Relating Domain-Specific Risk-Taking Behavior to Cognitive Functions in Older Adults
Abstract
1. Introduction
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Participants
2.2. Measures
Risk-Taking
2.3. Cognitive Tests
- i
- Processing Speed and Working Memory: The Trail Making Test is a measure of attention, processing speed, and mental flexibility where participants are asked to connect circles on a page as fast as possible without lifting their pencil. Part A, wherein participants are required to connect 25 randomly placed numbers on a page in numerical order, was used to assess processing speed. Part B, wherein examinees are asked to alternate between connecting numbers and alphabetical letters, was used to measure working memory [20]. On the Trail Making Test, score is dictated by seconds for completion, with higher scores indicating a slower/worse performance.
- ii
- Phonemic Fluency: Phonemic fluency was assessed using the ‘CFL’ version of the Controlled Oral Word Association Test, where participants are asked to recite from memory as many words that they can which begin with ‘c’, f’, or ‘l’ within a 60 s time limit [21]. Higher scores are indicative of more words being recited, so higher scores represent better performance on the CFL Task.
- iii
- Semantic Fluency: Semantic fluency was assessed using the ‘Animal Naming’ version of the category verbal fluency test. Participants are asked to name as many animals as they can think of within 60 s, so higher scores are indicative of better performance on the category verbal fluency test.
- iv
- Verbal Memory: Verbal memory was assessed with the Selective Reminding task (SRT), which includes both an immediate recall and delayed recall trial. The SRT is a cued verbal list-learning task consisting of 6 trials, with 12 words in each trial [22]. The SRT is used to detect episodic memory impairment in clinical and research settings at Columbia University where participants for the study were recruited. Higher scores are indicative of more word recall.
- v
- Associative Memory: Associative memory was assessed using the Face-Name Associative Memory Task (FNAME), which requires participants to learn names and occupations associated with faces shown to them [23,24]. The FNAME consists of a learning trial, an immediate memory trial, and a delayed recall trial and higher scores indicate better performance on the task. Immediate and delayed recall scores for the naming portion of the task were utilized for this study. The names portion of the FNAME was chosen for this study as performance on the naming portion has been linked to amyloid burden in healthy older adults, indicating it may be a cognitive measure of preclinical AD in aging populations [23]. This approach has been used before with similar populations [19].
2.4. Statistical Analyses
3. Results
4. Discussion
4.1. Financial Risk-Taking and Memory Tasks
4.2. Investment Versus Gambling: Differential Findings and Implications for the Field
4.3. Health and Safety Risk-Taking and Processing Speed
4.4. Strengths
4.5. Limitations
4.6. Future Directions
5. Conclusions
Supplementary Materials
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Samanez-Larkin, G.; Kuhnen, C.M.; Yoo, D.J.; Knutson, B. Variability in nucleus accumbens activity mediates age-related suboptimal financial risk taking. J. Neurosci. 2010, 30, 1426–1434. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bugg, J.M.; Zook, N.A.; DeLosh, E.L.; Davalos, D.B.; Davis, H.P. Age differences in fluid intelligence: Contributions of general slowing and frontal decline. Brain Cogn. 2006, 62, 9–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bruine de Bruin, W.; Parker, A.M.; Fischhoff, B. Explaining adult age differences in decision-making competence. J. Behav. Decis. Mak. 2012, 25, 352–360. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sun, W.; Matsuoka, T.; Narumoto, J. Decision-making support for people with alzheimer’s disease: A narrative review. Front. Psychol. 2021, 12, 750803. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Coco, D.L.; Nacci, P. Frontotemporal dementia presenting with pathological gambling. J. Neuropsychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 2004, 16, 117–118. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Compagne, C.; Gabriel, D.; Ferrero, L.; Magnin, E.; Tannou, T. Tools for the assessment of risk-taking behavior in older adults with mild dementia: A cross-sectional clinical study. Brain Sci. 2023, 13, 967. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Turner, C.; McClure, R. Age and gender differences in risk-taking behaviour as an explanation for high incidence of motor vehicle crashes as a driver in young males. Inj. Control. Saf. Promot. 2003, 10, 123–130. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rolison, J.J.; Hanoch, Y.; Wood, S.; Liu, P. Risk-taking differences across the adult life span: A question of age and domain. J. Gerontol. Ser. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 2014, 69, 870–880. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chou, K.; Lee, T.M.C.; Ho, A.H.Y.; Zacks, R.T. Does mood state change risk taking tendency in older adults? Psychol. Aging 2007, 22, 310–318. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dror, I.E.; Katona, M.; Mungur, K. Age differences in decision making: To take a risk or not? Gerontology 1998, 44, 67–71. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Albert, S.M.; Duffy, J. Differences in risk aversion between young and older adults. Neurosci. Neuroecon. 2012, 1, 3–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Weber, E.U.; Blais, A.-R. A domain-specific risk-taking (DOSPERT) scale for adult populations. Judgm. Decis. Mak. 2006, 1, 33–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Weber, E.U.; Blais, A.; Betz, N.E. A domain-specific risk-attitude scale: Measuring risk perceptions and risk behaviors. J. Behav. Decis. Mak. 2002, 15, 263–290. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- James, B.D.; Boyle, P.A.; Yu, L.; Han, S.D.; Bennett, D.A. Cognitive decline is associated with risk aversion and temporal discounting in older adults without dementia. PLoS ONE 2015, 10, e0121900. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Boyle, P.A.; Yu, L.; Buchman, A.S.; Bennett, D.A. Risk aversion is associated with decision making among community-based older persons. Front. Psychol. 2012, 3, 205. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Boyle, P.A.; Yu, L.; Buchman, A.S.; Laibson, D.I.; Bennett, D.A. Cognitive function is associated with risk aversion in community-based older persons. BMC Geriatr. 2011, 11, 53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dohmen, T.; Falk, A.; Huffman, D.; Sunde, U. Are risk aversion and impatience related to cognitive ability? Am. Econ. Rev. 2010, 100, 1238–1260. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Stern, Y.; Habeck, C.; Steffener, J.; Barulli, D.; Gazes, Y.; Razlighi, Q.; Shaked, D.; Salthouse, T. The reference ability neural network study: Motivation, design, and initial feasibility analyses. NeuroImage 2014, 103, 139–151. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chapman, S.; Sunderaraman, P.; Joyce, J.L.; Azar, M.; Colvin, L.E.; Barker, M.S.; McKeague, I.; Kreisl, W.C.; Cosentino, S. Optimizing subjective cognitive decline to detect early cognitive dysfunction. J. Alzheimer’s Dis. 2021, 80, 1185–1196. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Reitan, R.M.; Wolfson, D. Category test and trail making test as measures of frontal lobe functions. Clin. Neuropsychol. 1995, 9, 50–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Benton, A.L.; Hamsher, S.K.; Sivan, A.B. Controlled oral word association test (COWAT). Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 1983. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Buschke, H. Cued recall in amnesia. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 1984, 6, 433–440. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Rentz, D.M.; Amariglio, R.E.; Becker, J.A.; Frey, M.; Olson, L.E.; Frishe, K.; Carmasin, J.; Maye, J.E.; Johnson, K.A.; Sperling, R.A. Face-name associative memory performance is related to amyloid burden in normal elderly. Neuropsychologia 2011, 49, 2776–2783. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sperling, R.A.; Bates, J.F.; Cocchiarella, A.J.; Schacter, D.L.; Rosen, B.R.; Albert, M.S. Encoding novel face-name associations: A functional MRI study. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2001, 14, 129–139. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- IBM Corp. IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, version 27.0; IBM Corp.: Armonk, NY, USA, 2020.
- Mata, R.; Josef, A.K.; Hertwig, R. Propensity for risk taking across the life span and around the globe. Psychol. Sci. 2016, 27, 231–243. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Byrnes, J.P.; Miller, D.C.; Schafer, W.D. Gender differences in risk taking: A meta-analysis. Psychol. Bull. 1999, 125, 367–383. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Black, S.E.; Devereux, P.J.; Lundborg, P.; Majlesi, K. Learning to take risks? The effect of education on risk-taking in financial markets. Rev. Financ. 2018, 22, 951–975. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Anderson, L.R.; Mellor, J.M. Predicting health behaviors with an experimental measure of risk preference. J. Health Econ. 2008, 27, 1260–1274. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- König, A.N. Domain-specific risk attitudes and aging—A systematic review. Behav. Decis. Mak. 2020, 34, 359. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Weller, J.A.; Ceschi, A.; Randolph, C. Decision-making competence predicts domain-specific risk attitudes. Front. Psychol. 2015, 6, 540. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Clark, L.; Averbeck, B.; Payer, D.; Sescousse, G.; Winstanley, C.A.; Xue, G. Pathological choice: The neuroscience of gambling and gambling addiction. J. Neurosci. 2013, 33, 17617–17623. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Korniotis, G.M.; Kumar, A.; Langley, J.; Lim, S.; Loughran, T.; Michaelides, A. Does Investment Skill Decline Due to Cognitive Aging or Improve with Experience? 2007. Available online: http://www.ruf.rice.edu/~jgsfss/LoneStar/kumar.pdf (accessed on 4 September 2023).
- Finke, M.S.; Howe, J.S.; Huston, S.J. Old age and the decline in financial literacy. Manag. Sci. 2017, 63, 213–230. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Breuer, W.; Kollath, J.; Salzmann, A.J.; von Nitzsch, R. The assessment of risk behavior: A cross-cultural analysis using the dospert scale. In Behavioral Finance: Sophistication, Gender, Attention, Media, and Culture Effects on Investor Decisions; World Scientific Publishing Company: Singapore, 2013; pp. 159–180. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sunderaraman, P.; Ho, S.; Chapman, S.; Joyce, J.L.; Colvin, L.; Omollo, S.; Pleshkevich, M.; Cosentino, S. Technology use in everyday financial activities: Evidence from online and offline survey data. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 2020, 35, 385–400. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Sunderaraman, P.; Dong, Z.; Sampath, S.; Chapman, S.; Joyce, J.; Stern, Y.; Cosentino, S. Development of the online money management credit card task. Innov. Aging 2020, 4, 801. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Domain Subscale | Statement | ||
---|---|---|---|
Financial | Investing | Investing 5% of your annual income in a very speculative stock. | |
Investing 10% of your annual income in a moderate-growth mutual fund. | |||
Investing 10% of your annual income in a new business venture. | |||
Gambling | Betting a day’s income at the horse races. | ||
Betting a day’s income on the outcome of a sporting event. | |||
Betting a day’s income at a high-stake poker game. | |||
Total Subscale Score | 6–42 | ||
Health/Safety | Drinking heavily at a social function. | ||
Engaging in unprotected sex. | |||
Driving a car without wearing a seat belt. | |||
Riding a motorcycle without a helmet. | |||
Sunbathing without sunscreen. | |||
Walking home alone at night in an unsafe area of town. | |||
Total Subscale Score | 6–42 | ||
Social | Admitting that your tastes are different from those of a friend. | ||
Disagreeing with an authority figure on a major issue. | |||
Choosing a career that you truly enjoy over a more prestigious one. | |||
Speaking your mind about an unpopular issue in a meeting at work. | |||
Moving to a city far away from your extended family. | |||
Starting a new career in your mid-thirties. | |||
Total Subscale Score | 6–42 | ||
Ethical | Taking some questionable deductions on your income tax return | ||
Having an affair with a married man/woman | |||
Passing off somebody else’s work as your own. | |||
Revealing a friend’s secret to someone else. | |||
Leaving your young children alone at home while running an errand. | |||
Not returning a wallet you found that contains $200. | |||
Total Subscale Score | 6–42 | ||
Recreational | Going camping in the wilderness. | ||
Going down a ski run that is beyond your ability. | |||
Going whitewater rafting at high water in the spring. | |||
Taking a skydiving class. | |||
Bungee jumping off a tall bridge. | |||
Piloting a small plane. | |||
Total Subscale Score | 6–42 | ||
Overall Scale Score (Non-Domain Specific Risk-Taking) | 30–210 |
Variable | N (%) or Mean ± SD (Range) |
---|---|
Female | 33 (58.9%) |
Age (years) | 69.55 ± 7.35 (54–90) |
Education (years) | 16.69 ± 2.19 (12–20) |
Race | |
White | 45 (80.4%) |
Black | 6 (10.7%) |
Asian | 3 (5.4%) |
Chose not to identify | 2 (3.6%) |
Ethnicity | |
Non-Hispanic | 54 (96.4%) |
DOSPERT | |
General risk-taking | 84.05 ± 17.28 (50–119) |
Financial risk-taking | 12.93 ± 4.67 (6–24) |
Investing risk-taking | 9.27 ± 3.99 (3–18) |
Gambling risk-taking | 3.66 ± 1.64 (3–11) |
Health and Safety risk-taking | 13.63 ± 5.04 (6–23) |
Social risk-taking | 31.71 ± 5.98 (19–42) |
Ethical risk-taking | 11.12 ± 4.24 (6–21) |
Recreational risk-taking | 14.68 ± 8.11 (6–33) |
Cognitive Tests | |
Trails A * | 29.71 ± 9.28 (11–61) |
Trails B * | 68.66 ± 25.05 (32–172) |
Animal Naming | 24.02 ± 4.93 (15–37) |
CFL Verbal Fluency | 51.00 ± 11.44 (25–77) |
SRT Immediate Recall | 51.57 ± 7.50 (34–68) |
SRT Delayed Recall | 8.46 ± 2.18 (4–12) |
FaceName Immediate Recall | 5.73 ± 3.79 (0–16) |
FaceName Delayed Recall | 5.68 ± 3.69 (0–15) |
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2025 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Waltrip, L.H.; Chapman, S.; Bouchard-Liporto, M.; Joyce, J.L.; Kann, M.R.; Cosentino, S.; Sunderaraman, P. Relating Domain-Specific Risk-Taking Behavior to Cognitive Functions in Older Adults. Brain Sci. 2025, 15, 1044. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci15101044
Waltrip LH, Chapman S, Bouchard-Liporto M, Joyce JL, Kann MR, Cosentino S, Sunderaraman P. Relating Domain-Specific Risk-Taking Behavior to Cognitive Functions in Older Adults. Brain Sciences. 2025; 15(10):1044. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci15101044
Chicago/Turabian StyleWaltrip, Leah H., Silvia Chapman, Madison Bouchard-Liporto, Jillian L. Joyce, Michael Ryan Kann, Stephanie Cosentino, and Preeti Sunderaraman. 2025. "Relating Domain-Specific Risk-Taking Behavior to Cognitive Functions in Older Adults" Brain Sciences 15, no. 10: 1044. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci15101044
APA StyleWaltrip, L. H., Chapman, S., Bouchard-Liporto, M., Joyce, J. L., Kann, M. R., Cosentino, S., & Sunderaraman, P. (2025). Relating Domain-Specific Risk-Taking Behavior to Cognitive Functions in Older Adults. Brain Sciences, 15(10), 1044. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci15101044