Next Article in Journal
How Functional Connectivity Measures Affect the Outcomes of Global Neuronal Network Characteristics in Patients with Schizophrenia Compared to Healthy Controls
Previous Article in Journal
PsychArray-Based Genome Wide Association Study of Suicidal Deaths in India
Previous Article in Special Issue
MRI-Based Personalized Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation to Enhance the Upper Limb Function in Patients with Stroke: Study Protocol for a Double-Blind Randomized Controlled Trial
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Optimizing the Effect of tDCS on Motor Sequence Learning in the Elderly

1
Department of Psychology and Neurosciences, Leibniz Research Center for Working Environment and Human Factors, 44139 Dortmund, Germany
2
International Graduate School of Neuroscience, Ruhr-University Bochum, 44780 Bochum, Germany
3
Neuroplasticity and Movement Control Research Group, REVAL Rehabilitation Research Center, REVAL, Faculty of Rehabilitation Sciences, Hasselt University, 3590 Diepenbeek, Belgium
4
Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, Ruhr-University Bochum, 44801 Bochum, Germany
5
University Clinic of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy and University Clinic of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Protestant Hospital of Bethel Foundation, University Hospital OWL, Bielefeld University, 33617 Bielefeld, Germany
6
Movement Control and Neuroplasticity Research Group, Department of Movement Sciences, Group Biomedical Sciences, KU Leuven, 3001 Leuven, Belgium
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Brain Sci. 2023, 13(1), 137; https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci13010137
Submission received: 25 November 2022 / Revised: 5 January 2023 / Accepted: 6 January 2023 / Published: 12 January 2023

Abstract

:
One of the most visible effects of aging, even in healthy, normal aging, is a decline in motor performance. The range of strategies applicable to counteract this deterioration has increased. Transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS), a non-invasive brain stimulation technique that can promote neuroplasticity, has recently gained attention. However, knowledge about optimized tDCS parameters in the elderly is limited. Therefore, in this study, we investigated the effect of different anodal tDCS intensities on motor sequence learning in the elderly. Over the course of four sessions, 25 healthy older adults (over 65 years old) completed the Serial Reaction Time Task (SRTT) while receiving 1, 2, or 3 mA of anodal or sham stimulation over the primary motor cortex (M1). Additionally, 24 h after stimulation, motor memory consolidation was assessed. The results confirmed that motor sequence learning in all tDCS conditions was maintained the following day. While increased anodal stimulation intensity over M1 showed longer lasting excitability enhancement in the elderly in a prior study, the combination of higher intensity stimulation with an implicit motor learning task showed no significant effect. Future research should focus on the reason behind this lack of effect and probe alternative stimulation protocols.

1. Introduction

Non-invasive brain stimulation techniques (NIBS) have been developed in the last decades as tools to monitor and modulate neuronal activity and excitability in the human brain non-invasively. Transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) and transcranial electrical stimulation (tES) are the main techniques in the field [1]. Transcranial magnetic stimulation applies short-lasting magnetic pulses over the head, which induce electrical fields in the brain sufficiently strong at the cortical level to induce neuronal action potentials. Also, it is suited to monitor cortical excitability if applied over regions responsive to this stimulation, such as the primary motor cortex (M1) via motor evoked potentials, or other areas via cortical evoked potentials [2]. Beyond monitoring cortical excitability, specific TMS protocols, namely repetitive TMS (rTMS), which applies trains of pulses—usually a few hundred pulses in frequencies of about 1–50 Hz—are well-suited to induce cortical excitability alterations, whose direction depend on the specific protocol and which share features of long-term potentiation (LTP) and depression (LTD) [3,4]. A specific version of TMS, paired associative stimulation, which in its classical form combines peripheral nerve electrical stimulation with cortical TMS in specific intervals, induces a kind of plasticity related to spike timing-dependent or Hebbian-like plasticity [3]. A qualitatively different stimulation protocol to modify cortical excitability is transcranial electrical stimulation (tES). This stimulation protocol induces cortical excitability alterations via subthreshold neuronal membrane polarization by a small electrical current delivered to the brain through the scalp [5,6,7]. The most relevant version of tES in connection with the present study is transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) [8]. tDCS modulates cortical excitability by applying a weak direct current to the brain [6] and has been extensively applied in neuroscientific and clinical research [9]. The primary mechanism of tDCS is a subthreshold modulation of the neuronal resting membrane potential towards depolarization, or hyperpolarization, through a constant weak current delivered via two or more electrodes placed on the scalp [6,9,10]. With standard stimulation protocols, anodal tDCS, which refers to a surface inward current over the target area, enhances cortical excitability. In contrast, cathodal tDCS, which refers to an outward current over the target area, reduces excitability [11,12,13]. Pharmacological studies have shown that ion channels are involved in the acute polarization effects of tDCS, which evolve immediately after the start of stimulation [11,14]. Longer lasting stimulation induces neuroplastic after-effects, which depend on the modulation of GABA and glutamatergic receptors [14,15,16]. These after-effects share features of LTP (anodal tDCS) and LTD (cathodal tDCS). Specifically, anodal tDCS reduces GABA concentration and enhances glutamatergic NMDA receptor activity, while cathodal tDCS reduces both GABA and glutamate concentrations [17,18]. For LTP to occur, glutamatergic synapses are strengthened, whereas for LTD induction, glutamatergic activity is reduced. GABA has a gating function for glutamatergic plasticity.
Neuroplasticity refers to alterations in the strength of synaptic connections as a result of environmental demands, or perturbations, such as in case of NIBS, and constitutes the foundation of cognitive processes such as learning and memory formation [19,20,21]. Numerous studies have shown that the application of NIBS improves various cognitive processes, such as learning, memory formation, and executive functions, due to synergistic–task- and stimulation-dependent changes of excitability and plasticity (Ref. review by Levasseur-Moreau, 2013 [22]). In this line, brain stimulation techniques have revealed promising results with respect to cognitive and motor rehabilitation after stroke [23,24], other neurological, and also psychiatric diseases, such as depression and schizophrenia [25,26]. Moreover, performance-reducing effects of rTMS and tDCS were also demonstrated when stimulation-induced excitability alterations antagonized respective task-dependent alterations. This was shown for reconsolidation of fear memory via low-frequency, excitability-reducing rTMS. Here, rTMS reduced memory reconsolidation. The effect was state-and timing-dependent, since it emerged only with stimulation after fear memory reactivation and within the time window relevant for reconsolidation [27]. Similarly, excitability-diminishing cathodal tDCS reduced cue-guided outcome-specific transfer in a Pavlovian to instrumental transfer task [28]. Synergistic and antagonistic stimulation in relation to task-dependent physiology has thus a specific impact on performance.
In advanced age, LTP-like neuroplasticity is reduced, which results in the deterioration of cognitive and motor abilities in the elderly [29,30,31]. Although the prevalence of physical diseases rises with age, a notable slowing of movement and reactions is also observed in otherwise healthy senior adults [32]. Older adults experience the deterioration of fine motor skills [33], a decline in reaction time task performance [34,35], and difficulties in motor coordination [36,37,38]. Moreover, a reduction in the capacity to learn new motor skills has been reported [39,40]. Such decline of motor functions and slowing of movement is caused by a multitude of age-dependent alterations, including physiological changes, structural atrophy, and neurotransmitter imbalances [41,42,43,44,45,46]. As age-related motor decline significantly impacts independent daily activities, rehabilitation techniques including non-invasive brain stimulation have been developed to modulate neuroplasticity and improve motor function in the elderly [47]. They have shown promising results with respect to cognitive and motor improvement in healthy elderly individuals and in aging-related neurological disorders [31,48,49].
Motor learning is a crucial skill in everyday life. Two main forms of motor learning can be discerned, explicit and implicit, and can be induced and modulated in experimental settings. Explicit learning refers to conscious recollection of previous experiences, while implicit learning is an unintentional, nonconscious variant of learning characterised by behavioural improvement [50]. Motor learning involves the contribution of an extended cortical-subcortical network, including the primary motor and premotor cortex, supplementary motor area, posterior parietal cortex, prefrontal areas, the striatum, and the cerebellum, with predominant contribution of specific areas, dependent on the type of task and stage of learning [50,51]. For motor sequence learning, which is the topic of this study, at the cortical level especially, M1 is relevant in the early stage of learning, while premotor and parietal areas become more important during later stages of learning, and memory consolidation [50,52,53]. In addition, at subcortical levels, the striatum and cerebellum have been proposed to be involved in motor sequence learning [50]. Motor memory acquisition requires LTP and, thus, the strengthening of learning-related synapses. Comparable to LTP induction, GABA activity decreases in the motor cortex, while glutamate is enhanced during motor learning [54,55,56]. In contrast, motor learning deteriorates when glutamatergic activity is disrupted and/or synaptic GABA activity is increased [57,58]. Thus, motor learning deficits in people of advanced age can be at least partially explained by alterations of neurotransmitters in normal aging and reduced LTP [45,59,60,61,62]. Indeed, GABA and glutamate decline and regulation impairment in the aging process have been linked to motor dysfunctions [63,64,65].
Thus, for ameliorating motor functions, and especially motor learning, induction of LTP-like plasticity could be a valuable approach. Here, tDCS might be relevant. Application of anodal tDCS over M1 has indeed been shown to enhance performance in motor sequence learning tasks [66,67,68], as well as consolidation in young adults [68,69,70,71], with effects lasting for months after intervention in some studies [72,73]. However, some heterogeneity of results was also noted [52,74,75], which might be at least partially caused by intervention protocol differences. One of these is the timing-dependency of tDCS effects on motor learning. It was thus shown that the intervention should be conducted during the actual motor learning process to be effective, in order to make use of synergistic stimulation- and learning-dependent plasticity induction [58].
Likewise, tDCS studies to improve motor learning in the elderly revealed partially heterogeneous results. In some studies, tDCS in combination with sequence motor learning enhanced learning and improved consolidation in the elderly [76,77,78,79], as well as in patients with motor deficits and impaired movement abilities [80,81,82,83,84]. However, other studies show no enhancement of motor learning [47] or motor improvement by tDCS after stroke [83,85]. One reason for heterogeneous results beyond methodological causes, including the use of different intervention protocols, outcome measures, and target groups in respective studies, might be inter-individually heterogeneous brain structures and connectivity, as well as neuromodulator and neurotransmitter alterations in advanced age. Loss of grey matter volume with aging [86] causes a reduction of cognitive and motor performance due to the disintegration of cerebral networks [86]. These structural alterations also result in larger variability of the distribution of stimulation-induced electric fields [87], which might lead to heterogeneous tDCS effects. Studies in elderly populations have moreover revealed that induction of LTP-like plasticity by tDCS declines with advancing age, potentially requiring a higher intensity or longer duration of stimulation [88,89,90,91,92]. Thus, it might be speculated that enhancing the tDCS dosage strengthens the impact of this intervention on motor learning in the elderly. However, few studies have aimed to develop optimally suited tDCS protocols to prevent motor function deterioration in the elderly population.
Prior studies have shown that enhanced anodal tDCS intensity and duration (i.e., 3 mA-20 min, 3 mA-30 min) can induce larger and longer lasting LTP-like plasticity in the elderly [93]. Therefore, in this study, we systematically titrated the intensity of anodal tDCS over M1 during a motor sequence learning task in elderly participants, aiming for an optimized stimulation protocol for functional improvement. In a double-blind, sham-controlled, cross-over design, we applied three stimulation intensities (1, 2, and 3 mA) in combination with an implicit motor sequence learning task. We hypothesized that higher intensities of tDCS will result in greater improvement of motor learning. Establishing an effective stimulation protocol would potentially allow for motor function improvements in the healthy elderly population or patients with motor skill disorders.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants

A Power analysis (G*Power 3.1.9.4, Franz Faul, Universität Kiel, Germany), for estimating the required sample size, conducted based on a medium critical effect size (0.25) and critical alpha and beta-errors of 0.05, resulted in a sample size of 23 participants using an ANOVA as the primary statistical test. In order to account for dropouts, we added 2 more participants. Therefore, twenty-five healthy, right-handed, non-smoking elderly volunteers above 65 years (14 females; 11 males; mean age 72.13, SD ± 5.91) were recruited. The experiment was conducted by the same experimenter in Dortmund (Germany) and later in Hasselt (Belgium) using the identical experimental protocol. A physician examined all volunteers for exclusion criteria for non-invasive electrical brain stimulation [94,95]. Handedness was examined with the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory [96], and cognitive state was evaluated with the Montreal Cognitive Assessment test (MoCA) [97]. Volunteers participating in the study signed an informed consent form and received financial compensation. This study was approved by the ethics committee of Leibniz Research Centre for Working Environment and Human Factors (IfADo) and by the Committee for Medical Ethics of Hasselt University and is in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki [98].

2.2. Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation (tDCS)

Prior to stimulation, a topical anaesthetic cream (EMLA®, 2.5% lidocaine, 2.5% prilocaine (Dortmund, Aspen Pharma trading Limited, Dublin, Irland) or lidocaine HCL 2.5% buffered cetomacrogol cream (Hasselt, Aa.Pharma Oud-Turnhout, Hasselt, Belgium) was applied over the stimulation areas to ensure sufficient blinding of the participants [99]. In randomized order, all participants received 3 sessions of anodal stimulation at 1, 2, or 3 mA intensity, and one sham stimulation session. tDCS was delivered by a battery-powered constant current stimulator (neuroCare, Ilmenau, Germany) through a pair of carbonated rubber electrodes embedded in saline-soaked sponges (35 cm2). The anode was fixed over M1 (C3, according to the international 10–20 EEG system), and the cathode was placed contralaterally over the right supraorbital region. The duration of stimulation was adjusted to the duration of motor task performance and controlled via an external trigger (flip-flop mode) with a 30 s ramp-up at the start of the stimulation, and a 30 s ramp-down at the end of the stimulation. For sham stimulation, 1 mA was applied for 30 s (ramp-up and down duration was the same as for other stimulation protocols), followed by 0 mA until task completion. The interval between sessions was at least one week to avoid carry-over effects of the stimulation.

2.3. Serial Reaction Time Task (SRTT) as a Measure of Implicit Motor Sequence Learning

A custom-made response box with four response keys anatomically aligned to the right hand was used to record performance. The four fingers involved in performance were the index finger for the first button, the middle finger for the second button, the ring finger for the third button, and the little finger for the fourth button. Four horizontal lines representing the keys were presented on the computer screen. Participants were instructed to press the corresponding key with the correct finger as fast and accurately as possible when a stimulus (white dot) appeared above the corresponding line. The next trial appeared 500 ms following a button press, independent of a correct or incorrect response.
The test was performed on two consecutive days. On the first day, the task was accompanied by tDCS (main-SRTT [M-SRTT]), consisting of 8 blocks, each with 120 trials. Blocks 1 and 6 were random blocks, in which the trials were presented in a pseudo-random order. Blocks 2–5 and 7–8 displayed a 12-trial sequence of stimuli repeated 10 times in each block, which prompted implicit learning [100]. Blocks were separated by self-paced breaks and participants were not informed about the sequences. On the second day, a short form of the SRTT was performed (recall-SRTT [R-SRTT]), consisting of three blocks: one random block followed by two sequence blocks. In order to control for use-dependent sequence learning over sessions, four different sequences were designed for the main SRTT. Thus, participants performed a different sequence in each session. The sequence at the recall day was the same as the main SRTT session performed on the day before. Following the last session, participants were asked if they noticed any repeating/pattern of the stimuli.

2.4. Experimental Procedure

The study was performed in a cross-over, double-blind, randomized design. Both participants and experimenter were blinded with respect to the tDCS condition. Every session started with the identification of the C3 position (according to the 10–20 International EEG system). The anaesthetic cream was then applied to the scalp where the tDCS electrodes would be placed. tDCS and M-SRTT started 20 min later, allowing the anaesthetic effect of the cream to set in. Participants were seated in front of a computer screen at eye level, with the table and chair adjusted to each individual. The performing hand of each participant was covered by a box designed specifically for the experiment to avoid any distractions (e.g., by visual checking the button to be pressed). Before starting the main part of the experiment, participants were asked to perform a 20-trial random block of SRTT to familiarize themselves with the task and to determine the best position for their hands. The tDCS and M-SRTT were synchronized so that the first trial appeared after the 30 s ramp-up of tDCS. The stimulation stopped after the last trial, which prompted tDCS to ramp-down. Next, the electrodes were removed, and participants were asked to fill in a questionnaire that evaluated blinding and side effects of tDCS during and after stimulation [101,102]. The questionnaire contained (1) participants’ guess of the applied tDCS intensity (0, 1, 2, and 3 mA) and (2) rating scales for the presence and perception of visual phenomena, itching, tingling, burning, and pain during stimulation, and (3) rating scales for the presence and amount of skin redness, headache, fatigue, concentration difficulties, nervousness, and sleep problems within 24 h following stimulation. The third question was asked the following day. The side effects were rated on a numerical scale from zero (‘none’) to five (‘extremely strong’). The recall session was performed 24 ± 2 h after the main session (Figure 1., Course of the experiment).

2.5. Data Analysis and Statistics

The individual mean reaction time (RT) and standard deviation (SD) of correct responses was calculated for each block of the experimental condition and for each participant, separately. RTs above or below 3 standard deviations (SD) were discarded in each block. Mean RTs were normalised to block 1 for each participant, in each stimulation condition, separately.

2.5.1. Effect of tDCS on M-SRTT

To rule out the possibility of baseline RT differences between the tDCS conditions, the respective absolute RTs of block 1 were compared through a one-way ANOVA. Separate repeated measures ANOVAs were conducted for reaction time and accuracy, with the independent variables ’Condition’ (4 levels) and ‘Block’ (8 levels) and dependent variables, including absolute and normalized RTs, number of errors, and variability (SDs of absolute RTs).

2.5.2. Effect of tDCS on R-SRTT

To explore the effect of tDCS on recall SRTT, a repeated measures ANOVA was calculated with the independent variables ‘Condition’ (4 levels) and ‘Block’ (3 levels) and dependent variables, including both absolute and normalized RTs, number of errors, and variability of the recall session.

2.5.3. Assessment of tDCS Side Effects and Blinding

A Chi-square test was conducted to identify whether participants correctly guessed tDCS intensities. The presence of side effects during and after tDCS was analysed separately using a repeated measures ANOVA with ‘Condition’ (4 levels) as the within-subject factor and rating scores (0–5) as the dependent variable.
For the ANOVAs, Mauchly’s test of sphericity was conducted, and the Greenhouse–Geisser correction was applied when necessary. The critical significance level was set at p ≤ 0.05. In the case of significant ANOVA results, exploratory post-hoc Fisher’s Least Significant Difference (LSD) tests were conducted. Statistical analyses were performed with SPSS (IBM Corp. Version 27.0, Armonk, New York, USA).

3. Results

All participants tolerated the stimulation well. One participant was excluded from all data analyses due to poor performance in one session with an error rate of 50%.
The results of the MoCA test were in the normal range for all participants (mean ± SD: 26.21 ± 2.37). The duration of tDCS was adjusted to the length of the task, which was on average 21.79 ± 1.17 min (mean ± SD). There was no significant difference of stimulation duration between the conditions (F(2.279, 52.409) = 0.498, p = 0.635, η p 2 = 0.021 ). The average of discarded trials for the M-SRTT was 5.33% ± 3.81 (mean ± SD) and, for the R-SRTT, it was 6.78% ± 11.19. Regarding sequence awareness, only 3 of the 24 participants reported that they did not notice a sequence.

3.1. Effect of tDCS on M-SRTT

The one-way repeated measures ANOVA results of M-SRTT comparing absolute RTs of block 1 for each tDCS condition showed no significant difference (F(3, 69) = 0.300, p = 0.825, η p 2 = 0.013 ). The results of the repeated measures ANOVA for absolute RT values revealed a significant main effect of ‘Block’ (F(2.521, 57.977) = 13.486, p < 0.001, η p 2 = 0.370 ) but not ‘Condition’ effect (F(3, 69) = 0.827, p = 0.484, η p 2 = 0.035 ), nor a significant ‘Condition’ × ‘Block’ interaction (F(8.790, 202.169) = 1.290, p = 0.245, η p 2 = 0.053 ) (Table 1 (A) Effect of tDCS on M-SRTT; Figure 2A Results of the M-SRTT).
Similarly, the respective ANOVA for normalized RTs showed a significant main effect of ‘Block’ (F(2.502, 57.539) = 14.996, p < 0.001, η p 2 = 0.395 ) but not ‘Condition’ (F(3, 69) = 0.572, p = 0.635, η p 2 = 0.024 ), nor a ‘Condition’ × ‘Block’ interaction (F(8.982, 206.595) = 1.192, p = 0.302, η p 2 = 0.049 ) (Table 1 (B) Effect of tDCS on M-SRTT) (Figure 2B Results of the M-SRTT).
Furthermore, the ANOVA results for ‘Errors’ showed no significant effects of the main factors ‘Block’ (F(2.639, 60.695) = 0.637, p = 0.575, η p 2 = 0.027 ) and ‘Condition’ (F(1.785, 41.044) = 0.408, p = 0.645, η p 2 = 0.017 ), nor the ‘Condition’ × ‘Block’ interaction (F(3.763, 60.695) = 1.286, p = 0.282, η p 2 = 0.053 ) (Table 1 (C) Effect of tDCS on M-SRTT) (Figure 2C Results of the M-SRTT).
The ANOVA results of M-SRTT ‘Variability’ also showed a significant main effect of ‘Block’ (F(2.742, 63.069) = 11.787, p < 0.001, η p 2 = 0.339 ) but not ‘Condition’ (F(3, 69) = 0.538, p = 0.658, η p 2 = 0.023 ), nor the ‘Condition’ × ‘Block’ interaction (F(9.399, 216.171) = 0.743, p = 0.788, η p 2 = 0.031 ) (Table 1 (D) Effect of tDCS on M-SRTT) (Figure 2D Results of the M-SRTT).
Exploratory post-hoc reaction time tests in the 1 mA stimulation condition showed no significant differences between sequence blocks relative to block 1 (all post-hoc test results, including degrees of freedom, t-values, p-values, and effect sizes, are available in the Supplemental Material, Tables S1–S12), except for blocks 2 and 5. For 2 mA, all sequence blocks differed significantly from block 1. In addition, except for blocks 5 and 7, all sequenced blocks in the 3 mA tDCS condition differed significantly from block 1. Also, with the exception of block 7, all sequence blocks in the sham condition were significantly different from block 1. Furthermore, exploratory post-hoc LSD showed a significant difference between block 6 (random trials) and blocks 5 and 7 (sequence trials), demonstrating that motor sequence learning took place in all tDCS conditions.
Moreover, exploratory post-hoc LSD for error counts showed a significant decrement of errors in the 3 mA tDCS condition for blocks 3 and 5 compared with block 1. There was also a significant increment in the number of errors in block 6 in comparison to blocks 5 and 7 in the sham condition.
Regarding variability of absolute reaction time, the exploratory post-hoc test revealed a significant increase of variability of blocks 4 and 6–8, as compared with block 1, for the 1 and 2 mA conditions. Moreover, except for block 2, variability of all blocks in the 3 mA and sham conditions were significantly increased when compared to block 1.

3.2. Effects of tDCS on R-SRTT

The ANOVA conducted on R-SRTT comparing absolute RTs of block 1 between tDCS conditions showed no significant difference (F(3, 69) = 0.276, p = 0.843, η p 2 = 0.012 ). The respective repeated measures ANOVA of absolute RTs revealed a significant main effect of ‘Block’ (F(1.554, 35.751) = 42.761, p < 0.001, η p 2 = 0.650 ) but not ‘Condition’ (F(3, 69) = 0.351, p = 0.789, η p 2 = 0.015 ) nor the ‘Condition’ × ‘Block’ interaction (F(3.773, 86.780) = 0.517, p = 0.713, η p 2 = 0.022 ) (Table 2 (A) Effect of tDCS on R-SRTT; Figure 3A Results of R-SRTT).
In addition, the ANOVA conducted for normalized RTs showed a significant main effect of ‘Block’ (F(1.397, 32.128) = 41.488, p < 0.001, η p 2 = 0.643 ) but not ‘Condition’ (F(3, 69) = 0.139, p = 0.936, η p 2 = 0.006 ), nor the ‘Condition’ × ‘Block’ interaction (F(3.634, 83.587) = 0.508, p = 0.712, η p 2 = 0.022 ) (Table 2 (B) Effect of tDCS on R-SRTT; Figure 3B Results of R-SRTT).
The ANOVA results for ‘Errors’ showed no significant main effects of ‘Block’ (F(2, 46) = 0.666, p = 0.517, η p 2 = 0.028 ) or ‘Condition’ (F(1.779, 40.912) = 1.118, p = 0.331, η p 2 = 0.046 ), nor the ‘Condition’ × ‘Block’ interaction (F(3.584, 82.437) = 0.598, p = 0.647, η p 2 = 0.025 ) (Table 2 (C) Effect of tDCS on R-SRTT; Figure 3C Results of R-SRTT).
For R-SRTT variability of absolute reaction time, the ANOVA showed a significant main effect of ‘Block’ (F(1.565, 36.001) = 6.024, p < 0.009, η p 2 = 0.208 ) but not ‘Condition’ (F(3, 69) = 0.472, p = 0.703, η p 2 = 0.020 ). There was a significant effect of the ‘Condition’ × ‘Block’ interaction (F(6, 138) = 2.643, p = 0.019, η p 2 = 0.103 ) (Table 2 (D) Effect of tDCS on R-SRTT; Figure 3D Results of R-SRTT).
Additionally, exploratory post-hoc LSD for RTs of the R-SRTT showed a significant decrement of RTs between block 1 (random trials) and block 2 and 3 (sequence trials) in all conditions.
Furthermore, the post-hoc LSD showed a significant enhancement of variability between block 1 and block 3 in all conditions, except the 2 mA stimulation. Moreover, RT variability in the 1 mA, 3 mA, and sham conditions were significantly higher in block 3, as compared with block 2. Figure 4 presents the results of both absolute M-SRTT and R-SRTT for all conditions.

3.3. Assessment of tDCS Side Effects and Blinding Efficacy

The data points of the guessed versus actually received stimulation intensities are presented in Table 3 (Data points of guessed versus received tDCS intensity). The Chi-square test revealed no significant heterogeneity (χ2 Value = 3.042, F (9), p = 0.963), meaning blinding was successful.
Participants’ ratings for the presence and intensity of side effects during and within 24 h after stimulation are listed in Table 4 (Side effect ratings). The ANOVAs showed no significant difference in the side effect ratings during or 24 h following stimulation between conditions (Table 5. Side effect ratings of the participants, ANOVA results).

4. Discussion

All participants tolerated the intervention well, and blinding was successful.
As the results of the M-SRTT demonstrate, reaction time in sequence blocks decreased over time in comparison to random blocks in all stimulation conditions. Thus, motor learning took place; however, it did not significantly differ between intervention conditions. Only under 2 mA anodal stimulation, reaction time was significantly reduced in all sequence blocks compared to the random block 1, which might be interpreted as a trend for more stable motor learning in this condition. Furthermore, reaction times in block 5 and 7 (sequence) in relation to block 6 (random) were significantly reduced in all conditions, confirming motor sequence learning effects during all interventions. Also, for the sequence learning-specific analysis, the tDCS effects of different intensities did not differ. Regarding the number of errors and reaction time variability, the stimulation conditions also did not differ in the M-SRTT.
Likewise, the R-SRTT revealed a significant reduction of reaction time in the sequence blocks but no differences between stimulation conditions. This suggests that memory consolidation also took place in all stimulation conditions. With respect to errors, no difference was observed between conditions, but RT variability showed a significant increase when comparing block 3 (sequence) to block 2 (sequence) and block 3 (sequence) to block 1 (random) under sham, 1 mA, and 3 mA conditions. Hereby, the higher variability of block 3 might be caused by the fact that, for a well-learned sequence, the RT difference between well-performed and not well performed trials is expected to be larger than in a less well-learned sequence and within a random stimulus block where sequence memory does not interfere.
These results differ from most previous studies, which have shown that implicit motor sequence learning is enhanced by anodal tDCS applied to M1 [103]. Improvement of online reaction time [66,67,68,72,104,105], consolidation and retention [68,69,70,71], and reduction of the number of errors [71] has been documented in young adults. Nevertheless, some studies combining anodal tDCS over M1 with the serial reaction time task in young volunteers have shown contrasting results. In an offline tDCS study, no significant improvement of learning was observed after anodal stimulation with 1 mA for 10 min [74], suggesting that synchronous stimulation and task performance might be relevant for the effects. Effects of tDCS on learning-related plasticity should be stronger and more specific to synchronous learning and stimulation when task-related neurons are activated by the learning process and stimulation. Moreover, in another study investigating different intensities of stimulation on explicit motor sequence learning task performance, no difference was observed for either 1 mA or 1.5 mA stimulation compared to sham in the main experimental session, but, compared to sham stimulation, a significant improvement in the 1.5 mA condition in the retention session was revealed [70]. This delayed effect might be because tDCS-induced LTP-like plasticity should strengthen task-relevant neuronal connections for prolonged periods of time. These effects might be uncovered with a delay when only learning-related plasticity already starts to vanish [58]. These intervention protocol-dependent missing effects do not apply to the present study’s results, because tDCS was applied during learning, and we re-tested SRTT performance in a consolidation session.
Studies investigating motor sequence learning tasks with concurrent tDCS in elderly groups have also shown heterogeneous results. Reaction time and error decrements in motor sequence learning tasks under tDCS in the elderly have been reported [76,78,79]. Some of these studies have even investigated the effects of multi-session anodal tDCS in the elderly, which led to extended improved motor performance over several months after intervention [65]. Others, though, have reported no effect of tDCS on motor sequence learning [47,106], which might be partially due to methodological specifics, as outlined above, but these do not fully explain the missing effects in some studies, including the present one.
Results regarding consolidation in the present study revealed that implicit motor sequence learning was retained after 24 h regardless of the stimulation protocol. In contrast, other studies showed that motor memory consolidation was enhanced by tDCS in young [69,72] and elderly adults [79]. However, it should be emphasized that the aforementioned studies did not use the same methodology (e.g., stimulation duration, intensity, and timing-dependency of tDCS on motor learning) as the present study, and these effects on consolidation emerged along with improved learning under tDCS, which was not the case in the present study. This might explain the missing effects of tDCS on consolidation in the present study.
While methodological disparities might partially explain the heterogeneous effects between studies, these do not deliver a complete explanation. Therefore, we propose that age-dependent alterations of brain functions contribute and explain the missing effects of tDCS in the present study. Differences in LTP-like plasticity induction between age groups have been previously reported, which showed reduced LTP-like plasticity induction by anodal tDCS in the elderly [88,90]; however, intensified protocols of 3 mA-20 min and 3 mA-30 min of anodal tDCS, as applied in the present study, resulted in enhanced and longer-lasting LTP-like plasticity in this age group [93]. This led to the expectation that the combination of a motor sequence learning task with intensified stimulation would result in improved effects.
It might be the case that age-dependent alterations at the microscale level contributed to the missing effect of tDCS on SRTT performance in the present study. Imaging studies show that the reduction of GABA concentration in M1 plays an important role during the acquisition of a sequence of movements [58,107,108]. Moreover, decreased GABA and enhanced glutamate concentration under anodal tDCS have been shown to be necessary for LTP-like plasticity induction, that, as previously explained, shares a mechanism of action with motor learning [18,72,105,109,110]. The likely foundation of motor learning, including the acquisition and retention of skills, is use-dependent LTP-like plasticity, which is regulated by NMDA receptor activity and GABA [111]. In this connection, it is worth mentioning that dysregulation of neurotransmitters and neuromodulators in advanced age has been documented in animal and human studies. This includes a significant reduction of GABA concentration in M1 in older adults compared to those of younger age [112]. Likewise, glutamate concentration in the brain declines with age [62,113,114,115]. These GABA and glutamate alterations may potentially limit motor performance improvements by tDCS [116,117,118]. A fine-tuned excitation/inhibition (E/I) balance is required to induce the plasticity of task-relevant neurons. Reduced GABA in older age will compromise respective inhibition, which will be further be enhanced by tDCS [16], and thus, likely reduce de-activation of task-irrelevant neurons. Since the tDCS effects are furthermore not restricted to task-relevant neurons, this will likely result in a noisy LTP, which might not be efficient for improved learning. In accordance, an MRS motor sequence learning study in the elderly showed no reduction of GABA levels in the sensorimotor cortex during motor training, which, in connection to the general GABA reduction in the elderly, hints to a bottom effect [106]. It could be thus speculated that relative glutamatergic over-activation plays a role in these missing effects; therefore, applying cathodal tDCS to reduce this might cause enhanced performance in the elderly population. Such an effect has been shown to aid performance improvement in a ’noisy’ learning task in young adults; however, too high diffuse LTP, as induced by anodal tDCS, was detrimental [119]. This explanation is speculative at present and has to be substantiated in future studies.
Age-dependent alterations of brain structure and function at the macroscale level might have also contributed to the missing effects of anodal tDCS in the present study. As mentioned earlier, structural decline of the central nervous system in advanced age, such as grey and white matter atrophy, might reduce motor and cognitive performance [106,120,121] and also tDCS effects. The latter might be due to an enhanced stimulation electrode-to-brain distance [122]. With respect to functional connectivity, older adults exhibit weaker functional connectivity between brain regions within the core functional network, which compromises this network’s efficacy and performance. Since tDCS induces network effects [123], this might also reduce the impact of tDCS on motor learning. However, the elderly show a more distributed connectivity with other cerebral areas [124]. This was explained by compensatory activity, meaning that this additional activity is required to keep performance at a similar level despite reduced functionality of the core network [125]. It might thus be speculated that the primary motor cortex is already at its maximum level of functionality for the learning process without additional interventions; thus, its efficacy cannot be enhanced further by direct stimulation. Therefore, it might be speculated that compensatory areas are more promising intervention targets for performance improvement [124]. Nevertheless, this needs to be substantiated in future studies.
Taken together, enhanced stimulation intensity over the motor cortex did not lead to a relevant motor learning improvement in elderly participants in the present study. Thus, the application of tDCS for performance improvement in the elderly might be more challenging than in young adults. This might be caused by an aging-dependent LTP-like plasticity decline due to various alterations of the aging brain at micro- and macroscale levels, including neurotransmitter decline, brain atrophy, and altered network connectivity. Therefore, adapted stimulation protocols, including re-establishing compromised cortical inhibition and stimulation of different network components—such as those activated only in higher age to compensate for reduced functionality of the motor core network have to be developed and should be probed in future studies.

Limitations and Future Directions

There are several limitations that warrant further discussion. While session order was randomized in this crossover study and had no significant effect on the outcome, a counter-balanced design would have improved the validity of the results further by ruling out order effects more definitely. With consideration of the excluded participant, an additional sample of 27 participants would have had to be recruited for a balanced order. Given this present limited timeline and the pandemic, this was not feasible. A potential ceiling effect might have also affected the reduction in reaction time as participants performed multiple sessions of the task.
Finally, some studies have reported that stimulating the cerebellum induces better motor memory acquisition [71,126]. Exploring the effect of tDCS on other brain regions or combining it with imaging techniques in the elderly group could thus improve our mechanistic knowledge and might lead to advanced protocols.

5. Conclusions

This study explored the titration of anodal tDCS intensities (1, 2, and 3 mA) over M1 to optimize implicit motor learning performance in older adults. Our results show that implicit motor learning, as well as consolidation, occurred regardless of stimulation conditions. Exploration of the reasons for this null effect and respective adaptation of intervention protocols should be the subject of future studies. Identification of the optimal protocol for improving motor learning in the healthy elderly might improve performance in older, healthy adults and also enhance the efficacy of tDCS in neurorehabilitation patients. It is important to state that a one-to-one transferability of these effects to other cortical areas and patient populations should not be presumed, due to the state-dependency of tES effects, anatomical differences, and differences of neuromodulator activities and cortical excitability between healthy humans and respective patients.

Supplementary Materials

The following are available online at https://www.mdpi.com/article/10.3390/brainsci13010137/s1, Table S1: Post-hoc results of M-SRTT. Results of post-hoc pairwise comparisons of the absolute reaction time of M-SRTT under all stimulation conditions are shown. Table S2. Post-hoc results of M-SRTT. Results of post-hoc pairwise comparisons of the absolute reaction time of M-SRTT between block 6 (random block) to block 5 and 7 (sequence) under stimulation conditions are shown. Table S3. Post-hoc results of M-SRTT. Results of post-hoc pairwise comparisons of the normalised reaction time of M-SRTT under all stimulation conditions are shown. Table S4. Post-hoc results of M-SRTT. Results of post-hoc pairwise comparisons of the normalised reaction time of M-SRTT between block 6 (random block) to block 5 and 7 (sequence) under stimulation conditions are shown. Table S5. Post-hoc results of M-SRTT. Results of post-hoc pairwise comparisons of the errors of M-SRTT under all stimulation conditions are shown. Table S6. Post-hoc results of M-SRTT. Results of post-hoc pairwise comparisons of the errors of M-SRTT between block 6 (random block) to block 5 and 7 (sequence) under stimulation conditions are shown. Table S7. Post-hoc results of M-SRTT. Results of post-hoc pairwise comparisons of the variability of absolute reaction time of M-SRTT under all stimulation conditions are shown. Table S8. Post-hoc results of M-SRTT. Results of post-hoc pairwise comparisons of the variability of absolute reaction time of M-SRTT between block 6 (random block) to block 5 and 7 (sequence) under stimulation conditions are shown. Table S9. Post-hoc results of R-SRTT. Results of post-hoc pairwise comparisons of the absolute reaction time of R-SRTT under all stimulation conditions are shown. Table S10. Post-hoc results of R-SRTT. Results of post-hoc pairwise comparisons of the normalised reaction time of R-SRTT under all stimulation conditions are shown. Table S11. Post-hoc results of R-SRTT. Results of post-hoc pairwise comparisons of the variability of absolute reaction time of the R-SRTT under all stimulation conditions are shown. Table S12. Post-hoc results of R-SRTT. Results of post-hoc pairwise comparisons of the block × condition interaction for the variability of absolute reaction time of the R-SRTT are shown.

Author Contributions

E.G.-S.: Conceptualization; Data curation; Formal analysis; Investigation; Writing—original draft; Visualization. R.U.: Data curation, review and editing. L.M.: Formal analysis, review and editing. K.v.D.: Data curation. M.-F.K.: Conceptualization; Supervision, review & editing. M.A.N.: Conceptualization; Funding acquisition; Supervision; Writing—review and editing. R.L.J.M.: Conceptualization; Funding acquisition; Supervision; Writing—review and editing. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This work was supported by research grants from the German Federal Ministry of Education and Research (BMBF) and the Special Research Fund (BOF) of Hasselt University (BOF17BL03).

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was approved in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki by Faculteit Geneeskunde en Levenswetenschappen Comité voor Medische Ethiek, Hasselt University. Approval Code: B9115201939823. Approval Date: 26 March 2019 and Ethics committee in Leibniz Research Centre for Working Environment and Human Factors, Dortmund, Germany. Approval Code: 151. Approval Date: 9 October 2018, (Amendment 14 July 2020).

Informed Consent Statement

Written informed consent has been obtained from the participants to publish this paper.

Data Availability Statement

The data presented in this study are available on request from the corresponding author. The data are not publically available due to ethical restrictions.

Acknowledgments

We thank Nicole Rück and master students for their kind help in recruiting elderly participants in Germany and Belgium, respectively. We really appreciate our colleagues: Tobias Blanke, Tobias Klimek, Nina Abich, Mohsen Mosayebi-Samani in Germany and Mark Geraerts in Belgium for their technical support to build up the experimental setup. We are grateful to Klaus Golka and Jan G. Hengstler for medical screening of participants, and Hannah Schade and Benedikt Glinski for their help with psychological test assessment.

Conflicts of Interest

Declaration of competing interests: MA Nitsche is a member of the Scientific Advisory Board of Neuroelectrics. None of the remaining authors have potential conflicts of interest to be disclosed.

References

  1. Polanía, R.; Nitsche, M.A.; Ruff, C.C. Studying and modifying brain function with non-invasive brain stimulation. Nat. Neurosci. 2018, 21, 174–187. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Fitzgerald, P.B.; Brown, T.L.; Daskalakis, Z.J. The application of transcranial magnetic stimulation in psychiatry and neurosciences research. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 2002, 105, 324–340. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Huang, Y.-Z.; Edwards, M.J.; Rounis, E.; Bhatia, K.P.; Rothwell, J.C. Theta Burst Stimulation of the Human Motor Cortex. Neuron 2005, 45, 201–206. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  4. Rossini, P.M.; Rossini, L.; Ferreri, F. Brain-behavior relations: Transcranial magnetic stimulation: A review. IEEE Eng. Med. Biol. Mag. 2010, 29, 84–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Rossini, P.M.; Burke, D.; Chen, R.; Cohen, L.G.; Daskalakis, Z.; Di Iorio, R.; Di Lazzaro, V.; Ferreri, F.; Fitzgerald, P.B.; George, M.S.; et al. Non-invasive electrical and magnetic stimulation of the brain, spinal cord, roots and peripheral nerves: Basic principles and procedures for routine clinical and research application. An updated report from an I.F.C.N. Committee. Clin. Neurophysiol. Off. J. Int. Fed. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2015, 126, 1071–1107. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Nitsche, M.A.; Cohen, L.G.; Wassermann, E.M.; Priori, A.; Lang, N.; Antal, A.; Paulus, W.; Hummel, F.; Boggio, P.S.; Fregni, F. Transcranial direct current stimulation: State of the art 2008. Brain Stimul. 2008, 1, 206–223. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Dissanayaka, T.; Zoghi, M.; Farrell, M.; Egan, G.F.; Jaberzadeh, S. Does transcranial electrical stimulation enhance corticospinal excitability of the motor cortex in healthy individuals? A systematic review and meta-analysis. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2017, 46, 1968–1990. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Antal, A.; Alekseichuk, I.; Bikson, M.; Brockmöller, J.; Brunoni, A.R.; Chen, R.; Cohen, L.G.; Dowthwaite, G.; Ellrich, J.; Flöel, A.; et al. Low intensity transcranial electric stimulation: Safety, ethical, legal regulatory and application guidelines. Clin. Neurophysiol. Off. J. Int. Fed. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2017, 128, 1774–1809. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  9. Lefaucheur, J.-P.; Antal, A.; Ayache, S.S.; Benninger, D.H.; Brunelin, J.; Cogiamanian, F.; Cotelli, M.; De Ridder, D.; Ferrucci, R.; Langguth, B.; et al. Evidence-based guidelines on the therapeutic use of transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS). Clin. Neurophysiol. 2017, 128, 56–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Hsu, W.Y.; Ku, Y.; Zanto, T.P.; Gazzaley, A. Effects of noninvasive brain stimulation on cognitive function in healthy aging and Alzheimer's disease: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Neurobiol. Aging 2015, 36, 2348–2359. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Nitsche, M.A.; Paulus, W. Excitability changes induced in the human motor cortex by weak transcranial direct current stimulation. J. Physiol. 2000, 527, 633–639. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. Nitsche, M.A.; Nitsche, M.S.; Klein, C.C.; Tergau, F.; Rothwell, J.C.; Paulus, W. Level of action of cathodal DC polarisation induced inhibition of the human motor cortex. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2003, 114, 600–604. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Nitsche, M.A.; Paulus, W. Sustained excitability elevations induced by transcranial DC motor cortex stimulation in humans. Neurology 2001, 57, 1899–1901. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Nitsche, M.A.; Fricke, K.; Henschke, U.; Schlitterlau, A.; Liebetanz, D.; Lang, N.; Henning, S.; Tergau, F.; Paulus, W. Pharmacological modulation of cortical excitability shifts induced by transcranial direct current stimulation in humans. J. Physiol. 2003, 553, 293–301. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Nitsche, M.A.; Liebetanz, D.; Schlitterlau, A.; Henschke, U.; Fricke, K.; Frommann, K.; Lang, N.; Henning, S.; Paulus, W.; Tergau, F. GABAergic modulation of DC stimulation-induced motor cortex excitability shifts in humans. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2004, 19, 2720–2726. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Stagg, C.J. Magnetic Resonance Spectroscopy as a tool to study the role of GABA in motor-cortical plasticity. NeuroImage 2014, 86, 19–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Stagg, C.J.; Antal, A.; Nitsche, M.A. Physiology of Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation. J. ECT 2018, 34, 144–152. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Stagg, C.J.; Nitsche, M.A. Physiological basis of transcranial direct current stimulation. Neurosci. Rev. J. Bringing Neurobiol. Neurol. Psychiatry 2011, 17, 37–53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Malenka, R.C.; Bear, M.F. LTP and LTD: An embarrassment of riches. Neuron 2004, 44, 5–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  20. Burke, S.N.; Barnes, C.A. Neural plasticity in the ageing brain. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2006, 7, 30–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Rioult-Pedotti, M.-S.; Friedman, D.; Donoghue, J.P. Learning-induced LTP in neocortex. Science 2000, 290, 533–536. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  22. Levasseur-Moreau, J.; Brunelin, J.; Fecteau, S. Non-invasive brain stimulation can induce paradoxical facilitation. Are these neuroenhancements transferable and meaningful to security services? Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2013, 7, 449. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  23. Hara, T.; Shanmugalingam, A.; McIntyre, A.; Burhan, A.M. The effect of non-invasive brain stimulation (nibs) on attention and memory function in stroke rehabilitation patients: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Diagnostics 2021, 11, 227. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. Morishita, T.; Hummel, F.C. Non-invasive Brain Stimulation (NIBS) in Motor Recovery After Stroke: Concepts to Increase Efficacy. Curr. Behav. Neurosci. Rep. 2017, 4, 280–289. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Šimko, P.; Kent, J.A.; Rektorova, I. Is non-invasive brain stimulation effective for cognitive enhancement in Alzheimer’s disease? An updated meta-analysis. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2022, 144, 23–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  26. Flöel, A. tDCS-enhanced motor and cognitive function in neurological diseases. Neuroimage 2014, 85, 934–947. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Borgomaneri, S.; Battaglia, S.; Garofalo, S.; Tortora, F.; Avenanti, A.; di Pellegrino, G. State-Dependent TMS over Prefrontal Cortex Disrupts Fear-Memory Reconsolidation and Prevents the Return of Fear. Curr. Biol. 2020, 30, 3672–3679.e3674. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Garofalo, S.; Battaglia, S.; Starita, F.; di Pellegrino, G. Modulation of cue-guided choices by transcranial direct current stimulation. Cortex 2021, 137, 124–137. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Zec, R.F. The neuropsychology of aging. Exp. Gerontol. 1995, 30, 431–442. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Alexander, G.; Ryan, L.; Bowers, D.; Foster, T.; Bizon, J.; Geldmacher, D.; Glisky, E. Characterizing cognitive aging in humans with links to animal models. Front. Aging Neurosci. 2012, 4, 21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Summers, J.J.; Kang, N.; Cauraugh, J.H. Does transcranial direct current stimulation enhance cognitive and motor functions in the ageing brain? A systematic review and meta- analysis. Ageing Res. Rev. 2016, 25, 42–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  32. Bhandari, A.; Radhu, N.; Farzan, F.; Mulsant, B.H.; Rajji, T.K.; Daskalakis, Z.J.; Blumberger, D.M. A meta-analysis of the effects of aging on motor cortex neurophysiology assessed by transcranial magnetic stimulation. Clin. Neurophysiol. Off. J. Int. Fed. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2016, 127, 2834–2845. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  33. Calautti, C.; Serrati, C.; Baron, J.-C. Effects of Age on Brain Activation During Auditory-Cued Thumb-to-Index Opposition. Stroke 2001, 32, 139–146. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  34. Bedard, A.C.; Nichols, S.; Barbosa, J.A.; Schachar, R.; Logan, G.D.; Tannock, R. The development of selective inhibitory control across the life span. Dev. Neuropsychol. 2002, 21, 93–111. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Welford, A.T. Reaction Time, Speed of Performance, and Age. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1988, 515, 1–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Swinnen, S.; Verschueren, S.; Bogaerts, H.; Dounskaia, N.; Lee, T.; Stelmach, G.; Serrien, D. Age-related deficits in motor learning and differences in feedback processing during the production of a bimanual coordination pattern. Cogn. Neuropsychol. 1998, 15, 439–466. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Serrien, D.J.; Swinnen, S.P.; Stelmach, G.E. Age-related deterioration of coordinated interlimb behavior. J. Gerontol. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 2000, 55, P295–P303. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  38. Heuninckx, S.; Debaere, F.; Wenderoth, N.; Verschueren, S.; Swinnen, S.P. Ipsilateral coordination deficits and central processing requirements associated with coordination as a function of aging. J. Gerontol. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 2004, 59, P225–P232. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  39. Ruch, F.L. The Differentiative Effects of Age upon Human Learning. J. Gen. Psychol. 1934, 11, 261–286. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. McNay, E.C.; Willingham, D.B. Deficit in learning of a motor skill requiring strategy, but not of perceptuomotor recalibration, with aging. Learn. Mem. 1998, 4, 411–420. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Clark, B.C.; Manini, T.M. Sarcopenia =/= dynapenia. J. Gerontol. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 2008, 63, 829–834. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  42. Seidler, R.D.; Bernard, J.A.; Burutolu, T.B.; Fling, B.W.; Gordon, M.T.; Gwin, J.T.; Kwak, Y.; Lipps, D.B. Motor control and aging: Links to age-related brain structural, functional, and biochemical effects. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2010, 34, 721–733. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  43. Buckles, V.D. Age-Related Slowing. In Sensorimotor Impairment in the Elderly; Stelmach, G.E., Hömberg, V., Eds.; Springer: Dordrecht, The Netherlands, 1993; pp. 73–87. [Google Scholar]
  44. Darbin, O. The aging striatal dopamine function. Park. Relat. Disord. 2012, 18, 426–432. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  45. Heise, K.F.; Zimerman, M.; Hoppe, J.; Gerloff, C.; Wegscheider, K.; Hummel, F.C. The aging motor system as a model for plastic changes of GABA-mediated intracortical inhibition and their behavioral relevance. J. Neurosci. Off. J. Soc. Neurosci. 2013, 33, 9039–9049. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  46. Zhang, Z.; Andersen, A.; Smith, C.; Grondin, R.; Gerhardt, G.; Gash, D. Motor Slowing and Parkinsonian Signs in Aging Rhesus Monkeys Mirror Human Aging. J. Gerontol. Ser. A 2000, 55, B473–B480. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  47. Raw, R.K.; Allen, R.J.; Mon-Williams, M.; Wilkie, R.M. Motor Sequence Learning in Healthy Older Adults Is Not Necessarily Facilitated by Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation (tDCS). Geriatrics 2016, 1, 32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  48. Prehn, K.; Flöel, A. Potentials and limits to enhance cognitive functions in healthy and pathological aging by tDCS. Front. Cell. Neurosci. 2015, 9, 355. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  49. Bennabi, D.; Pedron, S.; Haffen, E.; Monnin, J.; Peterschmitt, Y.; Van Waes, V. Transcranial direct current stimulation for memory enhancement: From clinical research to animal models. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 2014, 8, 159. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  50. Halsband, U.; Lange, R.K. Motor learning in man: A review of functional and clinical studies. J. Physiol. Paris 2006, 99, 414–424. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Doyon, J.; Bellec, P.; Amsel, R.; Penhune, V.; Monchi, O.; Carrier, J.; Lehéricy, S.; Benali, H. Contributions of the basal ganglia and functionally related brain structures to motor learning. Behav. Brain Res. 2009, 199, 61–75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Ammann, C.; Spampinato, D.; Márquez-Ruiz, J. Modulating Motor Learning through Transcranial Direct-Current Stimulation: An Integrative View. Front. Psychol. 2016, 7, 1981. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  53. Clark, D.; Ivry, R.B. Multiple systems for motor skill learning. WIREs Cogn. Sci. 2010, 1, 461–467. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  54. Kelley, A.E.; Andrzejewski, M.E.; Baldwin, A.E.; Hernandez, P.J.; Pratt, W.E. Glutamate-Mediated Plasticity in Corticostriatal Networks. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 2003, 1003, 159–168. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Kim, S.; Stephenson, M.C.; Morris, P.G.; Jackson, S.R. tDCS-induced alterations in GABA concentration within primary motor cortex predict motor learning and motor memory: A 7 T magnetic resonance spectroscopy study. NeuroImage 2014, 99, 237–243. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  56. Castro-Alamancos, M.A.; Connors, B.W. Short-term synaptic enhancement and long-term potentiation in neocortex. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 1996, 93, 1335–1339. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  57. Pleger, B.; Schwenkreis, P.; Dinse, H.; Ragert, P.; Höffken, O.; Malin, J.-P.; Tegenthoff, M. Pharmacological suppression of plastic changes in human primary somatosensory cortex after motor learning. Exp. Brain Res. Exp. Hirnforschung. Expérimentation Cérébrale 2003, 148, 525–532. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Amadi, U.; Allman, C.; Johansen-Berg, H.; Stagg, C.J. The Homeostatic Interaction Between Anodal Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation and Motor Learning in Humans is Related to GABAA Activity. Brain Stimul. 2015, 8, 898–905. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  59. Roalf, D.R.; Sydnor, V.J.; Woods, M.; Wolk, D.A.; Scott, J.C.; Reddy, R.; Moberg, P.J. A quantitative meta-analysis of brain glutamate metabolites in aging. Neurobiol. Aging 2020, 95, 240–249. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Segovia, G.; Del Arco, A.; Mora, F. Effects of Aging on the Interaction Between Glutamate, Dopamine, and GABA in Striatum and Nucleus Accumbens of the Awake Rat. J. Neurochem. 1999, 73, 2063–2072. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Antonenko, D.; Thielscher, A.; Saturnino, G.B.; Aydin, S.; Ittermann, B.; Grittner, U.; Flöel, A. Towards precise brain stimulation: Is electric field simulation related to neuromodulation? Brain Stimul. 2019, 12, 1159–1168. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Wenk, G.L.; Walker, L.C.; Price, D.L.; Cork, L.C. Loss of NMDA, but not GABA-A, binding in the brains of aged rats and monkeys. Neurobiol. Aging 1991, 12, 93–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  63. Levin, O.; Fujiyama, H.; Boisgontier, M.P.; Swinnen, S.P.; Summers, J.J. Aging and motor inhibition: A converging perspective provided by brain stimulation and imaging approaches. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2014, 43, 100–117. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Gleichmann, M.; Chow, V.W.; Mattson, M.P. Homeostatic disinhibition in the aging brain and Alzheimer's disease. J. Alzheimer's Dis. 2011, 24, 15–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  65. Dumel, G.; Bourassa, M.-E.; Charlebois-Plante, C.; Desjardins, M.; Doyon, J.; Saint-Amour, D.; De Beaumont, L. Motor Learning Improvement Remains 3 Months After a Multisession Anodal tDCS Intervention in an Aging Population. Front. Aging Neurosci. 2018, 10, 335. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  66. Nitsche, M.A.; Schauenburg, A.; Lang, N.; Liebetanz, D.; Exner, C.; Paulus, W.; Tergau, F. Facilitation of implicit motor learning by weak transcranial direct current stimulation of the primary motor cortex in the human. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2003, 15, 619–626. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  67. Galea, J.M.; Celnik, P. Brain polarization enhances the formation and retention of motor memories. J. Neurophysiol. 2009, 102, 294–301. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  68. Kantak, S.S.; Mummidisetty, C.K.; Stinear, J.W. Primary motor and premotor cortex in implicit sequence learning--evidence for competition between implicit and explicit human motor memory systems. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2012, 36, 2710–2715. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Kang, E.K.; Paik, N.-J. Effect of a tDCS electrode montage on implicit motor sequence learning in healthy subjects. Exp. Transl. Stroke Med. 2011, 3, 4. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  70. Cuypers, K.; Leenus, D.J.F.; van den Berg, F.E.; Nitsche, M.A.; Thijs, H.; Wenderoth, N.; Meesen, R.L.J. Is motor learning mediated by tDCS intensity? PLoS ONE 2013, 8, e67344. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Ehsani, F.; Bakhtiary, A.H.; Jaberzadeh, S.; Talimkhani, A.; Hajihasani, A. Differential effects of primary motor cortex and cerebellar transcranial direct current stimulation on motor learning in healthy individuals: A randomized double-blind sham-controlled study. Neurosci. Res. 2016, 112, 10–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Reis, J.; Schambra, H.M.; Cohen, L.G.; Buch, E.R.; Fritsch, B.; Zarahn, E.; Celnik, P.A.; Krakauer, J.W. Noninvasive cortical stimulation enhances motor skill acquisition over multiple days through an effect on consolidation. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2009, 106, 1590–1595. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  73. Stagg, C.J.; Jayaram, G.; Pastor, D.; Kincses, Z.T.; Matthews, P.M.; Johansen-Berg, H. Polarity and timing-dependent effects of transcranial direct current stimulation in explicit motor learning. Neuropsychologia 2011, 49, 800–804. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  74. Kuo, M.-F.; Unger, M.; Liebetanz, D.; Lang, N.; Tergau, F.; Paulus, W.; Nitsche, M.A. Limited impact of homeostatic plasticity on motor learning in humans. Neuropsychologia 2008, 46, 2122–2128. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  75. Buch, E.R.; Santarnecchi, E.; Antal, A.; Born, J.; Celnik, P.A.; Classen, J.; Gerloff, C.; Hallett, M.; Hummel, F.C.; Nitsche, M.A.; et al. Effects of tDCS on motor learning and memory formation: A consensus and critical position paper. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2017, 128, 589–603. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  76. Hummel, F.C.; Heise, K.; Celnik, P.; Floel, A.; Gerloff, C.; Cohen, L.G. Facilitating skilled right hand motor function in older subjects by anodal polarization over the left primary motor cortex. Neurobiol. Aging 2010, 31, 2160–2168. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  77. Zimerman, M.; Hummel, F.C. Non-invasive brain stimulation: Enhancing motor and cognitive functions in healthy old subjects. Front. Aging Neurosci. 2010, 2, 149. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  78. Zimerman, M.; Nitsch, M.; Giraux, P.; Gerloff, C.; Cohen, L.G.; Hummel, F.C. Neuroenhancement of the aging brain: Restoring skill acquisition in old subjects. Ann. Neurol. 2013, 73, 10–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  79. Rumpf, J.-J.; Wegscheider, M.; Hinselmann, K.; Fricke, C.; King, B.R.; Weise, D.; Klann, J.; Binkofski, F.; Buccino, G.; Karni, A.; et al. Enhancement of motor consolidation by post-training transcranial direct current stimulation in older people. Neurobiol. Aging 2017, 49, 1–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  80. Fregni, F.; Boggio, P.S.; Santos, M.C.; Lima, M.; Vieira, A.L.; Rigonatti, S.P.; Silva, M.T.A.; Barbosa, E.R.; Nitsche, M.A.; Pascual-Leone, A. Noninvasive cortical stimulation with transcranial direct current stimulation in Parkinson's disease. Mov. Disord. 2006, 21, 1693–1702. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  81. Andrade, S.M.; Ferreira, J.J.d.A.; Rufino, T.S.; Medeiros, G.; Brito, J.D.; da Silva, M.A.; Moreira, R.d.N. Effects of different montages of transcranial direct current stimulation on the risk of falls and lower limb function after stroke. Neurol. Res. 2017, 39, 1037–1043. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  82. Saeys, W.; Vereeck, L.; Lafosse, C.; Truijen, S.; Wuyts, F.L.; Van De Heyning, P. Transcranial direct current stimulation in the recovery of postural control after stroke: A pilot study. Disabil. Rehabil. 2015, 37, 1857–1863. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  83. Yosephi, M.H.; Ehsani, F.; Zoghi, M.; Jaberzadeh, S. Multi-session anodal tDCS enhances the effects of postural training on balance and postural stability in older adults with high fall risk: Primary motor cortex versus cerebellar stimulation. Brain Stimul. 2018, 11, 1239–1250. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  84. Fregni, F.; Boggio, P.S.; Mansur, C.G.; Wagner, T.; Ferreira, M.J.; Lima, M.C.; Rigonatti, S.P.; Marcolin, M.A.; Freedman, S.D.; Nitsche, M.A.; et al. Transcranial direct current stimulation of the unaffected hemisphere in stroke patients. Neuroreport 2005, 16, 1551–1555. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  85. Rossi, C.; Sallustio, F.; Di Legge, S.; Stanzione, P.; Koch, G. Transcranial direct current stimulation of the affected hemisphere does not accelerate recovery of acute stroke patients. Eur. J. Neurol. 2013, 20, 202–204. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  86. Koini, M.; Duering, M.; Gesierich, B.G.; Rombouts, S.A.R.B.; Ropele, S.; Wagner, F.; Enzinger, C.; Schmidt, R. Grey-matter network disintegration as predictor of cognitive and motor function with aging. Brain Struct. Funct. 2018, 223, 2475–2487. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  87. Nandi, T.; Puonti, O.; Clarke, W.T.; Nettekoven, C.; Barron, H.C.; Kolasinski, J.; Hanayik, T.; Hinson, E.L.; Berrington, A.; Bachtiar, V.; et al. tDCS induced GABA change is associated with the simulated electric field in M1, an effect mediated by grey matter volume in the MRS voxel. Brain Stimul. 2022, 15, 1153–1162. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  88. Fathi, D.; Ueki, Y.; Mima, T.; Koganemaru, S.; Nagamine, T.; Tawfik, A.; Fukuyama, H. Effects of aging on the human motor cortical plasticity studied by paired associative stimulation. Clin. Neurophysiol. Off. J. Int. Fed. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2010, 121, 90–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  89. Foster, T.C. Involvement of hippocampal synaptic plasticity in age-related memory decline. Brain Res. Brain Res. Rev. 1999, 30, 236–249. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  90. Ghasemian-Shirvan, E.; Farnad, L.; Mosayebi-Samani, M.; Verstraelen, S.; Meesen, R.L.J.; Kuo, M.-F.; Nitsche, M.A. Age-related differences of motor cortex plasticity in adults: A transcranial direct current stimulation study. Brain Stimul. 2020, 13, 1588–1599. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  91. Freitas, C.; Farzan, F.; Pascual-Leone, A. Assessing brain plasticity across the lifespan with transcranial magnetic stimulation: Why, how, and what is the ultimate goal? Front. Neurosci. 2013, 7, 42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  92. Balietti, M.; Tamagnini, F.; Fattoretti, P.; Burattini, C.; Casoli, T.; Platano, D.; Lattanzio, F.; Aicardi, G. Impairments of synaptic plasticity in aged animals and in animal models of Alzheimer's disease. Rejuvenation Res. 2012, 15, 235–238. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  93. Farnad, L.; Ghasemian-Shirvan, E.; Mosayebi-Samani, M.; Kuo, M.-F.; Nitsche, M.A. Exploring and optimizing the neuroplastic effects of anodal transcranial direct current stimulation over the primary motor cortex of older humans. Brain Stimul. 2021, 14, 622–634. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  94. Bikson, M.; Grossman, P.; Thomas, C.; Zannou, A.L.; Jiang, J.; Adnan, T.; Mourdoukoutas, A.P.; Kronberg, G.; Truong, D.; Boggio, P.; et al. Safety of Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation: Evidence Based Update 2016. Brain Stimul. 2016, 9, 641–661. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  95. Rossi, S.; Hallett, M.; Rossini, P.M.; Pascual-Leone, A. Safety, ethical considerations, and application guidelines for the use of transcranial magnetic stimulation in clinical practice and research. Clin. Neurophysiol. Off. J. Int. Fed. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2009, 120, 2008–2039. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  96. Oldfield, R.C. The assessment and analysis of handedness: The Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia 1971, 9, 97–113. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  97. Nasreddine, Z.S.; Phillips, N.A.; Bédirian, V.; Charbonneau, S.; Whitehead, V.; Collin, I.; Cummings, J.L.; Chertkow, H. The Montreal Cognitive Assessment, MoCA: A brief screening tool for mild cognitive impairment. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 2005, 53, 695–699. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  98. Association, W.M. World Medical Association Declaration of Helsinki: Ethical Principles for Medical Research Involving Human Subjects. JAMA 2013, 310, 2191–2194. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  99. Guleyupoglu, B.; Febles, N.; Minhas, P.; Hahn, C.; Bikson, M. Reduced discomfort during high-definition transcutaneous stimulation using 6% benzocaine. Front. Neuroeng. 2014, 7, 28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  100. Robertson, E.M. The Serial Reaction Time Task: Implicit Motor Skill Learning? J. Neurosci. 2007, 27, 10073–10075. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  101. Poreisz, C.; Boros, K.; Antal, A.; Paulus, W. Safety aspects of transcranial direct current stimulation concerning healthy subjects and patients. Brain Res. Bull. 2007, 72, 208–214. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  102. Brunoni, A.R.; Amadera, J.; Berbel, B.; Volz, M.S.; Rizzerio, B.G.; Fregni, F. A systematic review on reporting and assessment of adverse effects associated with transcranial direct current stimulation. Int. J. Neuropsychopharmacol. 2011, 14, 1133–1145. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  103. Savic, B.; Meier, B. How Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation Can Modulate Implicit Motor Sequence Learning and Consolidation: A Brief Review. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2016, 10, 26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  104. López-Alonso, V.; Cheeran, B.; Fernández-del-Olmo, M. Relationship Between Non-invasive Brain Stimulation-induced Plasticity and Capacity for Motor Learning. Brain Stimul. 2015, 8, 1209–1219. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  105. Bachtiar, V.; Stagg, C.J. The role of inhibition in human motor cortical plasticity. Neuroscience 2014, 278, 93–104. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  106. King, B.R.; Rumpf, J.J.; Verbaanderd, E.; Heise, K.F.; Dolfen, N.; Sunaert, S.; Doyon, J.; Classen, J.; Mantini, D.; Puts, N.A.J.; et al. Baseline sensorimotor GABA levels shape neuroplastic processes induced by motor learning in older adults. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2020, 41, 3680–3695. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  107. Kolasinski, J.; Hinson, E.L.; Divanbeighi Zand, A.P.; Rizov, A.; Emir, U.E.; Stagg, C.J. The dynamics of cortical GABA in human motor learning. J. Physiol. 2019, 597, 271–282. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  108. Stagg, C.J.; Bachtiar, V.; Johansen-Berg, H. The Role of GABA in Human Motor Learning. Curr. Biol. 2011, 21, 480–484. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  109. Antonenko, D.; Schubert, F.; Bohm, F.; Ittermann, B.; Aydin, S.; Hayek, D.; Grittner, U.; Floel, A. tDCS-Induced Modulation of GABA Levels and Resting-State Functional Connectivity in Older Adults. J. Neurosci. Off. J. Soc. Neurosci. 2017, 37, 4065–4073. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  110. Floyer-Lea, A.; Wylezinska, M.; Kincses, T.; Matthews, P.M. Rapid modulation of GABA concentration in human sensorimotor cortex during motor learning. J. Neurophysiol. 2006, 95, 1639–1644. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  111. Berghuis, K.M.M.; De Rond, V.; Zijdewind, I.; Koch, G.; Veldman, M.P.; Hortobágyi, T. Neuronal mechanisms of motor learning are age dependent. Neurobiol. Aging 2016, 46, 149–159. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  112. Cuypers, K.; Verstraelen, S.; Maes, C.; Hermans, L.; Hehl, M.; Heise, K.F.; Chalavi, S.; Mikkelsen, M.; Edden, R.; Levin, O.; et al. Task-related measures of short-interval intracortical inhibition and GABA levels in healthy young and older adults: A multimodal TMS-MRS study. NeuroImage 2020, 208, 116470. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  113. Chang, L.; Jiang, C.S.; Ernst, T. Effects of age and sex on brain glutamate and other metabolites. Magn. Reson. Imaging 2009, 27, 142–145. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  114. Hadel, S.; Wirth, C.; Rapp, M.; Gallinat, J.; Schubert, F. Effects of age and sex on the concentrations of glutamate and glutamine in the human brain. J. Magn. Reson. Imaging JMRI 2013, 38, 1480–1487. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  115. Kaiser, L.G.; Schuff, N.; Cashdollar, N.; Weiner, M.W. Age-related glutamate and glutamine concentration changes in normal human brain: 1H MR spectroscopy study at 4 T. Neurobiol. Aging 2005, 26, 665–672. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  116. Sawaki, L.; Yaseen, Z.; Kopylev, L.; Cohen, L.G. Age-dependent changes in the ability to encode a novel elementary motor memory. Ann. Neurol. 2003, 53, 521–524. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  117. Fujiyama, H.; Garry, M.I.; Levin, O.; Swinnen, S.P.; Summers, J.J. Age-related differences in inhibitory processes during interlimb coordination. Brain Res. 2009, 1262, 38–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  118. Rogasch, N.C.; Dartnall, T.J.; Cirillo, J.; Nordstrom, M.A.; Semmler, J.G. Corticomotor plasticity and learning of a ballistic thumb training task are diminished in older adults. J. Appl. Physiol. 2009, 107, 1874–1883. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  119. Antal, A.; Nitsche, M.A.; Kruse, W.; Kincses, T.Z.; Hoffmann, K.-P.; Paulus, W. Direct current stimulation over V5 enhances visuomotor coordination by improving motion perception in humans. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2004, 16, 521–527. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  120. Goh, J.O.S. Functional Dedifferentiation and Altered Connectivity in Older Adults: Neural Accounts of Cognitive Aging. Aging Dis. 2011, 2, 30–48. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  121. Goh, J.O.; Park, D.C. Neuroplasticity and cognitive aging: The scaffolding theory of aging and cognition. Restor. Neurol. Neurosci. 2009, 27, 391–403. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  122. Mosayebi-Samani, M.; Jamil, A.; Salvador, R.; Ruffini, G.; Haueisen, J.; Nitsche, M.A. The impact of individual electrical fields and anatomical factors on the neurophysiological outcomes of tDCS: A TMS-MEP and MRI study. Brain Stimul. 2021, 14, 316–326. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  123. Polania, R.; Nitsche, M.A.; Paulus, W. Modulating functional connectivity patterns and topological functional organization of the human brain with transcranial direct current stimulation. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2011, 32, 1236–1249. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  124. Cassady, K.; Gagnon, H.; Lalwani, P.; Simmonite, M.; Foerster, B.; Park, D.; Peltier, S.J.; Petrou, M.; Taylor, S.F.; Weissman, D.H.; et al. Sensorimotor network segregation declines with age and is linked to GABA and to sensorimotor performance. NeuroImage 2019, 186, 234–244. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  125. Heuninckx, S.; Wenderoth, N.; Swinnen, S.P. Systems neuroplasticity in the aging brain: Recruiting additional neural resources for successful motor performance in elderly persons. J. Neurosci. Off. J. Soc. Neurosci. 2008, 28, 91–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  126. Galea, J.M.; Vazquez, A.; Pasricha, N.; de Xivry, J.J.; Celnik, P. Dissociating the roles of the cerebellum and motor cortex during adaptive learning: The motor cortex retains what the cerebellum learns. Cereb. Cortex 2011, 21, 1761–1770. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Course of the experiment. The anode electrode was placed over the left primary motor cortex (C3) and the cathode over the right supraorbital area. In a random order, sham, 1, 2, or 3 mA of tDCS was delivered in separate sessions while participants performed the serial reaction time task. Participants performed the recall session the following day.
Figure 1. Course of the experiment. The anode electrode was placed over the left primary motor cortex (C3) and the cathode over the right supraorbital area. In a random order, sham, 1, 2, or 3 mA of tDCS was delivered in separate sessions while participants performed the serial reaction time task. Participants performed the recall session the following day.
Brainsci 13 00137 g001
Figure 2. Results of the M-SRTT. The graphs show results for (A) absolute RT, (B) normalized RT, (C) number of errors, and (D) variability of absolute RT across different tDCS protocols. Filled symbols indicate a significant difference compared to block 1 in each condition. Asterisks (⁎) refer to significant reaction time differences between block 5–6 (sequence-random block) or block 6–7 (random-sequence block). Error bars represent standard error of means.
Figure 2. Results of the M-SRTT. The graphs show results for (A) absolute RT, (B) normalized RT, (C) number of errors, and (D) variability of absolute RT across different tDCS protocols. Filled symbols indicate a significant difference compared to block 1 in each condition. Asterisks (⁎) refer to significant reaction time differences between block 5–6 (sequence-random block) or block 6–7 (random-sequence block). Error bars represent standard error of means.
Brainsci 13 00137 g002aBrainsci 13 00137 g002b
Figure 3. Results of R-SRTT. The graphs show (A) absolute reaction time, (B) normalized reaction time, (C) number of errors, and (D) variability of absolute reaction time of R-SRTT performance 24 h after each tDCS protocol. Filled symbols indicate a significant difference in each block compared to block 1. Asterisks (⁎) refer to significant differences of block 2 to block 3 (both sequence blocks). Error bars represent standard error of means.
Figure 3. Results of R-SRTT. The graphs show (A) absolute reaction time, (B) normalized reaction time, (C) number of errors, and (D) variability of absolute reaction time of R-SRTT performance 24 h after each tDCS protocol. Filled symbols indicate a significant difference in each block compared to block 1. Asterisks (⁎) refer to significant differences of block 2 to block 3 (both sequence blocks). Error bars represent standard error of means.
Brainsci 13 00137 g003aBrainsci 13 00137 g003b
Figure 4. Absolute reaction time M-SRTT and R-SRTT. The graph displays the absolute reaction time of main and recall SRTT in all conditions.
Figure 4. Absolute reaction time M-SRTT and R-SRTT. The graph displays the absolute reaction time of main and recall SRTT in all conditions.
Brainsci 13 00137 g004
Table 1. Effect of tDCS on M-SRTT. Results of the repeated measures ANOVAs for the M-SRTT: (A) absolute RTs, (B) normalized RTs, (C) number of errors, and (D) variability of absolute RTs are shown. Asterisks indicate significant results (p < 0.05). d.f. = degrees of freedom, η p 2 = partial eta squared.
Table 1. Effect of tDCS on M-SRTT. Results of the repeated measures ANOVAs for the M-SRTT: (A) absolute RTs, (B) normalized RTs, (C) number of errors, and (D) variability of absolute RTs are shown. Asterisks indicate significant results (p < 0.05). d.f. = degrees of freedom, η p 2 = partial eta squared.
Factord.f., ErrorF Value η p 2 P Value
(A) Absolute
RT
Condition3, 690.8270.0350.484
Block2.521, 57.97713.4860.370<0.001 *
Condition × Block8.790, 202.1691.2900.0530.245
(B) Normalized
RT
Condition3, 690.5720.0240.635
Block2.502, 57.53914.9960.395<0.001 *
Condition × Block8.982, 206.5951.1920.0490.302
(C) ErrorsCondition1.785, 41.0440.4080.0170.645
Block2.639, 60.6950.6370.0270.575
Condition × Block3.763, 86.5481.2860.0530.282
(D) VariabilityCondition3, 690.5380.0230.658
Block2.742, 63.06911.7870.339<0.001 *
Condition × Block9.399, 216.1710.7430.0310.788
Table 2. Effect of tDCS on R-SRTT. Results of the mixed-model ANOVAs conducted for R-SRTT: (A) absolute RTs, (B) normalized RTs, (C) number of errors, and (D) variability of absolute RTs. Asterisks indicate significant results (p < 0.05). d.f. = degrees of freedom, η p 2 = partial eta squared.
Table 2. Effect of tDCS on R-SRTT. Results of the mixed-model ANOVAs conducted for R-SRTT: (A) absolute RTs, (B) normalized RTs, (C) number of errors, and (D) variability of absolute RTs. Asterisks indicate significant results (p < 0.05). d.f. = degrees of freedom, η p 2 = partial eta squared.
Factord.f., ErrorF Value η p 2 P Value
(A) Absolute
RT
Condition3, 690.3510.0150.789
Block1.554, 35.75142.7610.650<0.001 *
Condition × Block3.773, 86.7800.5170.0220.713
(B) Normalized
RT
Condition3, 690.1390.0060.936
Block1.397, 32.12841.4880.643<0.001 *
Condition × Block3.634, 83.5870.5080.0220.712
(C) ErrorsCondition1.779, 40.9121.1180.0460.331
Block2, 460.6660.0280.517
Condition × Block3.584, 82.4370.5980.0250.647
(D) VariabilityCondition3, 690.4720.0200.703
Block1.565, 36.0016.0240.2080.009 *
Condition × Block6, 1382.6430.1030.019*
Table 3. Data points of guessed versus received tDCS intensity. After each session, participants filled in a questionnaire asking them to guess the intensity of applied tDCS (0, 1, 2, 3 mA). The table contrasts actually applied (rows) with perceived stimulation intensities (columns).
Table 3. Data points of guessed versus received tDCS intensity. After each session, participants filled in a questionnaire asking them to guess the intensity of applied tDCS (0, 1, 2, 3 mA). The table contrasts actually applied (rows) with perceived stimulation intensities (columns).
Data PointsGuessed Stimulation IntensityTotal Data Points
Sham1 mA2 mA3 mA
Real tDCS IntensitySham1461425
1 mA1643225
2 mA1362425
3 mA1363325
Table 4. Side effect ratings included visual phenomena, itching, tingling, and pain during stimulation, and skin redness, headache, fatigue, difficulty in concentration, nervousness, and sleep problems within 24 h after stimulation. The presence and intensity of side effects were rated on a numerical scale from zero (feeling nothing) to five (extremely strong feeling). Data are presented as mean ± SD.
Table 4. Side effect ratings included visual phenomena, itching, tingling, and pain during stimulation, and skin redness, headache, fatigue, difficulty in concentration, nervousness, and sleep problems within 24 h after stimulation. The presence and intensity of side effects were rated on a numerical scale from zero (feeling nothing) to five (extremely strong feeling). Data are presented as mean ± SD.
Side EffectsSham1 mA2 mA3 mA
Visual Phenomena0.08 ± 0.400.04 ± 0.200.37 ± 1.270.12 ± 0.33
Itching0.00 ± 0.000.00 ± 0.000.12 ± 0.440.25 ± 0.67
Tingling0.04 ± 0.200.04 ± 0.200.04 ± 0.200.16 ± 0.48
Burning0.04 ± 0.200.12 ± 0.610.20 ± 0.650.20 ± 0.65
Pain0.00 ± 0.000.00 ± 0.000.00 ± 0.000.12 ± 0.44
Redness0.00 ± 0.000.16 ± 0.560.08 ± 0.400.08 ± 0.40
Headache0.00 ± 0.000.12 ± 0.610.08 ± 0.400.04 ± 0.20
Fatigue0.20 ± 0.500.12 ± 0.440.16 ± 0.480.08 ± 0.28
Concentration0.08 ± 0.280.08 ± 0.280.20 ± 0.410.16 ± 0.38
Nervousness0.00 ± 0.000.00 ± 0.000.00 ± 0.000.04 ± 0.20
Sleep Problems0.00 ± 0.000.04 ± 0.200.08 ± 0.400.04 ± 0.20
Table 5. Side effect ratings of the participants, ANOVA results. The results showed no significant effect for any of the side effects during or within 24 h following stimulation.
Table 5. Side effect ratings of the participants, ANOVA results. The results showed no significant effect for any of the side effects during or within 24 h following stimulation.
Side Effectsd.f., ErrorF Value η p 2 P Value
During StimulationVisual phenomena1.216, 27.9671.1740.0490.300
Itching1.692, 38.9222.2130.0880.094
Tingling1.000, 23.0001.8650.0750.185
Burning1.690, 38.8661.1050.0460.333
Pain1.000, 23.0001.8650.0750.185
After StimulationRedness1.428, 32.8400.7930.0330.422
Headache1.000, 23.0001.0000.0420.328
Fatigue2.284, 52.5421.2640.0520.294
Concentration3, 691.0840.0450.362
Nervousness1.000, 23.0001.0000.0420.328
Sleep Problems1.308, 30.0880.5610.0240.505
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Ghasemian-Shirvan, E.; Ungureanu, R.; Melo, L.; van Dun, K.; Kuo, M.-F.; Nitsche, M.A.; Meesen, R.L.J. Optimizing the Effect of tDCS on Motor Sequence Learning in the Elderly. Brain Sci. 2023, 13, 137. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci13010137

AMA Style

Ghasemian-Shirvan E, Ungureanu R, Melo L, van Dun K, Kuo M-F, Nitsche MA, Meesen RLJ. Optimizing the Effect of tDCS on Motor Sequence Learning in the Elderly. Brain Sciences. 2023; 13(1):137. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci13010137

Chicago/Turabian Style

Ghasemian-Shirvan, Ensiyeh, Ruxandra Ungureanu, Lorena Melo, Kim van Dun, Min-Fang Kuo, Michael A. Nitsche, and Raf L. J. Meesen. 2023. "Optimizing the Effect of tDCS on Motor Sequence Learning in the Elderly" Brain Sciences 13, no. 1: 137. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci13010137

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop