(0.00251)(0.00251)(0.00251)
Male0.792 ***0.800 ***0.802 ***
(0.0404)(0.0409)(0.0410)
Divorced, separated, or widowed1.1461.1481.142
(0.0966)(0.0967)(0.0962)
Unmarried0.514 ***0.511 ***0.510 ***
(0.0785)(0.0780)(0.0778)
Han Ethnic Group1.2091.2311.203
(0.176)(0.179)(0.175)
Elementary0.736 **0.734 **0.736 **
(0.112)(0.112)(0.112)
Junior middle school0.8020.8060.809
(0.117)(0.118)(0.118)
High school0.9450.9520.957
(0.144)(0.145)(0.146)
College and above (Graduate included)0.9210.9290.932
(0.151)(0.153)(0.153)
Area dummiesYESYESYES
Constant cut10.0791 ***0.0804 ***0.0801 ***
(0.0286)(0.0291)(0.0290)
Constant cut20.8070.8200.818
(0.248)(0.252)(0.252)
Constant cut36.178 ***6.287 ***6.267 ***
(1.880)(1.913)(1.908)
Constant cut479.25 ***80.87 ***80.60 ***
(24.48)(24.98)(24.91)
Observations626762676267
Pseudo R-squared0.1000.1010.101
Note: (1) The results are sample-weight adjusted. (2) Standard error reported in parentheses. *** p < 0.01, ** p < 0.05, * p < 0.1. 3) Though not reported here, area fixed effects are controlled in each regression.
Table 3. The association between religiosity/religious practices and happiness (ordered logistic model, odds ratios reported).
Table 3. The association between religiosity/religious practices and happiness (ordered logistic model, odds ratios reported).
(1)(2)(3)
VariablesHappinessHappinessHappiness
Religion being very important (Yes)1.630 ***
(0.254)
Regular religious attendance (Yes) 1.292 *
(0.197)
Ever practice praying (Yes) 1.192 **
(0.0969)
Town resident0.9940.9991.004
(0.163)(0.164)(0.165)
Rural resident0.724 **0.725 **0.721 **
(0.0990)(0.0990)(0.0985)
Observations626762676267
Pseudo R-squared0.1010.1000.100
Note: (1) The results are sample-weight adjusted. (2) Standard error reported in parentheses. *** p < 0.01, ** p < 0.05, * p < 0.1. (3) Not reported; however, all regressions have controlled demographic characteristics, socioeconomic feature variables, and area fixed effects that are the same as shown in Table 2. (4) Four constant cuts were included in the ordinary logistic regressions, though not reported in the table above.
Table 4. The association between happiness and religious affiliations among urban, town, or rural residents (ordered logistic model, odds ratios reported).
Table 4. The association between happiness and religious affiliations among urban, town, or rural residents (ordered logistic model, odds ratios reported).
(1) (2)
VariablesHappiness Happiness
Urban without religionRef.Urban without religion + other affiliationsRef.
Religious0.983Buddhist1.015
urban(0.109)urban(0.111)
Religious1.377 **Buddhist1.390 **
town(0.186)town(0.188)
Religious1.646 ***Buddhist1.324 *
rural(0.243)rural(0.191)
Protestant1.004
urban(0.276)
Protestant2.256 **
town(0.815)
Protestant1.709 *
rural(0.525)
Town0.956Town0.950
(0.159) (0.158)
Rural0.663 ***Rural0.686 ***
(0.0928) (0.0964)
Observations6267 6267
Pseudo R-squared0.101 0.101
Note: (1) The results are sample-weight adjusted. (2) Standard error reported in parentheses. *** p < 0.01, ** p < 0.05, * p < 0.1. (3) Not reported; however, all regressions have controlled demographic characteristics, socioeconomic feature variables, and area fixed effects that are the same as shown in Table 2. (4) Four constant cuts were included in the ordinary logistic regressions, though not reported in the table above.
Table 5. The association between religiosity/religious practices and happiness among various residential statuses (ordered logistic model, odds ratios reported).
Table 5. The association between religiosity/religious practices and happiness among various residential statuses (ordered logistic model, odds ratios reported).
(1) (2) (3)
VariablesHappiness Happiness Happiness
Religion not important (Ref.)
Urban residency X Religion very important0.855Urban residency X Regular relig. attend0.918Urban residency X ever praying1.076
(0.229) (0.228) (0.132)
Town residency X Religion very important2.336 ***Town residency X Regular relig. attend1.418Town residency X ever praying1.244
(0.624) (0.353) (0.198)
Rural residency X Religion very important1.916 **Rural residency X Regular relig. attend2.312 **Rural residency X ever praying1.517 **
(0.613) (0.858) (0.297)
Observations6267Observations6267Observations6267
Pseudo R-squared0.101Pseudo R-squared0.101Pseudo R-squared0.101
Note: (1) The results are sample-weight adjusted. (2) Standard error reported in parentheses. *** p < 0.01, ** p < 0.05. (3) Not reported; however, all regressions have controlled demographic characteristics, socioeconomic feature variables and area fixed effects that are same as shown in Table 2. (4) Four constant cuts were included in the ordinary logistic regressions, though not reported in the table above.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Zhang, J.H.; Zou, W.C.; Jiang, X.Y. One Religion, Two Tales: Religion and Happiness in Urban and Rural Areas of China. Religions 2019, 10, 532. https://doi.org/10.3390/rel10090532

AMA Style

Zhang JH, Zou WC, Jiang XY. One Religion, Two Tales: Religion and Happiness in Urban and Rural Areas of China. Religions. 2019; 10(9):532. https://doi.org/10.3390/rel10090532

Chicago/Turabian Style

Zhang, Jing Hua, Wen Chi Zou, and Xiao Yang Jiang. 2019. "One Religion, Two Tales: Religion and Happiness in Urban and Rural Areas of China" Religions 10, no. 9: 532. https://doi.org/10.3390/rel10090532

APA Style

Zhang, J. H., Zou, W. C., & Jiang, X. Y. (2019). One Religion, Two Tales: Religion and Happiness in Urban and Rural Areas of China. Religions, 10(9), 532. https://doi.org/10.3390/rel10090532

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop