Effects of Mood on Psychophysiological Detection of Concealed Information and the Relation to Self-Assessed Lying Ability
Abstract
:1. Introduction
1.1. Impact of Mood
1.2. Self-Assessed Lying Ability
1.3. The Current Study
2. Methods and Materials
2.1. Participants and Statistical Power
2.2. Apparatus
2.3. Procedure
2.3.1. First Phase of the Experiment
2.3.2. The Second Phase of the Experiment
3. Results
3.1. Manipulation Checks
3.1.1. Mood
3.1.2. Guilt
3.1.3. Excitement
3.1.4. Success Assessments
3.1.5. A Memory of the Critical Items
3.2. Physiological Response Scoring and Analysis
3.2.1. Skin Conductance Responses
3.2.2. Finger Pulse Waveform Length
3.2.3. Respiration Line Length
3.3. Association between the Physiological Responses
3.4. Comparing the Detection Efficiency of the Physiological Responses
3.5. Hypotheses Testing
3.5.1. Mood and Physiological Detection in the CIT
3.5.2. Signal Detection Model—ROC
3.5.3. Self-Assessed Lie-Telling Abilities
3.5.4. High Lie-Telling Ability Scorers and Mood Effects in the CIT
4. Discussion
Limitations and Future Research
5. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Lykken, D.T. A Tremor in the Blood. In Uses and Abuses of the Lie Detector, 2nd ed.; Plenum Trade: New York, NY, USA, 1998. [Google Scholar]
- Ben-Shakhar, G.; Bar-Hillel, M.; Kremnitzer, M. Trial by polygraph Reconsidering the use of the GKT in court. Law Hum. Behav. 2002, 26, 527–541. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
- Lykken, D.T. Psychology and the lie detection industry. Am. Psychol. 1974, 29, 225–239. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Verschuere, B.; Ben-Shakhar, G. Theory of the Concealed Information Test. In Memory Detection: Theory and Application of the Concealed Information Test; Verschuere, B., Ben-Shakhar, G., Meijer, E., Eds.; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2011; pp. 128–148. [Google Scholar]
- Elaad, E. Effects of Goal-and Task-Oriented Motivation in the Guilty Action Test. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 2013, 88, 82–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Elaad, E.; Zvi, L. Frequent countermeasure usage by narcissistic examinees in the Concealed Information Test. Front. Psychol. Forensic Leg. Psychol. 2019, 10, 1068. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ben-Shakhar, G.; Elaad, E. The validity of psychophysiological detection of information with the Guilty Knowledge Test: A meta-analytic review. J. Appl. Psychol. 2003, 88, 131–151. [Google Scholar]
- Meijer, E.H.; Klein Selle, N.; Elber, L.; Ben-Shakhar, G. Memory detection with the Concealed Information Test: A meta-analysis of skin conductance, respiration, heart rate, and P300 data. Psychophysiology 2014, 51, 879–904. [Google Scholar]
- Verschuere, B.; Crombez, G.; Koster, E.; Van Bockstaele, B.; de Clercq, A. Startling secrets: Startle eyeblink modulation by concealed crime information. Biol. Psychol. 2007, 76, 52–60. [Google Scholar]
- Bower, G.H. Mood and Memory. Am. Psychol. 1981, 36, 129–148. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Forgas, J.P.; Laham, S.M.; Vargas, P.T. Mood effects on eyewitness memory: Affective influences on susceptibility to misinformation. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 2005, 41, 574–588. [Google Scholar]
- Fiedler, K. Affective influences on social information processing. In The Handbook of Affect and Social Cognition; Forgas, J.P., Ed.; Erlbaum: Mahwah, NJ, USA, 2001; pp. 163–185. [Google Scholar]
- Schwarz, N.; Bless, H. Happy and mindless, but sad and smart? The impact on affective states on analytic reasoning. In Emotion and Social Judgments; Forgas, J.P., Ed.; Pergamon Press: Oxford, UK, 1991; pp. 55–71. [Google Scholar]
- Clark, M.S.; Isen, A.M. Towards understanding the relationship between feeling states and social behavior. In Cognitive Social Psychology; Hastorf, A.H., Isen, A.M., Eds.; Elsevier-North Holland: New York, NY, USA, 1982; pp. 73–108. [Google Scholar]
- Forgas, J.P.; East, R. How real is that smile? Mood effects on accepting or rejecting the veracity of emotional facial expressions. J. Nonverbal Behav. 2008, 32, 157–170. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Forgas, J.P.; East, R. On being happy and gullible: Mood effects on skepticism and the detection of deception. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 2008, 44, 1362–1367. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Elaad, E. Effects of feedback on the overestimated capacity to detect lies and the underestimated ability to tell lies. Appl. Cogn. Psychol. 2003, 17, 349–363. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Elaad, E. Personality, demographic, and psychophysiological correlates of people’s self-assessed lying abilities. In Detecting Concealed Information and Deception: Verbal, Behavioral, and Biological Methods; Rosenfeld, J.P., Ed.; Academic Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2018; pp. 353–376. [Google Scholar]
- Elaad, E. Self-Assessed Lie and Truth Telling Abilities: Demographic Personality, and Behavioral Correlates. In The Palgrave Handbook of Deceptive Communication; Docan-Morgan, T., Ed.; Palgrave Macmillan: London, UK, 2019; pp. 445–462. [Google Scholar]
- Elaad, E. Lie-detection biases among male police interrogators, prisoners and lay-persons. Psychol. Rep. 2009, 105, 1047–1056. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
- Elaad, E.; Sommerfeld, E. Effects of guilt, disbelief, and assessed lie-truth telling abilities on physiological responses in the Guilty Action Test. Psychology 2016, 7, 1075–1091. [Google Scholar]
- Bradley, M.T.; Rettinger, J. Awareness of crime relevant information and the guilty knowledge test. J. Appl. Psychol. 1992, 77, 55–59. [Google Scholar]
- Bradley, M.T.; Warfield, J.F. Innocence, information, and the guilty knowledge test in the detection of deception. Psychophysiology 1984, 21, 683–689. [Google Scholar]
- Zvi, L.; Elaad, E. Contributions of personality dimensions to spontaneous and deliberate information processing in the guilty actions test. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 2016, 110, 18–26. [Google Scholar]
- Zvi, L.; Nachson, I.; Elaad, E. Effects of coping and cooperating instructions on guilty and informed innocents’ physiological responses to concealed information. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 2012, 84, 140–148. [Google Scholar]
- Forgas, J.P. Mood and Judgment: The affect infusion model (AIM). Psychol. Bull. 1995, 116, 39–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Elaad, E.; Ben-Shakhar, G. Finger Pulse Waveform Length in the Detection of Concealed Information. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 2006, 61, 226–234. [Google Scholar]
- Elaad, E.; Ginton, A.; Jungman, N. Detection measures in real-life criminal guilty knowledge tests. J. Appl. Psychol. 1992, 77, 757–767. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ben-Shakhar, G. A further study of the dichotomization theory in detection of information. Psychophysiology 1977, 14, 408–413. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
- Elaad, E. Effects of perceived reliability and generalization of crime-related information on detection in the concealed information test. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 2010, 75, 295–303. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Elaad, E.; Ben-Shakhar, G. Effects of motivation and verbal response type on psychophysiological detection of information. Psychophysiology 1989, 26, 442–451. [Google Scholar]
- Elaad, E.; Ben-Shakhar, G. Effects of item repetitions and variations on the efficiency of the guilty knowledge test. Psychophysiology 1997, 34, 587–596. [Google Scholar]
- Vossel, G.; Gamer, M.; Godert, H.W.; Rill, H.G. The efficiency of detecting concealed information with the guilty knowledge test: A comparison of different physiological variables and non-physiological methods of credibility assessment using signal detection theory. J. Psychophysiol. 2003, 17, 184. [Google Scholar]
- National Research Council Report. The Polygraph and lie-detection. In Committee to Review the Scientific Evidence on the Polygraph, Division of Behavioral and Social Sciences and Education; The National Academies Press: Washington, DC, USA, 2003. [Google Scholar]
- Meijer, E.H.; Smulders, J.E.; Johnston, E.; Merckelbach, H.L.G.J. Combining skin conductance and forced choice in the detection of concealed information. Psychophysiology 2007, 44, 814–822. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bamber, D. The area above the ordinal dominance graph and the area below the receiver operating characteristic graph. J. Math. Psychol. 1975, 12, 387–415. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- klein-Selle, N.; Verschuere, B.; Kindt, M.; Meijer, E.; Ben-Shakhar, G. Orienting versus Inhibition in the Concealed Information Test: Different cognitive processes drive different physiological measures. Psychophysiology 2016, 53, 579–590. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- klein-Selle, N.; Verschuere, B.; Kindt, M.; Meijer, E.; Ben-Shakhar, G. Unraveling the roles of orienting and inhibition in the Concealed Information Test. Psychophysiology 2017, 54, 628–639. [Google Scholar]
- Elaad, E. The distrusted truth: Examination of challenged perceptions and expectations. Psychology 2015, 6, 560–571. [Google Scholar]
- Elaad, E. Personality, Religiosity, and Gender Correlates of Self-Assessed Lie and Truth Related Abilities. Adv. Appl. Psychol. 2018, 3, 1–9. [Google Scholar]
- Verigin, B.L.; Meijer, E.H.; Bogaard, G.; Vrij, A. Lie prevalence, lie characteristics and strategies of self-reported happy liars. PLoS ONE 2019, 14, e0225566. [Google Scholar]
- Davidson, R.J. Affective style: Causes and consequences. In Essays in Social Neuroscience; Cacioppo, J.T., Berntson, G.G., Eds.; MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2004; pp. 77–91. [Google Scholar]
Feelings | Sad Mood | Neutral Mood | Happy Mood |
---|---|---|---|
Happy/Sad before | 4.50 (2.04) | 6.00 (1.48) | 6.28 (1.71) |
Happy/Sad after | 6.00 (1.00) | 6.12 (1.29) | 6.03 (1.76) |
Good/Bad before | 6.00 (1.60) | 6.21 (1.97) | 7.17 (1.73) |
Good/Bad after | 7.14 (1.59) | 6.85 (1.52) | 7.17 (1.20) |
N | 22 | 33 | 29 |
Block | SCR | FPWL | RLL |
---|---|---|---|
Block 1 | 0.44 (0.54) | 0.41 (0.42) | 0.28 (0.42) |
Block 2 | 0.41 (0.61) | 0.42 (0.41) | 0.25 (0.46) |
Across | 0.42 (0.49) | 0.41 (0.34) | 0.27 (0.36) |
Measure | Sad Mood | Neutral Mood | Happy Mood | Across |
---|---|---|---|---|
SCR | 0.39 (0.53) | 0.58 (0.54) | 0.34 (0.54) | 0.44 (0.54) |
FPWL | 0.46 (0.43) | 0.31 (0.43) | 0.47 (0.39) | 0.41 (0.42) |
RLL | 0.37 (0.45) | 0.28 (0.43) | 0.20 (0.36) | 0.28 (0.42) |
N | 36 | 36 | 36 | 108 |
Measure | Area | SE | 95% CI |
---|---|---|---|
SCR | 0.75 * | 0.039 | 0.67–0.82 |
FPWL | 0.82 * | 0.033 | 0.76–0.89 |
RLL | 0.75 * | 0.038 | 0.68–0.83 |
Measure | Sad Mood | Neutral Mood | Happy Mood |
---|---|---|---|
SCR | 0.52 (0.49) | 0.47 (0.41) | 0.35 (0.37) |
FPWL | 0.64 (0.33) | 0.22 (0.48) | 0.45 (0.39) |
RLL | 0.37 (0.40) | 0.19 (0.51) | 0.31 (0.39) |
N | 12 | 15 | 15 |
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2023 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Elaad, E.; Zvi, L. Effects of Mood on Psychophysiological Detection of Concealed Information and the Relation to Self-Assessed Lying Ability. Brain Sci. 2023, 13, 291. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci13020291
Elaad E, Zvi L. Effects of Mood on Psychophysiological Detection of Concealed Information and the Relation to Self-Assessed Lying Ability. Brain Sciences. 2023; 13(2):291. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci13020291
Chicago/Turabian StyleElaad, Eitan, and Liza Zvi. 2023. "Effects of Mood on Psychophysiological Detection of Concealed Information and the Relation to Self-Assessed Lying Ability" Brain Sciences 13, no. 2: 291. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci13020291
APA StyleElaad, E., & Zvi, L. (2023). Effects of Mood on Psychophysiological Detection of Concealed Information and the Relation to Self-Assessed Lying Ability. Brain Sciences, 13(2), 291. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci13020291