3.2.1. CATI Subscales as Covariates
Table 4 summarises the MANCOVA results for the CATI subscales as covariates and SDQ and WBPQ items as dependent variables. When the overall significance of the CATI subscales was assessed, ‘CATI_SOC’ (Wilks’ Λ = 0.95,
p < 0.05), ‘CATI_CAM’ (Wilks’ Λ = 0.95,
p < 0.05), and ‘CATI_REP’ (Wilks’ Λ = 0.86,
p < 0.001) had a statistically significant effect for the SDQ items. More specifically, more social interactions were associated with more emotional problems (β = 0.720, F = 6.15,
p < 0.05) and fewer peer problems (β = −0.590, F = 7.74,
p < 0.01). Greater social camouflaging was associated with more emotional problems (β = 0.768, F = 5.81,
p < 0.05) and fewer peer problems (β = −0.528, F = 5.15,
p < 0.05). Finally, more repetitive behaviours were associated with greater hyperactivity (β = 1.856, F = 39.83,
p < 0.001), more emotional problems (β = 0.699, F = 4.84,
p < 0.05), and reduced prosocial behaviour (β = −0.501, F = 5.60,
p < 0.05).
‘CATI_RIG’ (Wilks’ Λ = 0.92, p < 0.01) and ‘CATI_REP’ (Wilks’ Λ = 0.93, p < 0.01) had a statistically significant effect on the well-being outcomes. Greater cognitive rigidity was associated with higher negative well-being (β = 0.690, F = 6.08, p < 0.05) and greater perceived stress (β = 0.549, F = 5.232, p < 0.05). More repetitive behaviours were associated with worse physical health (β = −0.521, F = 4.14, p < 0.05) and greater levels of anxiety (β = 0.752, F = 6.41, p < 0.05).
3.2.2. Well-Being Predictors as Covariates
The significant effects of the well-being predictors are shown in
Table 5.
‘Social Support’ (Wilks’ Λ = 0.94, p < 0.005), ‘Negative Coping’ (Wilks’ Λ = 0.91, p < 0.001), ‘Resilience’ (Wilks’ Λ = 0.93, p < 0.005), ‘Psychological Capital’ (Wilks’ Λ = 0.93, p < 0.001) and ‘Student stressors’ (Wilks’ Λ = 0.89, p < 0.001) had a statistically significant effect on the SDQ items. More specifically, greater social support was associated with fewer peer problems (β = 0.226, F = 12.98, p < 0.001). Greater use of negative coping was associated with greater hyperactivity (β = 0.211, F = 8.99, p < 0.005), more emotional problems (β = 0.232, F = 15.54, p < 0.001), and more peer problems (β = 0.100, F = 4.39, p < 0.05). Greater resilience was associated with fewer peer problems (β = −0.197, F = 12.32, p < 0.001) and greater prosocial behaviour (β = 0.196, F = 13.94, p < 0.001). Higher psychological capital was associated with decreased emotional problems (β = −0.334, F = 14.00, p < 0.001). Finally, more student stressors were associated with more emotional problems (β = 0.319, F = 21.57, p < 0.001) and more prosocial problems (β = −0.177, F = 11.37, p < 0.001).
When considering the well-being outcomes, ‘Social Support’ (Wilks’ Λ = 0.86, p < 0.001), ‘Negative Coping’ (Wilks’ Λ = 0.90, p < 0.001), ‘Resilience’ (Wilks’ Λ = 0.93, p < 0.01), ‘Psychological Capacity’ (Wilks’ Λ = 0.77, p < 0.001), ‘Student stressors’ (Wilks’ Λ = 0.87, p < 0.001), ‘Academic Stress’ (Wilks’ Λ = 0.82, p < 0.001), and ‘Flow’ (Wilks’ Λ = 0.83, p < 0.001) had overall significant effects. More specifically, greater social support was associated with greater positive well-being (β = 0.212, F = 10.38, p < 0.005), greater flourishing (β = 0.181, F = 15.69, p < 0.001), better physical health (β = 0.198, F = 9.53, p < 0.005), greater life satisfaction (β = 0.187, F = 15.21 p < 0.001) and feeling less depressed (β = −0.329, F = 20.08, p < 0.001). Greater use of negative coping was associated with greater negative well-being (β = 0.208, F = 13.10, p < 0.001), higher levels of perceived stress (β = 0.162, F = 15.46, p < 0.001), and greater levels of anxiety (β = 0.204, F = 14.59, p < 0.001) and depression (β = 0.257, F = 17.45, p < 0.001). Greater resilience was associated with greater negative well-being (β = 0.135, F = 4.20, p < 0.05), better physical health (β = 0.151, F = 6.01, p < 0.05), and feeling greater levels of depression (β = 0.159, F = 5.10, p < 0.05).
High psychological capital was associated with greater positive well-being (β = 0.367, F = 20.27, p < 0.001), lower negative well-being (β = −0.282, F = 10.98, p < 0.005), greater flourishing (β = 0.436, F = 59.15, p < 0.001), better physical health (β = 0.174, F = 4.76, p < 0.05), greater life satisfaction (β = 0.304, F = 26.20, p < 0.001), lower perceived stress (β = −0.171, F = 7.92, p < 0.01), and lower levels of anxiety (β = −0.294, F = 13.85, p < 0.001) and depression (β = −0.373, F = 16.80, p < 0.001). Furthermore, more student stressors were associated with greater negative well-being (β = 0.220, F = 10.99, p < 0.005), lower flourishing (β = −0.115, F = 6.75, p < 0.05), greater perceived stress (β = 0.273, F = 32.91, p < 0.001), and feeling more anxious (β = 0.264, F = 18.36, p < 0.001) and depressed (β = 0.249, F = 12.33, p < 0.001). Greater academic stress was associated with greater negative well-being (β = 0.238, F = 12.90, p < 0.001), lower flourishing (β = −0.099, F = 5.03, p < 0.05), greater perceived stress (β = 0.318, F = 44.73, p < 0.001) and feeling more anxious (β = 0.190, F = 9.54, p < 0.005). Finally, greater flow was associated with greater flourishing (β = 0.265, F = 42.94, p < 0.001) and greater life satisfaction (β = 0.185, F = 19.20, p < 0.001).
3.2.3. Sleep Questions
Table 6 summarises the MANCOVA results for the sleep questions as covariates and SDQ and WBPQ items as dependent variables.
Amongst the Sleep Questions, ‘Difficulty Sleeping’ (Wilks’ Λ = 0.95, p < 0.01), ‘Sleep Quality’ (Wilks’ Λ = 0.95, p < 0.01) and ‘Daytime Sleepiness’ (Wilks’ Λ = 0.83, p < 0.001) had an overall statistically significant effect on SDQ items. More specifically, greater difficulty falling asleep was associated with more hyperactivity (β = 0.166, F = 6.86, p < 0.01) and more emotional problems (β = 0.167, F = 8.00, p < 0.01). Better sleep quality was associated with fewer peer problems (β = −0.213, F = 10.07, p < 0.005). Finally, more significant daytime sleepiness was associated with greater hyperactivity (β = 0.236, F = 18.05, p < 0.001) and more emotional problems (β = 0.356, F = 45.73, p < 0.001).
‘Difficulty Sleeping’ (Wilks’ Λ = 0.89, p < 0.001), ‘Sleep Quality’ (Wilks’ Λ = 0.80, p < 0.001), and ‘Daytime Sleepiness’ (Wilks’ Λ = 0.83, p < 0.001) had a statistically overall significant effect on the well-being outcomes. More specifically, greater difficulty with falling asleep was associated with more perceived stress (β = 0.132, F = 7.72, p < 0.01) and more anxiety (β = 0.125, F = 5.44, p < 0.05) and depression (β = 0.277, F = 20.51, p < 0.001). Better sleep quality was associated with greater positive well-being (β = 0.359, F = 21.96, p < 0.001), greater flourishing (β = 0.464, F = 48.38, p < 0.001), better physical health (β = 0.204, F = 8.52, p < 0.005), greater life satisfaction (β = 0.399, F = 45.08, p < 0.001), lower perceived stress (β = −0.166, F = 6.34, p < 0.05), and lower levels of anxiety (β = −0.205, F = 7.62, p < 0.01) and depression (β = −0.206, F = 7.03, p < 0.01). Finally, more daytime sleepiness was associated with greater negative well-being (β = 0.316, F = 40.62, p < 0.001), lower flourishing (β = −0.106, F = 6.20, p < 0.05), more significant perceived stress (β = 0.247, F = 34.37, p < 0.001), and more anxiety (β = 0.284, F = 35.63, p < 0.001) and depression (β = 0.242, F = 19.95, p < 0.001).
3.2.4. Effects of All Covariates in a Single Analysis
The following analyses included all the covariates in analyses of the SDQ and well-being outcomes. When all three groups of covariates, i.e., CATI subscales, well-being predictors and Sleep Questions, were assessed (as summarised in
Table 7), ‘Daytime Sleepiness’ (Wilks’ Λ = 0.94,
p < 0.05), ‘Social Support’ (Wilks’ Λ = 0.94,
p < 0.05), ‘Negative Coping’ (Wilks’ Λ = 0.95,
p < 0.05), ‘Resilience’ (Wilks’ Λ = 0.95,
p < 0.05), ‘Psychological Capital’ (Wilks’ Λ = 0.93,
p < 0.005), ‘Student Stressors’ (Wilks’ Λ = 0.90,
p < 0.001) and ‘CATI_REP’ (Wilks’ Λ = 0.87,
p < 0.001) had an overall statistically significant effect on the SDQ items.
More specifically, greater daytime sleepiness was associated with more emotional problems (β = 0.179, F = 13.11, p < 0.001). More social support was associated with fewer peer problems (β = −0.223, F = 12.03, p < 0.001). Greater use of negative coping was associated with more emotional problems (β = 0.151, F = 6.37, p < 0.05) and more peer problems (β = 0.115, F = 4.88, p < 0.05). Better resilience was associated with fewer peer problems (β = −0.174, F = 7.98, p < 0.01) and more prosocial behaviour (β = 0.160, F = 7.72, p < 0.01). Greater psychological capital was associated with fewer emotional problems (β = −0.338, F = 14.19, p < 0.001). More student stressors were associated with greater emotional problems (β = 0.310, F = 20.13, p < 0.001) and reduced prosocial behaviour (β = −0.141, F = 6.30, p < 0.05). Finally, more repetitive behaviours were associated with greater hyperactivity (β = 1.762, F =33.84, p < 0.001).
Considering the well-being outcomes (summarised in
Table 8), ‘Difficulty Sleeping’ (Wilks’ Λ = 0.91,
p < 0.005), ‘Sleep Quality’ (Wilks’ Λ = 0.92,
p < 0.05), ‘Daytime Sleepiness’ (Wilks’ Λ = 0.87,
p < 0.001), ‘Social Support’ (Wilks’ Λ = 0.88,
p < 0.001), ‘Negative Coping’ (Wilks’ Λ = 0.91,
p < 0.005), ‘Resilience’ (Wilks’ Λ = 0.93,
p < 0.05), ‘Psychological Capital’ (Wilks’ Λ = 0.80,
p < 0.001), ‘Student Stressors’ (Wilks’ Λ = 0.87,
p < 0.001) Academic Stress’ (Wilks’ Λ = 0.86,
p < 0.001), ‘Flow’ (Wilks’ Λ = 0.83,
p < 0.001), ‘Rumination’ (Wilks’ Λ = 0.93,
p < 0.05) and ‘CATI_RIG’ (Wilks’ Λ = 0.93,
p < 0.05) had overall statistically significant effects.
More specifically, greater difficulty with falling asleep was associated with more depression (β = 0.203, F = 13.81, p < 0.001). Better sleep quality was associated with greater flourishing (β = 0.152, F = 8.58, p < 0.005) and greater life satisfaction (β = 0.154, F = 7.77, p < 0.01). Greater daytime sleepiness was associated with greater negative well-being (β = 0.211, F = 18.44, p < 0.001), more perceived stress (β = 0.131, F = 13.32, p < 0.001), and feeling more anxious (β = 0.171, F = 14.06, p < 0.001) and depressed (β = 0.130, F = 6.32, p < 0.05). Greater social support was associated with greater positive well-being (β = 0.214, F = 9.04, p < 0.005), greater flourishing (β = 0.201, F = 16.41, p < 0.001), better physical health (β = 0.177, F = 5.73, p < 0.05), greater life satisfaction (β = 0.170, F = 10.39, p < 0.005) and lower levels of depression (β = −0.260, F = 11.24, p < 0.001).
Greater use of negative coping was associated with greater negative well-being (β = 0.141, F = 5.68, p < 0.05), lower flourishing (β = −0.112, F = 7.91, p < 0.01), more perceived stress (β = 0.137, F = 10.02, p < 0.005), and more anxiety (β = 0.179, F = 10.60, p < 0.005) and depression (β = 0.215, F = 11.90, p < 0.001). Greater resilience was associated with better physical health (β = 0.195, F = 7.90, p < 0.01) and greater life satisfaction (β = 0.119, F = 5.76, p < 0.05). High psychological capital was associated with greater positive well-being (β = 0.304, F = 13.07, p < 0.001), lower negative well-being (β = −0.211, F = 5.85, p < 0.05), greater flourishing (β = 0.388, F = 43.98, p < 0.001), greater life satisfaction (β = 0.283, F = 20.67, p < 0.001), and lower levels of anxiety (β = −0.236, F = 8.47, p < 0.005) and depression (β = −0.318, F = 12.01, p < 0.001).
More student stressors were associated with greater negative well-being (β = 0.215, F = 9.93, p < 0.005), lower flourishing (β = −0.136, F = 8.84, p < 0.005), more perceived stress (β = 0.265, F = 28.30, p < 0.001), and more anxiety (β = 0.253, F = 15.94, p < 0.001) and depression (β = 0.217, F = 9.10, p < 0.005). Greater academic stress was associated with greater negative well-being (β = 0.173, F = 6.48, p < 0.05), lower flourishing (β = −0.094, F = 4.27, p < 0.05), greater perceived stress (β = 0.248, F = 24.80, p < 0.001) and greater life satisfaction (β = 0.097, F = 3.98, p < 0.05). Greater flow was associated with greater flourishing (β = 0.241, F = 34.49, p < 0.001) and better life satisfaction (β = 0.165, F = 14.30, p < 0.001). More positive rumination was associated with positive well-being (β = 0.144, F = 8.63, p < 0.005). Finally, greater cognitive rigidity was associated with greater negative well-being (β = 0.672, F = 7.71, p < 0.01).