Next Article in Journal
Hyperammonaemic Encephalopathy Caused by Adult-Onset Ornithine Transcarbamylase Deficiency
Previous Article in Journal
Strategies to Limit Cognitive Impairments under Sleep Restriction: Relationship to Stress Biomarkers
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Is It Just Face Blindness? Exploring Developmental Comorbidity in Individuals with Self-Reported Developmental Prosopagnosia

Department of Psychology, University of Copenhagen, DK-1353 Copenhagen, Denmark
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Brain Sci. 2022, 12(2), 230; https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci12020230
Submission received: 16 December 2021 / Revised: 31 January 2022 / Accepted: 1 February 2022 / Published: 8 February 2022
(This article belongs to the Section Neuropsychology)

Abstract

:
Developmental prosopagnosia (DP)—or ‘face blindness’—refers to life-long problems with facial recognition in the absence of brain injury. We know that neurodevelopmental disorders tend to co-occur, and this study aims to explore if individuals with self-reported DP also report indications of other neurodevelopmental disorders, deficits, or conditions (developmental comorbidity). In total, 115 individuals with self-reported DP participated in this online cross-sectional survey. Face recognition impairment was measured with a validated self-report instrument. Indications of difficulties with navigation, math, reading, or spelling were measured with a tailored questionnaire using items from published sources. Additional diagnoses were measured with direct questions. We also included open-ended questions about cognitive strengths and difficulties. Results: Overall, 57% reported at minimum one developmental comorbidity of interest, with most reflecting specific cognitive impairment (e.g., in memory or object recognition) rather than diagnostic categories (e.g., ADHD, dyslexia). Interestingly, many participants reported cognitive skills or strengths within the same domains that others reported impairment, indicating a diverse pattern of cognitive strengths and difficulties in this sample. The frequency and diversity of self-reported developmental comorbidity suggests that face recognition could be important to consider in future investigations of neurodevelopmental comorbidity patterns.

1. Introduction

Without the ability to recognize faces, one would live in a world of strangers. This is the reality of people suffering from developmental prosopagnosia (DP), a neurodevelopmental disorder of face recognition. The hallmark of this disorder is severely impaired face recognition in the absence of brain injury, intellectual impairment, or significantly impaired vision [1]. Consequences of DP vary in severity but may include anxiety and compromised social and occupational functioning [2,3,4]. While acquired prosopagnosia following brain injury to the occipito-temporal or anterior temporal regions [5,6] has been studied for many decades, DP did not receive notable research interest until this millennium [7,8]. This is intriguing, as estimates suggest that 2–2.5% of the population suffer from DP [9,10], but see [11] for a discussion. In comparison, more known disorders, like obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD) or attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), have a (lifetime) prevalence of 1.3% [12] and 2.8–4.4% [13,14], respectively. DP research has primarily been concerned with mechanistic or cognitive interpretations of the disorder, and in particular the question of whether individuals with DP also have abnormalities in other domains [15,16,17,18]. Specifically, it has been debated whether object agnosia and reading difficulties could be associated with DP, and this has been linked to discussions about the functional organization of high-level vision, e.g., [19,20,21,22,23,24], see [25] and accompanying commentaries for review. However, the broader question of whether face recognition deficits might be overlapping with other neurodevelopmental disorders or conditions has received less attention, and here we aim to take the initial steps to explore this question.
It is well-known that neurodevelopmental disorders tend to cluster and co-occur [16,26,27,28], e.g., comorbid dyslexia and ADHD [29,30], but the co-occurrence of DP or face recognition problems with other diagnostic categories is largely unknown. Anecdotal evidence and some initial studies do suggest some overlap between DP and some other specific cognitive deficits/disorders with a developmental origin, e.g., navigation problems (developmental topographical disorientation) e.g., [31] and dyslexia e.g., [32], but if and how DP or face recognition deficits fits into a larger ‘comorbidity space’ of neurodevelopmental disorders is at present unknown.
The exception is the relation to autism spectrum disorder (ASD), where observations of face recognition deficits are common [33,34,35]. However, ASD often presents with severe social and intellectual deficits not found in DP, which is why ASD is one of the most common exclusion criteria from DP studies [1]. In addition, ASD can co-occur with a range of other neurodevelopmental disorders independently of face processing deficits. For these reasons, the potential overlap between ASD and DP is not considered in the present study, and participants with ASD were excluded from participation.
One reason why neurodevelopmental comorbidity in DP has not been described could be the lack of a gold standard to identify DP. Studies investigating DP have typically applied few and broad exclusion criteria, which also vary substantially between studies [1,11]. The majority of studies investigating DP include participants performing 1.7–2 standard deviations (SD) lower than a control mean of one or more face processing tests [11], e.g., The Cambridge Face Memory Test (CFMT) [36], and the most common exclusion criteria are ASD, brain damage, and/or neurological disorder [21,25,37,38,39]. Thus, with the exception of ASD, potential co-occurring neurodevelopmental disorders are typically not part of inclusion or exclusion criteria in studies of DP.
In a first attempt to address the question of neurodevelopmental comorbidity in DP, we took an exploratory and broad approach to allow for potentially valuable unexpected findings [40], an approach which of course also presents with a number of study design challenges that may limit the generalizability of the results.
We asked whether self-reported developmental prosopagnosia (s-DP) is likely to co-occur with other neurodevelopmental disorders or conditions (comorbidities), and if so, to what degree. To this aim, we asked how people with s-DP rate their navigation, reading, spelling, and mathematical abilities, which all represent potentially related cognitive abilities/impairments [15,41]. In addition, using directed content analysis, which is a qualitative descriptive approach, we analyzed the respondents’ own descriptions of their difficulties as well as potential strengths [42]. Because of the lack of previous knowledge to guide hypotheses, we chose a coarse-grained and exploratory approach, and thus there were no formal statistical hypotheses to be tested. We were, however, particularly interested in the relationship between the severity of self-reported face recognition deficits and presence of comorbidities and exploring the relationship between ratings of face recognition, and items regarding navigation, reading, spelling, and mathematical abilities. In addition, we were interested to see whether open-ended questions could provide indications of possible comorbidities that have not previously been explored in relation to DP. We would like to acknowledge from the start that the participants in this study were self-referred and their face recognition impairment was assessed by a questionnaire only. Thus, we cannot determine whether our participants would qualify for an actual diagnosis of DP based on the present data, as this demands additional objective evidence of impairment on tests of face recognition [11,43]. We do, however, still think our findings can provide a starting point for future investigations of the relationship between face recognition impairment and developmental disorders.

2. Material and Methods

2.1. Which Neurodevelopmental Deficits and Conditions Could Be Relevant to Assess?

While we wanted to take a broad and open approach, we also used existing literature to guide the inclusion of relevant types of developmental disorders or deficits previously investigated in relation to face processing or DP as a starting point. These included navigation problems/developmental topographical disorientation [15,31,44,45,46], object agnosia e.g., [25], and reading problems/dyslexia [21,23,32,47,48,49,50,51]. As a novelty, we included dyscalculia, a developmental deficit in number processing, as this is commonly associated with dyslexia [52,53,54]. Along the same vein, we included ADHD, which is reported in one DP case study [55], and which also tends to co-occur with dyslexia [29,30]. Face processing deficits have also been indicated in the neurodevelopmental condition synesthesia, which is characterized by a merging of senses such as seeing letters in colors [56,57]. Synesthesia is an atypical developmental condition, but not a disorder per se as it is not associated with deficits in or loss of function. Rather, it has been suggested to be related to creativity [58] and enhanced memory [59]. Lastly, memory problems in a developmental variant were found relevant to investigate for two reasons: First, because DP in many cases is found to reflect problems with face memory [60,61]. Secondly, because of observations of aphantasia, a deficit in voluntary visual mental imagery [45,62,63] in DP. Aphantasia has been associated with severely deficient autobiographical memory [64], which is a deficit in re-experiencing personal events [65]. This raises the question as to whether severely deficient autobiographical memory could also be linked to DP.
In sum, and in alphabetical order, aphantasia, attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder, dyscalculia, dyslexia, memory problems, navigation problems, object agnosia, and synesthesia were considered relevant conditions to investigate in DP. For practical reasons, they are collectively referred to here as developmental comorbidity.

2.2. Participants

This cross-sectional survey study used convenience sampling. In total, 148 respondents filled out the Difficulties and Abilities in Developmental Prosopagnosia Questionnaire (described in Section 2.4). Exclusion criteria were (1) incomplete responses (n = 13); (2) reports of neurological disorder or brain injury (n = 4); (3) not reporting normal vision (n = 4); and (4) neurodevelopmental or psychiatric disorders that can cause impairment in intellectual or social functioning (n = 12), e.g., ASD [8,34] and schizophrenia [66,67]. Overall, 115 participants with s-DP were included. Participants were encouraged to participate if they experienced difficulties recognizing familiar faces in their everyday life and thus were suspected to suffer from DP. Thirty-four (29.6%) participants also reported previously being diagnosed with DP by a health professional or researcher. The participants were represented broadly in all age intervals from 18–24 years to 75 years or older (Table 1). Furthermore, 33% of the participants fell within the median interval of 35–44 years of age. All included participants reported normal or corrected-to-normal vision. Participants were not asked to indicate their gender for data protection reasons. Demographic data are reported in Table 2.

2.3. Data Collection

The survey was distributed online using the software SurveyXact By Ramboll [68]. The participants were recruited via the social networking platform Facebook, where the survey was posted in three groups (two international and one Norwegian group) with relevance to prosopagnosia. The survey was written in English and was available for six weeks.
Informed consent was obtained from all participants prior to their participation, and they were informed that all collected data would be anonymous. The study was conducted in adherence to the Declaration of Helsinki [69], and received ethical approval by the Institutional Ethical Review Board, University of Copenhagen, Department of Psychology (Project ID: IP-IRB/02042020).

2.4. Measures

2.4.1. The Difficulties and Abilities in Developmental Prosopagnosia Questionnaire

To measure indications of potential developmental comorbidity and abilities in s-DP, we constructed the Difficulties and Abilities in Developmental Prosopagnosia Questionnaire. It comprised items from published questionnaires aimed at adult participants, as well as items specifically formulated for this study (see Section 2.4.2, Section 2.4.3, Section 2.4.4, Section 2.4.5 and Section 2.4.6). The full questionnaire is available in the Supplementary Material. In addition to the items from formal questionnaires, two items asked directly about specific other diagnoses or comorbidity with categorical response options (yes/no), and a free-text field for adding other diagnoses. The first of these items concerns primarily well-known developmental disorders of interest: ‘Do you have other diagnoses? Particularly related to vision, mental illness, learning difficulties, sensory disorders, developmental disorders, motor disorders, etc.’ with the response options ADHD, ASD, dyslexia, dyscalculia, and other (free text). The second item was assessing self-reports of other, perhaps less conventional conditions of interest: ‘Do you have any of the following conditions related to cognitive strengths or difficulties?’ with the response options aphantasia, synesthesia, object agnosia, memory problems, and other (free text). Finally, our questionnaire included two open-ended questions regarding cognitive strengths and difficulties, respectively, to allow participants to describe this in their own words (see Section 2.4.6).
It takes approximately 15–20 min to fill out the Difficulties and Abilities in Developmental Prosopagnosia Questionnaire, and it was kept short due to the online distribution format [70].

2.4.2. The 20-Item Prosopagnosia Index (PI20)

To assess self-reported face recognition in the sample of s-DP, the full PI20 was included in the Difficulties and Abilities in Developmental Prosopagnosia Questionnaire. The PI20 is a widely used 20-item self-report measure of face recognition developed to identify and assess severity of prosopagnosia, [71]. The PI20 uses a 5-point Likert-type scale (1 = strongly agree to 5 = strongly disagree) to score agreement with statements about face recognition experiences, e.g., ‘My face recognition ability is worse than most people’ and ‘At family gatherings, I sometimes confuse individual family members’ [71] (p. 4). The psychometric properties of the PI20 are good: In the original validation study by Shah et al. [71] it was shown to have very high internal consistency (Chronbach’s α = 0.96), and good convergent validity as it correlates well with objective measures of face recognition, e.g., CFMT (r = −0.68, p < 0.0001). The maximum score is 100, and higher scores indicate more difficulty recognizing faces. Scores of 65–74, 75–84, and 85–100 can broadly indicate mild, moderate, and severe DP, respectively [71].

2.4.3. Items from the Wayfinding Questionnaire

To investigate navigation abilities, the Difficulties and Abilities in Developmental Prosopagnosia Questionnaire included five items from the Wayfinding Questionnaire [72,73], which uses a 7-point Likert-type scale (1 = Not at all applicable to me, 2 = Almost never applicable to me, 3 = Rarely applicable to me, 4 = Sometimes applicable to me, 5 = Sometimes applicable to me, 6 = Almost always applicable to me, and 7 = Fully applicable to me.). Higher scores indicate better wayfinding ability. The Wayfinding Questionnaire was originally developed for screening for navigation-related complaints in stroke patients and was applied here as a substantial number of individuals with DP have voiced similar complaints [44,45]. De Rooij and colleagues [73] (p. 1051) define a score of ≤3 on an item as indicating clinically relevant complaints. The Wayfinding Questionnaire has been validated in both healthy participants (N = 356) [72] and stroke patients (n = 78) [73]. The stroke patients with low scores on the Wayfinding Questionnaire performed worse on objective measures of navigation ability in a virtual reality setting, suggesting good concurrent validity [73,74]. Four of the five chosen items have been applied previously by Rice et al. [75], and the fifth item was included as it concerned indoor navigation. The five items all had satisfactory loadings on the Wayfinding Questionnaire subscale ‘Navigation and Orientation’ (0.631–0.841) [72].

2.4.4. Items from the Adult Reading History Questionnaire

The Adult Reading History Questionnaire (revised) is a 23-item self-report questionnaire, one of the few designed to identify a history of reading difficulties indicating dyslexia in adults [76]. As mentioned, face recognition difficulties have been identified in dyslexia [32,48,49,50], and a common etiology for dyslexia and DP has been suggested [28,77]. Even though normal reading skills have been demonstrated in DP [21,22,23,78], the relationship between the disorders is not fully understood, and hence the Adult Reading History Questionnaire was included in the current study. The Adult Reading History Questionnaire has good psychometric properties [76,79], including good internal consistency (Chronbach’s α = 0.94 and 0.92), test-retest reliability (r = 0.87 and 0.84), and construct validity (r = 0.57–70 between the Adult Reading History Questionnaire and reading measures) [76]. Furthermore, the Adult Reading History Questionnaire has predicted the occurrence of dyslexia reasonably well [76].
Responses to the Adult Reading History Questionnaire are given on a 5-point Likert-scale, with higher scores corresponding to more difficulty. The original range is 0–4, but in the current study it was adapted to 1–5 to provide consistency amongst the majority of the scales [70]. The four items with the highest loadings (0.920–0.794) on the subscale ‘Dyslexia Symptoms’ were chosen to be included in our questionnaire [79]. These four items were concerned with literacy skills in elementary school, and a fifth item was included to represent current reading ability. As we only used a subset of the items from this questionnaire, the psychometric properties of the included items are unknown.

2.4.5. Items Concerning Mathematical Ability

Three items were constructed as no self-report measures of mathematical ability and dyscalculia for adults exist [80,81]. Schelke et al. [80] suggested the math subscale from the Colorado Learning Difficulty Questionnaire [82], originally a parent-report questionnaire, as a candidate for investigating dyscalculia retrospectively in adults. Two of these items were rephrased to target an adult population. One item (‘Are/were your child worse at math than at reading and spelling?’) was discarded as its content was misleading due to the high co-occurrence between dyscalculia and dyslexia [83]. Instead, a third item was formulated concerning estimation of quantities (subitizing), which is central to dyscalculia [84,85]. These three items were graded with a 5-point Likert-type scale (1 = strongly agree to 5 = strongly disagree).

2.4.6. Open-Ended Questions Regarding Difficulties and Abilities

The Difficulties and Abilities in Developmental Prosopagnosia Questionnaire also contains two guided open-ended questions aimed at a less constrained and suggestive assessment of the abilities and difficulties experienced by participants with s-DP in their own words. Aligned with the explorative nature of our questionnaire, this provides an opportunity for non-expected findings or responses [86]. The questions were phrased ‘Are there things that you are particularly good at, where you experience performing better than others?’ and ‘Are there things or situations, where you experience performing worse than others?’. The questions were provided with a guiding text, e.g., ‘Please consider and elaborate on any difficulties you might have, for example within the areas of math, memory, navigation, visualizing images or objects, literacy, sports or tone deafness’.

3. Data Analysis

3.1. Statistical Analysis

The statistical analyses were conducted using IBM SPSS, version 27.0 [87] with a significance level of p ≤ 0.05.

3.1.1. The Effect of Prosopagnosia Severity on the Presence of Developmental Comorbidity

A Chi-square test of independence was used to analyze whether the presence of any binary-measured developmental comorbidity (ADHD, aphantasia, dyscalculia, dyslexia, memory problems, object agnosia, or synesthesia) was independent of severity of self-reported face recognition impairment (PI20 total scores) within the sample of participants with s-DP [88,89]. The guiding criteria from the PI20 was used as cutoffs for dividing the severity groups into mild DP with total scores 65–74, moderate DP with total scores 75–84, and severe DP with total scores 85–100 [71].

3.1.2. Exploratory Factor Analysis of the Difficulties and Abilities in Developmental Prosopagnosia Questionnaire

An exploratory factor analysis was performed to investigate the latent variables underlying the responses of the sample of individuals with s-DP (N = 115) to the PI20, the Wayfinding Questionnaire, the Adult Reading History Questionnaire, and the mathematical ability items. In other words, the interest is the underlying structure and relationship between self-reported face recognition ability and navigation, reading/spelling, and mathematical ability. A reasonable assumption could be that a four-factor structure would emerge. It has been demonstrated that the PI20 has one factor [71], and the Wayfinding Questionnaire, the Adult Reading History Questionnaire, and the math items would be expected to represent different abilities, and hence different factors. The comorbidity patterns described above [31,32,52], e.g., co-occurring developmental reading and mathematical difficulties, could allow for speculations of correlation between these measures, potentially resulting in three factors. However, in both cases the knowledge is still too limited to allow for precise predictions of the factor outcome, and thus an exploratory approach was taken.
Fabrigar et al. [90] argue that principal axis factoring is the most adequate choice of factor extraction method for exploratory factor analysis when data are not normally distributed. This is typically the case for ordinal data obtained using a Likert-type scale [91], which also applies to the data in the current sample when considering general guidelines for determining the distribution of the data [92] (p. 61).
Rotation in exploratory factor analysis is performed to simplify and clarify data structure as well as improving the interpretation of the factors. An oblique type of rotation (direct oblimin in SPSS) was chosen to allow the factors to correlate [93]. The number of factors to retain was based on the Kaiser criterion, stating that factors with an eigenvalue of more than 1.0 should be retained and checked for agreement with the scree test [93] and qualitative interpretation to find the most meaningful number of factors to extract. Kaiser–Mayer–Olkin measure and Bartlett’s sphericity were applied as measures of the factorability of the dataset, and 0.6 was used as cutoff for an acceptable sampling adequacy [94] (p. 668). Factor loadings of above 0.32 or below −0.32 were regarded as reflecting interpretable and meaningful relationships between an item and a factor, as a factor loading of this size corresponds to approximately 10% of the overlapping variance [94].

3.2. Qualitative Analysis

Directed content analysis, a deductive approach, was applied to the responses of the open-ended questions to investigate the frequency and variance in themes regarding abilities and difficulties, and thus provide a descriptive summary of what was reported [42]. A mainly deductive qualitative approach was found appropriate considering the questions and the more quantitative framework of the whole Difficulties and Abilities in Developmental Prosopagnosia Questionnaire, which the open-ended questions were a part of.
Initial coding categories and operational definitions were developed for the statements concerning difficulties based on the developmental comorbidities (except for synesthesia, which is not considered a disorder as there is no loss of function) [95]. First version codes were based on the examples provided in the guiding text, e.g., ‘(…) mathematical abilities, memory, navigation, visualizing images or objects, literacy, sports or musical talents’. When appropriate and reflected in the data, the first version codes were created in pairs reflecting a strength and a difficulty, e.g., Good Memory vs. Memory Problems. N.S. (first author) coded the first version codes and recorded meaningful information provided about experienced abilities and difficulties that was not captured by these first version codes. Secondly, new categories, e.g., Skills In Visual Arts, were created in an inductive fashion to fit the data that could not be coded with existing codes but still seemed to reflect meaningful themes in the data [95]. Thus, when a meaningful amount of description of either difficulty or ability was present in the data and was not captured by the first version codes, a new code category was titled and described in the coding manual. Meaningful in this regard entails number of descriptions, richness of description, magnitude of described impact on daily life, and potential relatedness to the cluster of developmental comorbidities.
Recoding and revisiting the coding categories was repeated as a part of an iterative process. A final coding manual was written and discussed with a second independent coder (S.B.J., a student assistant) and revised accordingly (the coding manual is available at OSF, see Supplementary Material). During the second coder’s coding, doubts and disagreements were discussed continuously between the two raters, and necessary revisions to the codes and the coding manual were made to ensure objectivity and credibility [42]. The interrater reliability between N.S. and S.B.J. was 0.84, measured as a weighted Kappa coefficient. R.S. (second author) served as an expert coder and decided between significant disagreements in the coding between the two initial coders. For minor discrepancies in the coding, N.S. was considered the main coder, unless S.B.J. and N.S. agreed that S.B.J.’s coding adhered more closely to the coding manual for the particular discrepancy. Data were stored and coded using NVivo software, which was also used for the calculation of the Kappa coefficient [96].

4. Results

The 115 participants’ responses to the Difficulties and Abilities in Developmental Prosopagnosia Questionnaire showed that they generally experienced problems with face recognition in their everyday life. The average total PI20 score was 83.8 (SD = 8.5), which is close to the suggested cutoff for severe DP, which is 85 [71]. For the severity groups based on the PI20 [71], 15 participants scored within the mild DP group, 42 participants scored within the moderate DP group, and 58 scored within the severe DP group. Two individuals had PI20 total scores below the mild cutoff of 65 and were included in the group with the lowest scores, thus creating a group for mild s-DP and individuals with self-reported face recognition difficulties scoring under the mild cutoff. The total scores of these individuals were 61 and 48, respectively. For the items from other scales, the median (range) was as follows: Wayfinding Questionnaire: 19 (30); Adult Reading History Questionnaire: 6 (17); and mathematical ability items: 6 (12).

4.1. Amount of Self-Reported Developmental Comorbidity

The descriptive results for self-reported developmental comorbidity are reported in Table 3 and Table 4.

4.2. Prosopagnosia Severity and Developmental Comorbidity

The Chi-square test of independence did not reveal a significant relationship between presence of developmental comorbidity (navigation problems were not included in this analysis) and severity of self-reported face recognition impairment (PI20 total scores) within the sample p = 0.79 . We report this while being aware that p-values should be interpreted with great caution in explorative studies [40]. For the severity groups, the cases of developmental comorbidity were distributed as outlined in Table 5.
This suggests that the presence of developmental comorbidity is independent of severity of self-reported face recognition impairment [71] in this sample.

4.3. Factor Structure of the Difficulties and Abilities in Developmental Prosopagnosia Questionnaire

An exploratory factor analysis extracting factors with principal axis factoring, using an oblique rotation, was applied to assess the underlying structure of the latent variables explaining the covariance amongst the responses to our questionnaire from the 115 included participants with s-DP. The data demonstrated good factorability: The Kaiser–Mayer–Olkin measure of sampling adequacy was 0.696 and above the accepted value of 0.6 [94] (p. 668). Additionally, all items except for four (0.457–0.476) met the cutoff of above 0.50 for individual measures of sampling adequacy when inspecting the diagonal element of the anti-image correlation matrix in the preliminary analysis of the data’s fit for exploratory factor analysis [97]. As it was close to the cutoff, the items were retained. Bartlett’s test of sphericity was significant χ 2   528   = 1757.6 ,   p < 0.001 . At first, solutions with up to 10 factors emerged with initial eigenvalues >1, together explaining 55.9% of the extracted variance. Investigating the scree plot (Figure 1) suggested retaining five factors due to the elbow on the curve [93,98].
However, the theoretical a priori factor structure was assumed to have four factors or less if the factors were correlated, and the five-factor structure was not considered qualitatively meaningful and less stable than the four-factor structure when assessed (the five-factor solution is available at the OSF site associated with the project). Costello and Osborne [93] (p. 3) argue that interpretability and stability of the factor structure are also important to consider when analyzing the number of factors to extract. This was thus judged by comparing the item loading tables (pattern matrix in SPSS), which favored a four-factor structure due to interpretability, as the five-factor solution divided the PI20 into two factors with cross-loading items, which is diverging with previous validation studies using exploratory factor analysis reporting a single factor structure of the PI20 [71,99]. Thus, four factors were extracted, and they accounted for 39.9% of the variance. The factor loadings of the items in our questionnaire on the four-factor structure are displayed in Table 6.
The first factor (Developmental Prosopagnosia Symptoms) consisted of 14 items loading above 0.32 on the factor, assessing different aspects of impaired face recognition, such as negative feelings associated with not recognizing someone (e.g., Item 11 and 16 from the PI20), not recognizing familiar people (e.g., Item 4 and 20 from the PI20), and coping strategies (e.g., Item 7 and 10 from the PI20). All five navigation items had strong loadings on the second factor (Navigation and Orientation). The five items assessing reading and spelling ability, primarily in elementary school, collectively loaded on the third factor (History of Reading/Spelling Difficulty). The three items concerning mathematical ability in childhood (e.g., mathematical ability Item 1 and 2) and current difficulties assessing quantity (e.g., mathematical ability Item 3) loaded strongly on the fourth factor (History of Difficulties with Math).
Additionally, Item 3 and 12 from the PI20 loaded on Factor 3, and Item 19 from the PI20 loaded on the fourth factor, which did not correspond with the content of the rest of the items within these factors. However, these loadings were quite small, e.g., just around −0.32. Items 2, 8, and 9 from the PI20 did not load above 0.32 or below −0.32 on any factor.
All factors had at minimum three strong factor loadings (above 0.5 or below -0.5), and all factors consisted of more than three items, suggesting a stable solution [93]. Factor 1 and 3 had a weak correlation of r = 0.168 , suggesting that an oblique rotation was appropriate [97]. Additionally, the residuals indicated a good fit with 35% non-redundant residuals (e.g., less than 50% of residuals had values greater than 0.05 in the Reproduced Correlation Matrix) [97].

4.4. Qualitative Categories from the Open-Ended Questions

Applying directed content analysis to the open-ended questions in the DAP-Q allowed for more detailed analysis of self-reports of the strengths and difficulties experienced by the participants. The coding categories representing the difficulties and strengths are presented in Table 7, with coding frequencies and a quote serving as an example of the category. Table 7 provides an overview of the diversity within the participants’ qualitative reports of their strengths and difficulties. A range of categories reflects domains where both impairments and strengths are reported (by different individuals). This applies to visual imagery, mathematical abilities, reading/spelling, memory, musicality, sports skills, and navigation, where, interestingly, some participants reported severe problems while others reported excelling. For some domains, e.g., imagery, reading/spelling, and navigation, more participants reported either strengths or difficulties, while for other domains (e.g., math, musicality) representations of strengths and difficulties were more evenly distributed. For some categories, only impairment (object recognition, motor coordination, inattention), or abilities/strengths (synesthesia, social interaction, visual arts) were represented in the qualitative categories.

5. Discussion

5.1. Discussion of Results

5.1.1. Summary of Results

This study was designed to investigate which types of developmental comorbidity, as well as subjective difficulties and strengths, are reported by participants with s-DP. First, most participants in the study reported face recognition problems on the PI20 compatible with prosopagnosia, although we note again that we did not ascertain this in any other way. Two participants scored lower than the suggested range for DP, but we decided to keep their responses in the analysis as they did self-report significant problems with faces. Fifty-eight participants scored within the most severe group, as defined by Shah et al. [71].
More than half of the sample reported the presence of at least one of the predefined conditions of interest (aphantasia, ADHD, dyscalculia, dyslexia, memory problems, object agnosia, and synesthesia). Relatively few participants reported dyslexia, dyscalculia, and ADHD. Synesthesia was reported by 10.4%. Aphantasia was reported by ~20%, and memory problems and object agnosia were reported by almost a fourth of the sample of individuals with s-DP. In total, 25% reported indications of clinically relevant navigation complaints on all of the selected Wayfinding Questionnaire items. In addition, the presence of developmental comorbidity was observed to be independent of severity of s-DP (PI20 total score) when the participants were divided into a mild, moderate, or severe group.
Finally, an exploratory factor analysis revealed a stable four-factor structure corresponding to the independent scales included, with a very weak correlation between the factors of Developmental Prosopagnosia Symptoms and History of Reading/Spelling Difficulty. The stability of the factor structure serves as an indication of validity of the composition of items, allowing for interpretation and analysis of the data from our questionnaire.
When asked to describe their difficulties and strengths, e.g., in which domains they experience performing better or worse relative to others, a considerable number of participants report difficulties in the categories Navigation Problems, Difficulties with Math, Aphantasia, Memory Problems, and Difficulties with Sports. For strengths, the statements most frequently match the coding categories Good Memory, Reading and Spelling Abilities, Musicality, and Mathematical Abilities. For Aphantasia and Navigation Problems, more participants reported impairment, while comparatively fewer reported performing well in these domains. For Reading and Spelling and Memory, more participants with s-DP reported it as a strength rather than a difficulty, even though many also described memory problems. For skills within math, musicality, and sports, there were a number of reports of both strengths and difficulties.

5.1.2. Discussion of Quantitative Results

The exploratory factor analysis showed a clear and stable four-factor structure of the responses to our questionnaire, largely separating the different (dis)abilities in this sample of s-DP into self-reports of face recognition ability (PI20), navigation (the Wayfinding Questionnaire), reading (the Adult Reading History Questionnaire), and mathematical ability.
However, the factor structure also revealed some interesting aspects of the PI20, where some items did not load on the factor of Developmental Prosopagnosia Symptoms (the first factor) (Item 3, 12, and 19), but instead on the third or the fourth factor, and some items did not load sufficiently on any factor (Items 2, 8, and 9) [93]. Common aspects of these poorly performing PI20 items were either being positively worded (Items 3, 8, 9, and 19) or potentially not distinguishing adequately between the non-DP and DP. Exemplifying the latter, Item 3 seemed to reflect a statement that is characteristic to normal face recognition as well, e.g., typical individuals would also be expected to answer that it is easier to recognize people with distinctive facial features. Thus, although the original validation study of the PI20 revealed a strong single factor [71], both when including controls and DP-suspects and analyzing DPs in isolation, our results indicate that some items could require re-evaluation.
Furthermore, Factor 1 (Developmental Prosopagnosia Symptoms) and Factor 3 (History of Reading/Spelling Difficulty) correlated weakly (r = −0.168), suggesting a slight association between reading and face recognition ability in this sample. On one hand, it could simply be due to the PI20 Items 3 and 12 which loaded weakly on the History of Reading/Spelling Difficulty factor. On the other hand, individuals with dyslexia can have impaired face processing, e.g., [49], and perhaps some participants with both were included in the present sample.
Regarding the fourth factor, History of Difficulties with Math, it is worth noting that these items showed a strong factor with convincing factor loadings (0.653–816), even though that there were only three items in total [93]. These were the items we were least sure of when constructing the scale, but the factor analysis seems to indicate that these items might be useful in a future attempt to construct a self-report scale for adult dyscalculia.

5.1.3. Comparing the Quantitative and Qualitative Results

Both the quantitative and the qualitative analyses provided insights into the difficulties and developmental comorbidities that the participants experienced, and the qualitative data further allowed us to understand their self-reported strengths as well. Hence, we will discuss the quantitative and qualitative reports of difficulties and strengths in s-DP in relation to each other and the available literature. To provide a structure for this section, we divided it into cognitive domains/disorders which are presented in alphabetical order.
Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder. Seven (6.1%) s-DP participants reported ADHD as a comorbid disorder, which is within the range of self-report of ADHD in college students of between 2–8% [100]. Within the qualitative data there were only two descriptions matching Problems with Inattentiveness and Impulsivity or Hyperactivity. ADHD-like symptoms are generally not mentioned in the literature on DP, e.g., in these overviews [25,45], and to our knowledge there is only one published report of comorbid DP and ADHD [55]. There is, however, high comorbidity between ADHD and reading difficulties [29,30], and as mentioned, dyslexia can present with face processing deficits e.g., [49], which may perhaps be part of the explanation for why a few participants in this sample of s-DP reported ADHD.
Mathematical Abilities or Difficulties. Both mathematical ability and difficulties with math were reported at fairly high levels relative to the rest of the qualitative codes. However, only four (3.5%) participants reported dyscalculia in the close-ended questions (see Table 4). To our knowledge, no prevalence studies of adults exist, but the impairment can continue into adulthood [52]. Generally, the struggles described in the qualitative data seemed to revolve around mental math and basic arithmetic operations—or simply feeling ‘below average’. The mathematical skills are broadly or very simply described, but some participants mention academic achievements. The discrepancy between the qualitative and quantitative data could suggest that the difficulties with math that some experience might still fall within a normal range, or affect abilities not typically considered as dyscalculia. We are unaware of any investigations of face processing and dyscalculia, and generally self-reported dyscalculia in adults has received little research attention [80], hence the literature is sparse. This may partly be because it is difficult, if not impossible, to identify dyscalculia without rigorous testing, as there is no current self-report measure available for adults [80,81].
Memory and Aphantasia. Lack of mental imagery, which we here interpret as possibly reflecting Aphantasia, was reported frequently both in the qualitative and quantitative data, by 24 participants (20.9%) in both cases. Interestingly, Good Visual Mental Imagery was also reported, although not as frequently (n = 7).
Given the suggested link between aphantasia and severely deficient autobiographical memory, memory deficits were also of interest [64,101]. Memory problems were reported by 27 (23.5%), hence by almost a fourth of the participants, and interestingly, a co-occurrence of self-reported aphantasia and memory problems was seen in 11 (9.6%) participants. This is in line with another survey study, where participants (N = 2000) with self-reported aphantasia reported more difficulties with face recognition and autobiographical memory relative to 200 participants with vivid visual imagery [102].
Memory problems can be considered a quite broad and non-specific term, and it was deliberately formulated in lay language in this survey study and guided with the description as remembering things, details, or personal past events’. This was done with the aim of eliciting responses related to various forms of memory. It is notable in the qualitative data that both Good Memory and Memory Problems were amongst the most frequently used codes. The statements indicate that some participants, but not the majority, described problems related to autobiographic memory: ‘remembering names, holidays, places we have been, events I have been to, films I have seen’. At the same time, many also reported good semantic memory or good memory for pieces of music, e.g.: ‘I am good at remembering historical facts (people, events, dates) and other random facts; taking multiple-choice tests’. Thus, the memory components the participants feel skilled at might not be the same ones that cause trouble to others.
Musicality. Both good Musical Ability (n = 26) and Lack of Musicality (n = 17) were reported by participants. For the former, ability to play an instrument and play by ear were often mentioned. For the latter, tone deafness and difficulty singing in tune were often reported, which seems to fit with reports of tone deafness in some cases of DP [103] and impaired pitch discrimination in others [37]. This could also support the argument that face recognition can co-occur with deficits in other modalities.
Navigation Problems or Abilities. Navigation problems (n = 41) was the most frequently represented category in the qualitative data. Not many reported good navigation abilities. Impaired navigation ability is not an official diagnosis, but is occasionally referred to as (developmental) topographical disorientation [104]. In the current sample, 29 participants with s-DP (25.2%) scored ≤3 on all five the Wayfinding Questionnaire items, which has been suggested to indicate clinically relevant complaints about navigation ability [73].
A few studies have investigated topographical orientation and navigation ability in DP, and there are numerous anecdotal reports of co-occurrence [44]. Existing studies have diverging conclusions: The current observations align with one study which concludes that deficits in navigation, particularly topographical memory, tend to co-occur with DP (N = 9) [31]. Another study finds that topographical deficits (cognitive maps formation) are less frequent in DP (N = 7) [46]. As opposed to the current study, these previous findings stem from formal testing of different topographical skills implicated in navigation, but the sample sizes are rather small [25]. Interestingly, Bate, Adams, Bennetts, and Line [15] found deficits in retrieval and formation of cognitive maps in a DP case and point out that 6/7 of the sample in the study by Corrow and colleagues [46] did not self-report navigation problems in an initial interview. This of course highlights a recurring issue in DP research (and in general) in that self-report and objective measures do not always coincide.
Object Agnosia. The relationship between face and object recognition has perhaps been the most central debate in DP [25], as it concerns the very nature of the architecture of the visual recognition system: whether faces and objects are processed by distinct (domain-specific) or common systems (domain-general) [105]. Twenty-nine participants (25.2%), a fourth of this sample, reported object agnosia as a part of the close-ended items. This, however, was not reflected in the qualitative data. Findings regarding prevalence and severity of deficits in object recognition in DP are mixed, which also applies to whether or not a clear dissociation exists. Regardless, it is clear that DP can occur both with and without self-reported impairments in non-face object recognition [19,20,24,106,107,108,109,110,111,112]. As the current findings are based on self-reported deficits in object recognition, they do not advance the debate further, as studies with formal assessments of face and object recognition (for a review of DP cases up to 2016 see Geskin and Behrman [25]), as well as neuroimaging studies [113,114], still fail to provide consistent results. It does, however, seem safe to conclude that deficits in object recognition are present in at least some cases of DP, as is indicated in this sample as well as in the literature.
Reading and Spelling. The participants generally reported more instances of good reading and spelling ability, and few instances of the opposite. The abilities within reading/spelling are many, e.g., flair for languages, literacy, and learning to read from an early age. Few participants reported difficulties with reading or dyslexia on the direct question (3.5% compared with prevalence of childhood dyslexia of 5–10% [115,116,117]), while a few more (7%) reported issues with reading or spelling in the open-ended questions. This aligns with research demonstrating a double dissociation between face recognition and reading abilities [21,22,23,51], although dyslexia and face recognition deficits can also co-occur [32,47,48,49,50]. This suggests that self-reported dyslexia is not more likely to be present with s-DP than in general, nor do the scores on the selected the Adult Reading History Questionnaire items indicate reading difficulties in the sample (Table 3).
Social and Interpersonal Skills. Most descriptions of inadequacy in social and interpersonal skills were directly related to face recognition problems, and thus did not receive separate coding in the qualitative analysis. There were, however, some interesting descriptions of social strengths too, e.g.: ‘Knowing what other people feel, and why they feel it. ‘intuitive profiling’ so to say’. Some examples also refer to the ability to read emotions and show empathy. While we know that DP can be accompanied by anxiety and avoidance of social gatherings [3], our results may suggest that further investigations of social coping and explorations of when individuals with DP feel socially capable might prove interesting as well.
Sports Difficulties/Skills and Motor Coordination and Body–Space Perception. There were a number of reports of Difficulties with Sport (n = 17), but also of good Sports Skills (n = 12). Sports Skills were often described with an example of excelling in one particular discipline. For Difficulties with Sport, the statements often reflected a broader lack of skill important for many sports. Some statements overlap with experiences coded as Motor Coordination and Body–Space Perception, and are coded with both categories e.g., ‘I have a hard time with things that require hand-eye coordination like playing sports or physical games’. Moreover, besides being clumsy, there are elements of having a hard time judging the distance between oneself and other objects in Motor Coordination and Body–Space Perception. One particularly interesting example is:
‘(…) At work we have to make sure we do not walk within 5 m of an operational machine. I cannot imagine how far that is. I need to memorize the spots where the machines are often positioned, and memorise where I can walk based on where others have walked safely. I will not pass a machine in an unfamiliar spot without instruction from driver or my manager to do so because I can’t tell if I’m at a safe distance.’
As far as we are aware, there is no existing literature on difficulties with motor coordination or body–space perception related to DP, and no investigations of the possible relationship between DP and developmental coordination disorder.
Skills in Visual Arts. Some of the participants described talents within primarily drawing and painting. A few described themselves as established artists or/and receivers of awards for their work. For example:
‘I am very artistic. I have shown my work and been featured in a variety of mediums throughout my whole life. Most specifically I paint faces and have since I was about 12 years old and was fascinated with faces.’
To our knowledge, there are no studies regarding the artistic ability of participants with DP, although there are examples in popular media.
Synesthesia. Twelve (10.4%) participants with s-DP reported synesthesia. There are few published estimates of synesthesia prevalence in the adult population, and these vary widely, but one well-controlled study reported a prevalence of 1.1–4.4%. [118]. Investigations of synesthesia involve some measure of self-report, but Simner and colleagues’ [118] measures were much more sophisticated than in the present study, potentially limiting the comparability.
The quantitative reports do not align with the qualitative analysis, where only two descriptions of synesthesia are represented. This might be caused by the fact that synesthetic experiences might not instantly be categorized as difficulties or strengths, and thus are not reported in the open-ended questions. Interestingly, it has been shown that some individuals with grapheme-color synesthesia have abnormal face processing measured with, e.g., The Cambridge Face Perception Test (N = 3) [57] and CFMT (N = 16) [56]. It has been suggested that synesthesia develops in response to complex categorical learning challenges as a perceptual aid [119] or incomplete synaptic pruning during development [120]. Ulimoen and Sørensen [56] hypothesize that in the case of synesthesia, atypical categorization processes are not limited to the inducer stimulus, e.g., a letter inducing color, but might apply to other categories as well, e.g., faces, which could serve as a preliminary interpretation for the current reports.

5.2. General Discussion

5.2.1. High Rates of Self-Reported Developmental Comorbidity in s-DP

The amount of developmental comorbidity reported by the sample was quite astonishing, as 65 participants (56.5%) reported suffering from at least one of the following: ADHD, aphantasia, dyscalculia, dyslexia, memory problems, object agnosia, or/and synesthesia. In addition, a large number of participants reported navigational problems on the questionnaire items. The qualitative data revealed that a large number of participants experienced problems in specific domains, in particular with imagery, math, memory, and navigation. However, for all these domains, there were also reports of good abilities—indicating that the link between any of these domains and DP might not be entirely systematic (i.e., these functions might be both associated with and dissociated from DP). To our knowledge, this study is the first to address the possible overlap between DP and other neurodevelopmental disorders and impairments on such a large scale. Our findings seem to suggest a curious link between at least some of the developmental comorbidities [16,28,77]. This could perhaps be interpreted as indicating that DP might be a more disabling disorder than previously thought [15], and/or potentially also less domain-specific and not selective to deficits in facial processing. Another way of interpreting the current findings is that face recognition impairments might be an important node in a developmental co-morbidity space and should be investigated further in other developmental disorders. Even if DP exists in a pure form, the current results suggest that face recognition might be affected or impaired in a range of developmental disorders.
An additional explanation for the high rates of comorbidity observed could be that developmental comorbidity has not been in focus in the literature on DP, and it has not been customary to screen for or ask DP participants about other disorders besides ASD and brain injury [1,11]. Many studies, for instance, do not screen for or exclude participants with dyslexia, and even fewer include observations regarding the other impairments and disorders considered here.

5.2.2. Limitations

While the strength of the present study is in the exploratory and broad approach to the investigation of developmental comorbidity in self-reported DP in a fairly large sample, there are of course also limitations. The main limitation is the lack of a control group without s-DP, which leaves unknown what the frequencies of reports would be in the general population. In addition, all our results are based on self-reports, and while some of our questions are taken from published and validated instruments, we also included yes/no questions regarding the presence of a given condition without the use of validated self-reports or clinical judgement. Thus, our results should be viewed only as a first step in the investigation of possible developmental comorbidity [121,122].
While the PI20 can indicate whether people have trouble with face recognition, it cannot stand alone in determining whether someone is suffering from DP. It is debated within the field how valid self-report instruments like the PI20 are in measuring actual face recognition problems, and relatedly, how much insight people actually have into their facial recognition performance. It has been shown that people in general have only modest insight into their own performance on objective measures of face recognition abilities [9,121,123], but that DPs may have better insight into their objective performances in comparison [121]. Some researchers regard self-reports of face recognition as highly valid due to moderately high correlations (often ~0.35–0.55) between the PI20 (as well as other self-report measures of face recognition) and objective measures of face processing in typical individuals and prosopagnosics [71,124,125,126,127]. In contrast, Arizpe et al. [43] concluded that the PI20 is not sensitive enough to be used as a screening tool for DP. To be clear, the PI20 or other self-report instruments should not be used in isolation for diagnosing DP. Convergent objective testing of face recognition performance in addition to self-report measures is imperative, and on this there is increasing consensus in the field [11,43,127]. Thus, although we based this exploratory study on self-reported face recognition impairment using the PI20, we do not consider this sufficient for an actual diagnosis of DP.
Thus, it is possible that not all participants included in our study would qualify for a diagnosis of DP. Bate and colleagues [60] found that only 38.2% of a sample of 165 self-referred DPs met criteria for impairment on 2/3 tests of face processing. Hence, it seems reasonable to assume that the same could be true for the sample in question. On the other hand, it is also possible that some of the participants have another main disorder with accompanying DP or face recognition deficits, so that the amount of self-reported developmental comorbidity is not due to DP but other primary neurodevelopmental disorders [28] or developmental neural vulnerability [16,77,128]. There is also an important caveat in interpreting the frequencies with which specific disorders or deficits were reported, as we did pre-specify some of these in the questionnaire. Thus, had we included other specific disorders, e.g., developmental coordination disorder or OCD, our findings would likely look different. The mentioning of specific disorders in the questionnaire could also prime the participants to consider impairments they would not have considered otherwise, e.g., aphantasia, which was mentioned specifically and was frequently reported both in the quantitative and qualitative data. This, however, does not detract from the main finding that a large proportion of the included s-DP reported indications of additional impairments and disorders.
Relatedly, recruiting from online fora might bias the sample towards people with particular interest in their suspected condition or in the research question, and perhaps also people with an interest in and experience of other diagnoses. We are not aware of any studies on bias in online recruitment in relation to prosopagnosia, but based on the general literature on self-selection and online recruitment [129,130] this is an important caveat to bear in mind.
In addition, the current sample size of 115, and the additional metrics from the exploratory factor analysis, lies in the lower end to satisfy the criteria of Maccallum et al. [131]. There are, however, differing views on the right sample size for this large sample procedure [132].
For the qualitative data, the aim of applying the open-ended questions was to elicit more detailed descriptions that expanded our understanding of the phenomena under investigation, however, not all responses were detailed. For example, some participants would describe their difficulties or abilities by listing them very briefly like this: ‘Memory, navigation, math’. As this was an online survey with unnamed participants, there was also no way we could ask clarifying questions in relation to statements that were arbitrary or lacked detail.

6. Conclusions

The observed high co-occurrence of developmental comorbidity in self-reported DP may have important implications for our understanding of neurodevelopmental disorders and conditions and their interplay. Particularly, high rates of self-reported synesthesia, navigational difficulties, aphantasia, and memory problems (particularly autobiographical memory) and diversity within subjective difficulties and strengths, e.g., math skills and motor coordination, suggests that there is ample room for future research on this topic. Investigating the relationship of DP to other developmental conditions in more detail, and whether face recognition deficits may exist in other neurodevelopmental disorders, might prove fruitful in characterizing the neurodevelopmental co-morbidity space or overlap between disorders more fully. While we have taken diagnostic categories and pre-defined cognitive domains as our starting point here, further work might go beyond the ‘core-deficit’ approach [133], and our findings suggest that face recognition may be an important aspect to assess when aiming to map transdiagnostic features of developmental disorders [133,134] in the future.

Supplementary Materials

The following are available online at https://www.mdpi.com/article/10.3390/brainsci12020230/s1. The following materials are openly available at https://osf.io/752pk/ (accessed on 14 December 2021): The Difficulties and Abilities in Developmental Prosopagnosia Questionnaire, the complete dataset, Table S4, the coding manual for the qualitative analysis, as well as the five-factor solution from the factor analysis.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, N.S. and R.S.; formal analysis, N.S.; investigation, N.S. and R.S.; data curation, N.S. and R.S.; writing—original draft preparation, N.S.; writing—review and editing, R.S.; visualization, N.S.; supervision, R.S.; project administration, R.S. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This work has received no funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted in adherence to the Declaration of Helsinki [69], and received ethical approval by the Institutional Ethical Review Board, University of Copenhagen, Department of Psychology (Project ID: IP-IRB/02042020).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all participants involved in the study, and they were informed that all collected data would be anonymous.

Data Availability Statement

The data presented in this study are openly available in an OSF-storage at https://osf.io/752pk/ (accessed on 14 December 2021).

Acknowledgments

We wish to thank Ro J. Robotham for feedback on the questionnaire items; Martin Stolpe Andersen and Stine Lykke Jørgensen for advice on the explorative factor analysis; Thomas Rune Nielsen for help with the qualitative analysis; and Sofie N. Binding Jakobsen for her assistance on the qualitative coding. The work was supported by the Friends of Fakutsi Association (FFA).

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Dalrymple, K.A.; Palermo, R. Guidelines for studying developmental prosopagnosia in adults and children. Wiley Interdiscip. Rev. Cogn. Sci. 2016, 7, 73–87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  2. Yardley, L.; McDermott, L.; Pisarski, S.; Duchaine, B.; Nakayama, K. Psychosocial consequences of developmental prosopagnosia: A problem of recognition. J. Psychosom. Res. 2008, 65, 445–451. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  3. Dalrymple, K.A.; Fletcher, K.; Corrow, S.; das Nair, R.; Barton, J.J.S.; Yonas, A.; Duchaine, B. ‘A room full of strangers every day’: The psychosocial impact of developmental prosopagnosia on children and their families. J. Psychosom. Res. 2014, 77, 144–150. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  4. Fine, D.R. We need a simple test for prosopagnosia. BMJ 2011, 342, 710. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Barton, J.J.S. Structure and function in acquired prosopagnosia: Lessons from a series of 10 patients with brain damage. J. Neuropsychol. 2008, 2, 197–225. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Starrfelt, R.; Barton, J.J.S. Prosopagnosia. In Encyclopedia of Behavioral Neuroscience, 2nd ed.; Della Sala, S., Ed.; Elsevier: Oxford, UK, 2022; pp. 597–604. [Google Scholar]
  7. Kress, T.; Daum, I. Developmental Prosopagnosia: A Review. Behav. Neurol. 2003, 14, 109–121. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  8. Bate, S.; Tree, J.J. The Definition and Diagnosis of Developmental Prosopagnosia. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 2017, 70, 193–200. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  9. Bowles, D.C.; McKone, E.; Dawel, A.; Duchaine, B.; Palermo, R.; Schmalzl, L.; Rivolta, D.; Wilson, C.E.; Yovel, G. Diagnosing prosopagnosia: Effects of ageing, sex, and participant–stimulus ethnic match on the Cambridge Face Memory Test and Cambridge Face Perception Test. Cogn. Neuropsychol. 2009, 26, 423–455. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Kennerknecht, I.; Grueter, T.; Welling, B.; Wentzek, S.; Horst, J.; Edwards, S.; Grueter, M. First report of prevalence of non-syndromic hereditary prosopagnosia (HPA). Am. J. Med. Genet. Part A 2006, 140A, 1617–1622. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Barton, J.J.S.; Corrow, S.L. The problem of being bad at faces. Neuropsychologia 2016, 89, 119–124. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  12. Fawcett, E.J.; Power, H.; Fawcett, J.M. Women Are at Greater Risk of OCD Than Men: A Meta-Analytic Review of OCD Prevalence Worldwide. J. Clin. Psychiatry 2020, 81, e1–e13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Ohnishi, T.; Kobayashi, H.; Yajima, T.; Koyama, T.; Noguchi, K. Psychiatric comorbidities in adult attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Prevalence and patterns in the routine clinical setting. Innov. Clin. Neurosci. 2019, 16, 11–16. [Google Scholar]
  14. Kessler, R.C.; Adler, L.; Barkley, R.; Biederman, J.; Conners, C.K.; Demler, O.; Faraone, S.V.; Greenhill, L.L.; Howes, M.J.; Secnik, K.; et al. The Prevalence and Correlates of Adult ADHD in the United States: Results From the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Am. J. Psychiatry 2006, 163, 716–723. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Bate, S.; Adams, A.; Bennetts, R.; Line, H. Developmental prosopagnosia with concurrent topographical difficulties: A case report and virtual reality training programme. Neuropsychol. Rehabil. 2019, 29, 1290–1312. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Gray, K.L.H.; Cook, R. Should developmental prosopagnosia, developmental body agnosia, and developmental object agnosia be considered independent neurodevelopmental conditions? Cogn. Neuropsychol. 2018, 35, 59–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Biotti, F.; Gray, K.L.H.; Cook, R. Is developmental prosopagnosia best characterised as an apperceptive or mnemonic condition? Neuropsychologia 2019, 124, 285–298. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Liu, X.; Li, X.; Song, Y.; Liu, J. Separate and shared neural basis of face memory and face perception in developmental prosopagnosia. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 2021, 15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  19. Duchaine, B.; Nakayama, K. Dissociations of Face and Object Recognition in Developmental Prosopagnosia. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2005, 17, 249–261. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  20. Duchaine, B.; Germine, L.; Nakayama, K. Family resemblance: Ten family members with prosopagnosia and within-class object agnosia. Cogn. Neuropsychol. 2007, 24, 419–430. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Rubino, C.; Corrow, S.L.; Corrow, J.C.; Duchaine, B.; Barton, J.J.S. Word and text processing in developmental prosopagnosia. Cogn. Neuropsychol. 2016, 33, 315–328. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Burns, E.J.; Bennetts, R.J.; Bate, S.; Wright, V.C.; Weidemann, C.T.; Tree, J.J. Intact word processing in developmental prosopagnosia. Sci. Rep. 2017, 7, 1683. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  23. Starrfelt, R.; Klargaard, S.K.; Petersen, A.; Gerlach, C. Reading in Developmental Prosopagnosia: Evidence for a Dissociation Between Word and Face Recognition. Neuropsychology 2018, 32, 138–147. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  24. Gerlach, C.; Klargaard, S.K.; Starrfelt, R. On the relation between face and object recognition in developmental prosopagnosia: No dissociation but a systematic association. PLoS ONE 2016, 11, 1–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  25. Geskin, J.; Behrmann, M. Congenital prosopagnosia without object agnosia? A literature review. Cogn. Neuropsychol. 2018, 35, 4–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  26. Kaplan, B.J.; Dewey, D.M.; Crawford, S.G.; Wilson, B.N. The Term Comorbidity Is of Questionable Value in Reference to Developmental Disorders: Data and Theory. J. Learn. Disabil. 2001, 34, 555–565. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Dewey, D. What Is Comorbidity and Why Does It Matter in Neurodevelopmental Disorders? Curr. Dev. Disord. Rep. 2018, 5, 235–242. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Ramus, F. Neurobiology of dyslexia: A reinterpretation of the data. Trends Neurosci. 2004, 27, 720–726. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  29. Carroll, J.M.; Maughan, B.; Goodman, R.; Meltzer, H. Literacy difficulties and psychiatric disorders: Evidence for comorbidity. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 2005, 46, 524–532. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Willcutt, E.G.; Pennington, B.F.; Olson, R.K.; Chhabildas, N.; Hulslander, J. Neuropsychological Analyses of Comorbidity Between Reading Disability and Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder: In Search of the Common Deficit. Dev. Neuropsychol. 2005, 27, 35–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Klargaard, S.K.; Starrfelt, R.; Petersen, A.; Gerlach, C. Topographic processing in developmental prosopagnosia: Preserved perception but impaired memory of scenes. Cogn. Neuropsychol. 2016, 33, 405–413. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Sigurdardottir, H.M.; Fridriksdottir, L.E.; Gudjonsdottir, S.; Kristjánsson, Á. Specific problems in visual cognition of dyslexic readers: Face discrimination deficits predict dyslexia over and above discrimination of scrambled faces and novel objects. Cognition 2018, 175, 157–168. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  33. Dawson, G.; Carver, L.; Meltzoff, A.N.; Panagiotides, H.; McPartland, J.; Webb, S.J. Neural Correlates of Face and Object Recognition in Young Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder, Developmental Delay, and Typical Development. Child Dev. 2002, 73, 700–717. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  34. Schultz, R.T. Developmental deficits in social perception in autism: The role of the amygdala and fusiform face area. Int. J. Dev. Neurosci. 2005, 23, 125–141. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Suri, K.; Lewis, M.; Minar, N.; Willson, E.; Ace, J. Face memory deficits in children and adolescents with autism spectrum disorder. J. Psychopathol. Behav. Assess 2021, 43, 108–118. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Duchaine, B.; Nakayama, K. The Cambridge Face Memory Test: Results for neurologically intact individuals and an investigation of its validity using inverted face stimuli and prosopagnosic participants. Neuropsychologia 2006, 44, 576–585. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Corrow, S.L.; Stubbs, J.L.; Schlaug, G.; Buss, S.; Paquette, S.; Duchaine, B.; Barton, J.J.S. Perception of musical pitch in developmental prosopagnosia. Neuropsychologia 2019, 124, 87–97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Garrido, L.; Furl, N.; Draganski, B.; Weiskopf, N.; Stevens, J.; Tan, G.C.-Y.; Driver, J.; Dolan, R.J.; Duchaine, B. Voxel-based morphometry reveals reduced grey matter volume in the temporal cortex of developmental prosopagnosics. Brain 2009, 132, 3443–3455. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Bate, S.; Cook, S.J.; Duchaine, B.; Tree, J.J.; Burns, E.J.; Hodgson, T.L. Intranasal inhalation of oxytocin improves face processing in developmental prosopagnosia. Cortex 2014, 50, 55–63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  40. McIntosh, R.D. Exploratory reports: A new article type for Cortex. Cortex 2017, 96, A1–A4. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  41. Moreau, D.; Wilson, A.J.; McKay, N.S.; Nihill, K.; Waldie, K.E. No evidence for systematic white matter correlates of dyslexia and dyscalculia. Neuroimage Clin. 2018, 18, 356–366. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Colorafi, K.J.; Evans, B. Qualitative Descriptive Methods in Health Science Research. HERD Health Environ. Res. Des. J. 2016, 9, 16–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  43. Arizpe, J.M.; Saad, E.; Douglas, A.O.; Germine, L.; Wilmer, J.B.; DeGutis, J.M. Self-reported face recognition is highly valid, but alone is not highly discriminative of prosopagnosia-level performance on objective assessments. Behav. Res. Methods 2019, 51, 1102–1116. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  44. Duchaine, B.; Yovel, G. 2.21.4.2—Developmental Prosopagnosia. In The Senses: A Comprehensive Reference; Masland, R.H., Albright, T.D., Albright, T.D., Masland, R.H., Dallos, P., Oertel, D., Firestein, S., Beauchamp, G.K., Catherine Bushnell, M., Basbaum, A.I., et al., Eds.; Academic Press: New York, NY, USA, 2008; pp. 345–347. [Google Scholar]
  45. Grueter, M.; Grueter, T.; Bell, V.; Horst, J.; Laskowski, W.; Sperling, K.; Halligan, P.W.; Elli, H.D.; Kennerknecht, I. Hereditary Prosopagnosia: The First Case Series. Cortex 2007, 43, 734–749. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Corrow, J.C.; Corrow, S.; Lee, E.; Pancaroglu, R.; Burles, F.; Duchaine, B.; Iaria, G.; Barton, J. Getting lost: Topographic skills in acquired and developmental prosopagnosia. Cortex 2016, 76, 89–103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  47. Collins, E.; Dundas, E.; Gabay, Y.; Plaut, D.C.; Behrmann, M. Hemispheric organization in disorders of development. Vis. Cogn. 2017, 25, 416–429. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Gabay, Y.; Dundas, E.; Plaut, D.; Behrmann, M. Atypical perceptual processing of faces in developmental dyslexia. Brain Lang. 2017, 173, 41–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Sigurdardottir, H.M.; Ívarsson, E.; Kristinsdóttir, K.; Kristjánsson, Á. Impaired Recognition of Faces and Objects in Dyslexia: Evidence for Ventral Stream Dysfunction? Neuropsychology 2015, 29, 739–750. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  50. Tarkiainen, A.; Helenius, P.; Salmelin, R. Category-specific occipitotemporal activation during face perception in dyslexic individuals: An MEG study. NeuroImage 2003, 19, 1194–1204. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Kühn, C.D.; Gerlach, C.; Andersen, K.B.; Poulsen, M.; Starrfelt, R. Face recognition in developmental dyslexia: Evidence for dissociation between faces and words. Cogn. Neuropsychol. 2021, 38, 107–115. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Wilson, A.J.; Andrewes, S.G.; Struthers, H.; Rowe, V.M.; Bogdanovic, R.; Waldie, K.E. Dyscalculia and dyslexia in adults: Cognitive bases of comorbidity. Learn. Individ. Differ. 2015, 37, 118–132. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Landerl, K.; Moll, K. Comorbidity of learning disorders: Prevalence and familial transmission. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 2010, 51, 287–294. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  54. Moreau, D.; Wiebels, K.; Wilson, A.J.; Waldie, K.E. Volumetric and surface characteristics of gray matter in adult dyslexia and dyscalculia. Neuropsychologia 2019, 127, 204–210. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  55. Esins, J.; Schultz, J.; Bülthoff, I.; Kennerknecht, I. Galactose uncovers face recognition and mental images in congenital prosopagnosia: The first case report. Nutr. Neurosci. 2014, 17, 239–240. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  56. Ulimoen, T.K.; Sørensen, T.A. Is Grapheme Colour Synesthesia linked to Prosopagnosia? J. Vis. 2019, 19, 24b. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Sørensen, T.A. Performance in face perception task is decreased in observers with colour-grapheme synaesthesia. In Proceedings of the Synaesthesia in Perspective: Development, Networks, and Multisensory Processing, Hamburg, Germany, 2014; Universitätsklinikum Hamburg-Eppendorf: Hamburg, Germany, 2014. [Google Scholar]
  58. Mulvenna, C. Synesthesia and Creativity. In Oxford Handbook of Synesthesia; Simmer, J., Hubbard, S., Eds.; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2013; pp. 1–27. [Google Scholar]
  59. Rothen, N.; Meier, B.; Ward, J. Enhanced memory ability: Insights from synaesthesia. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2012, 36, 1952–1963. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Bate, S.; Bennetts, R.J.; Gregory, N.; Tree, J.J.; Murray, E.; Adams, A.; Bobak, A.K.; Penton, T.; Yang, T.; Banissy, M.J. Objective Patterns of Face Recognition Deficits in 165 Adults with Self-Reported Developmental Prosopagnosia. Brain Sci. 2019, 9, 133. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  61. Ulrich, P.I.N.; Wilkinson, D.T.; Ferguson, H.J.; Smith, L.J.; Bindemann, M.; Johnston, R.A.; Schmalzl, L. Perceptual and Memorial Contributions to Developmental Prosopagnosia. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 2017, 70, 298–315. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Tree, J.J.; Wilkie, J. Face and object imagery in congenital prosopagnosia: A case series. Cortex 2010, 46, 1189–1198. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Grüter, T.; Grüter, M.; Bell, V.; Carbon, C.-C. Visual mental imagery in congenital prosopagnosia. Neurosci. Lett. 2009, 453, 135–140. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Zeman, A.; Dewar, M.; Della Sala, S. Lives without imagery—Congenital aphantasia. Cortex 2015, 73, 378–380. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  65. Palombo, D.J.; Alain, C.; Söderlund, H.; Khuu, W.; Levine, B. Severely deficient autobiographical memory (SDAM) in healthy adults: A new mnemonic syndrome. Neuropsychologia 2015, 72, 105–118. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  66. Chen, Y.; Norton, D.; Ongur, D.; Heckers, S. Inefficient Face Detection in Schizophrenia. Schizophr. Bull. 2007, 34, 367–374. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  67. Belge, J.-B.; Maurage, P.; Mangelinckx, C.; Leleux, D.; Delatte, B.; Constant, E. Facial decoding in schizophrenia is underpinned by basic visual processing impairments. Psychiatry Res. 2017, 255, 167–172. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. SurveyXact By Ramboll. Available online: https://www.surveyxact.dk/ (accessed on 14 December 2021).
  69. World Medical Association (WMA). World Medical Association Declaration of Helsinki: Ethical principles for medical research involving human subjects. JAMA 2013, 310, 2191–2194. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  70. Guterbock, T.M.; Marcopulos, B.A. Survey methods for neuropsychologists: A review of typical methodological pitfalls and suggested solutions. Clin. Neuropsychol. 2020, 34, 13–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  71. Shah, P.; Gaule, A.; Sowden, S.; Bird, G.; Cook, R. The 20-item prosopagnosia index (PI20): A self-report instrument for identifying developmental prosopagnosia. R. Soc. Open Sci. 2015, 2, 140343. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  72. Claessen, M.H.G.; Visser-Meily, J.M.A.; de Rooij, N.K.; Postma, A.; van der Ham, I.J.M. The Wayfinding Questionnaire as a Self-report Screening Instrument for Navigation-related Complaints After Stroke: Internal Validity in Healthy Respondents and Chronic Mild Stroke Patients. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 2016, 31, 839–854. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  73. de Rooij, N.K.; Claessen, M.H.G.; van der Ham, I.J.M.; Post, M.W.M.; Visser-Meily, J.M.A. The Wayfinding Questionnaire: A clinically useful self-report instrument to identify navigation complaints in stroke patients. Neuropsychol. Rehabil. 2019, 29, 1042–1061. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  74. Gregory, R.J. Chapter 4 VALIDITY AND TEST CONSTRUCTION. In Psychological Testing: History, Principles, and Applications; Gregory, R.J., Ed.; Pearson Education: Essex, UK, 2014; pp. 118–153. [Google Scholar]
  75. Rice, G.E.; Kerry, S.J.; Robotham, R.J.; Leff, A.; Ralph, M.L.; Starrfelt, R. Category-selective deficits are the exception and not the rule: Evidence from a case-series of 64 patients with ventral occipito-temporal cortex damage. Cortex 2020, 138, 266–281. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  76. Lefly, D.L.; Pennington, B.F. Reliability and validity of the Adult Reading History Questionnaire. J. Learn. Disabil. 2000, 33, 286–296. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  77. Susilo, T.; Duchaine, B. Advances in developmental prosopagnosia research. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 2013, 23, 423–429. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  78. Gerlach, C.; Klargaard, S.K.; Alnæs, D.; Kolskår, K.K.; Karstoft, J.; Westlye, L.T.; Starrfelt, R. Left hemisphere abnormalities in developmental prosopagnosia when looking at faces but not words. Brain Commun. 2019, 1, 1–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  79. Bjornsdottir, G.; Halldorsson, J.G.; Steinberg, S.; Hansdottir, I.; Kristjansson, K.; Stefansson, H.; Stefansson, K. The Adult Reading History Questionnaire (ARHQ) in Icelandic: Psychometric Properties and Factor Structure. J. Learn. Disabil. 2014, 47, 532–542. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  80. Schelke, M.W.; Shapiro, S.D.; Hackett, K.; Chen, J.; Simchon-Steinhof, S.; Ganzer, C.A.; Isaacson, R.S.; Tamboer, P.; Van Vliet, E.; Assuras, S.; et al. Diagnosis of developmental learning and attention disorders in adults: A review of clinical modalities. Neurol. Psychiatry Brain Res. 2017, 23, 27–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  81. Butterworth, B. Personal communication. 26 March. 2020.
  82. Willcutt, E.G.; Boada, R.; Riddle, M.W.; Chhabildas, N.; DeFries, J.C.; Pennington, B.F. Colorado Learning Difficulties Questionnaire: Validation of a parent-report screening measure. Psychol. Assess. 2011, 23, 778–791. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  83. Barbaresi, W.J.; Katusic, S.K.; Colligan, R.C.; Weaver, A.L.; Jacobsen, S.J. Math Learning Disorder: Incidence in a Population-Based Birth Cohort, 1976–1982, Rochester, Minn. Ambul. Pediatrics 2005, 5, 281–289. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  84. Butterworth, B.; Varma, S.; Laurillard, D. Dyscalculia: From Brain to Education. Science 2011, 332, 1049–1053. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  85. De Visscher, A.; Noël, M.-P.; Pesenti, M.; Dormal, V. Developmental dyscalculia in adults: Beyond numerical magnitude impairment. J. Learn. Disabil. 2018, 51, 600–611. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  86. Popping, R. Analyzing Open-ended Questions by Means of Text Analysis Procedures. Bull. Méthodol. Sociol. 2015, 128, 23–39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  87. IBM Corp. IBM SPSS Statistics for Macintosh, 27; IBM Corp.: Armonk, NY, USA, 2020. [Google Scholar]
  88. McHugh, M.L. The chi-square test of independence. Biochem. Med. 2013, 23, 143–149. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  89. Kim, H.-Y. Statistical notes for clinical researchers: Chi-squared test and Fisher’s exact test. Restor. Dent. Endod. 2017, 42, 152–155. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  90. Fabrigar, L.R.; Wegener, D.T.; Maccallum, R.C.; Strahan, E.J. Evaluating the Use of Exploratory Factor Analysis in Psychological Research. Psychol. Methods 1999, 4, 272–299. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  91. Schmitt, T.A. Current Methodological Considerations in Exploratory and Confirmatory Factor Analysis. J. Psychoeduc. Assess. 2011, 29, 304–321. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  92. Hair, J.F.; Hult, G.T.M.; Ringle, C.M.; Sarstedt, M. Data Distribution. In A Primer on Partial Least Squares Structural Squation Modeling (PLS-SEM), 2nd ed.; Hair, J.F., Ed.; Sage: Los Angeles, CA, USA, 2017; pp. 60–62. [Google Scholar]
  93. Costello, A.B.; Osborne, J.W. Best practices in exploratory factor analysis: Four recommendations for getting the most from your analysis. Pract. Assess. Res. Eval. 2005, 10, 7. [Google Scholar]
  94. Tabachnick, B.G.; Fidell, L.S. Principal Components and Factor Analysis. In Using Multivariate Statistics: Pearson New International Edition, 6th ed.; Pearson Education Limited: Essex, UK, 2013; pp. 660–713. [Google Scholar]
  95. Hsieh, H.-F.; Shannon, S.E. Three Approaches to Qualitative Content Analysis. Qual. Health Res. 2005, 15, 1277–1288. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  96. NVivo Qualitative Data Analysis Software; Version 12.6; QSR International Pty Ltd.: Doncaster, VIC, Australia, 2019.
  97. Yong, A.G.; Pearce, S. A Beginner’s Guide to Factor Analysis: Focusing on Exploratory Factor Analysis. Quant. Methods Psychol. 2013, 9, 79–94. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  98. DeVellis, R.F. Scale Development: Theory and Applications, 4th ed.; Sage: Los Angeles, CA, USA, 2017. [Google Scholar]
  99. Ventura, P.; Livingston, L.A.; Shah, P. Adults have moderate-to-good insight into their face recognition ability: Further validation of the 20-item Prosopagnosia Index in a Portuguese sample. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 2018, 71, 2677–2679. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  100. DuPaul, G.J.; Weyandt, L.L.; O’Dell, S.M.; Varejao, M. College Students With ADHD:Current Status and Future Directions. J. Atten. Disord. 2009, 13, 234–250. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  101. Watkins, N.W. (A)phantasia and severely deficient autobiographical memory: Scientific and personal perspectives. Cortex 2018, 105, 41–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  102. Zeman, A.; Milton, F.; Della Sala, S.; Dewar, M.; Frayling, T.; Gaddum, J.; Hattersley, A.; Heuerman-Williamson, B.; Jones, K.; MacKisack, M.; et al. Phantasia—The psychological significance of lifelong visual imagery vividness extremes. Cortex 2020, 130, 426–440. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  103. Paquette, S.; Li, H.C.; Corrow, S.L.; Buss, S.S.; Barton, J.J.S.; Schlaug, G. Developmental Perceptual Impairments: Cases When Tone-Deafness and Prosopagnosia Co-occur. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2018, 12, 1–6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  104. Iaria, G.; Barton, J.J.S. Developmental topographical disorientation: A newly discovered cognitive disorder. Exp. Brain Res. 2010, 206, 189–196. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  105. Susilo, T. The face specificity of lifelong prosopagnosia. Cogn. Neuropsychol. 2018, 35, 1–3. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  106. De Haan, E.H.; Campbell, R. A fifteen year follow-up of a case of developmental prosopagnosia. Cortex 1991, 27, 489–509. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  107. Johnen, A.; Schmukle, S.C.; Hüttenbrink, J.; Kischka, C.; Kennerknecht, I.; Dobel, C. A family at risk: Congenital prosopagnosia, poor face recognition and visuoperceptual deficits within one family. Neuropsychologia 2014, 58, 52–63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  108. Gauthier, I.; Behrmann, M.; Tarr, M.J. Can face recognition really be dissociated from object recognition? J. Cogn. Neurosci. 1999, 11, 349–370. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  109. Gerlach, C.; Starrfelt, R. Patterns of perceptual performance in developmental prosopagnosia: An in-depth case series. Cogn. Neuropsychol. 2021, 38, 27–49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  110. Duchaine, B.C.; Nakayama, K. Developmental prosopagnosia: A window to content-specific face processing. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 2006, 16, 166–173. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  111. Kimchi, R.; Behrmann, M.; Avidan, G.; Amishav, R. Perceptual separability of featural and configural information in congenital prosopagnosia. Cogn. Neuropsychol. 2012, 29, 447–463. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  112. Stollhoff, R.; Jost, J.; Elze, T.; Kennerknecht, I. Deficits in Long-Term Recognition Memory Reveal Dissociated Subtypes in Congenital Prosopagnosia. PLoS ONE 2011, 6, 1–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  113. Zhao, Y.; Li, J.; Liu, X.; Song, Y.; Wang, R.; Yang, Z.; Liu, J. Altered spontaneous neural activity in the occipital face area reflects behavioral deficits in developmental prosopagnosia. Neuropsychologia 2016, 89, 344–355. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  114. Jiahui, G.; Yang, H.; Duchaine, B. Developmental prosopagnosics have widespread selectivity reductions across category-selective visual cortex. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2018, 115, E6418–E6427. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  115. Shaywitz, S.E.M.D. Dyslexia. N. Engl. J. Med. 1998, 338, 307–312. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  116. Shaywitz, S.E.; Shaywitz, B.A.; Fletcher, J.M.; Escobar, M.D. Prevalence of reading disability in boys and girls. Results of the Connecticut Longitudinal Study. JAMA 1990, 264, 998–1002. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  117. Cai, L.; Chen, Y.; Hu, X.; Guo, Y.; Zhao, X.; Sun, T.; Wu, Y.; Li, X. An Epidemiological Study of Chinese Children with Developmental Dyslexia. J. Dev. Behav. Pediatr. 2020, 41, 203–211. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  118. Simner, J.; Mulvenna, C.; Sagiv, N.; Tsakanikos, E.; Witherby, S.A.; Fraser, C.; Scott, K.; Ward, J. Synaesthesia: The Prevalence of Atypical Cross-Modal Experiences. Perception 2006, 35, 1024–1033. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  119. Watson, M.R.; Akins, K.A.; Spiker, C.; Crawford, L.; Enns, J.T. Synesthesia and learning: A critical review and novel theory. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2014, 8, 1–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  120. Maurer, D.; Ghloum, J.K.; Gibson, L.C.; Watson, M.R.; Chen, L.M.; Akins, K.; Enns, J.T.; Hensch, T.K.; Werker, J.F. Reduced perceptual narrowing in synesthesia. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2020, 117, 10089–10096. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  121. Palermo, R.; Rossion, B.; Rhodes, G.; Laguesse, R.; Tez, T.; Hall, B.; Albonico, A.; Malaspina, M.; Daini, R.; Irons, J.; et al. Do People Have Insight into their Face Recognition Abilities? Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 2017, 70, 218–233. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  122. Zell, E.; Krizan, Z. Do People Have Insight Into Their Abilities? A Metasynthesis. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 2014, 9, 111–125. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  123. Murray, E.; Bate, S. Self-ratings of face recognition ability are influenced by gender but not prosopagnosia severity. Psychol. Assess. 2019, 31, 828–832. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  124. Livingston, L.A.; Shah, P. People with and without prosopagnosia have insight into their face recognition ability. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 2017, 71, 1260–1262. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  125. Shah, P.; Sowden, S.; Gaule, A.; Catmur, C.; Bird, G. The 20 item prosopagnosia index (PI20): Relationship with the Glasgow face-matching test. R. Soc. Open Sci. 2015, 2, 150305. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  126. Gray, K.L.H.; Bird, G.; Cook, R. Robust associations between the 20-item prosopagnosia index and the Cambridge Face Memory Test in the general population. R. Soc. Open Sci. 2017, 4, 1–5. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  127. Tsantani, M.; Vestner, T.; Cook, R. The Twenty Item Prosopagnosia Index (PI20) provides meaningful evidence of face recognition impairment. R. Soc. Open Sci. 2021, 8, 202062. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  128. Fields, R.D. White matter in learning, cognition and psychiatric disorders. Trends Neurosci. 2008, 31, 361–370. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  129. Groves, R.M.; Presser, S.; Dipko, S. The Role of Topic Interest in Survey Participation Decisions. Public Opin. Q. 2004, 68, 2–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  130. Rubenstein, E.; Furnier, S.J.A.i.A. # Bias: The opportunities and challenges of surveys that recruit and collect data of autistic adults online. Autism Adulthood 2021, 3, 120–128. [Google Scholar]
  131. Maccallum, R.C.; Widaman, K.F.; Zhang, S.; Hong, S. Sample Size in Factor Analysis. Psychol. Methods 1999, 4, 84–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  132. de Winter, J.C.F.; Dodou, D.; Wieringa, P.A. Exploratory Factor Analysis With Small Sample Sizes. Multivar. Behav. Res. 2009, 44, 147–181. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  133. Astle, D.E.; Fletcher-Watson, S. Beyond the Core-Deficit Hypothesis in Developmental Disorders. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2020, 29, 431–437. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  134. Siugzdaite, R.; Bathelt, J.; Holmes, J.; Astle, D.E. Transdiagnostic Brain Mapping in Developmental Disorders. Curr. Biol. 2020, 30, 1245–1257.e1244. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Scree plot before factor extraction.
Figure 1. Scree plot before factor extraction.
Brainsci 12 00230 g001
Table 1. Participants’ age.
Table 1. Participants’ age.
Age in Yearsn (%)
18–242 (1.7)
25–34 18 (15.7)
35–4438 (33.0)
45–54 23 (20.0)
55–64 18 (15.7)
65–74 14 (12.2)
≥752 (1.7)
Total115 (100.0)
Table 2. Demographics.
Table 2. Demographics.
Demographicsn (%)
DP diagnosis *34 (29.6)
English as first language77 (67.0)
Handedness (left/right/ambidextrous)10/99/6 (8.7/86.1/5.2)
* Self-report of being diagnosed with DP by healthcare professional or researcher.
Table 3. Self-reported developmental comorbidity co-occurring with s-DP.
Table 3. Self-reported developmental comorbidity co-occurring with s-DP.
Self-Reported Developmental Comorbidityn (%)
Presence of at least one developmental comorbidity a65 (56.5)
Dyslexia4 (3.5)
Dyscalculia4 (3.5)
ADHD7 (6.1)
Aphantasia24 (20.9)
Synesthesia12 (10.4)
Object agnosia29 (25.2)
Memory problems27 (23.5)
Presence of not-predefined mental, cognitive or intellectual disorder b13 (11.3)
Presence of not-predefined conditions related to vision b2 (1.7)
Presence of not-predefined somatic disorder b2 (1.7)
Navigation problems *29 (25.2)
Note that for the individual disorders, a participant reporting, e.g., both ADHD and dyslexia, is counted in both respective categories. a Number of participants reporting at least one comorbid disorder or deficit being either dyslexia, dyscalculia, ADHD, aphantasia, synesthesia, object agnosia, or/and memory problems. Navigation problems are not counted here as they are not a binary variable. b Details about the specific self-reported conditions can be found in Supplementary Table S4. Not-predefined refers to conditions reported in the free-text options. * Navigation problems are reported as the number of participants reporting indications of clinically relevant complaints (≤3) on all five items from the Wayfinding Questionnaire.
Table 4. Number of reported developmental comorbidities (besides s-DP).
Table 4. Number of reported developmental comorbidities (besides s-DP).
Number of Developmental Comorbidities *n (%)
050 (43.5)
134 (29.6)
222 (19.1)
38 (7.0)
51 (0.9)
Total115 (100.0)
* These percentages are based on participants reporting as having ADHD, dyslexia, or dyscalculia to the questions ‘Do you have other diagnoses? Particularly related to vision, mental illness, learning difficulties, sensory disorders, developmental disorders, motor disorders, etc.’ or aphantasia, synesthesia, object agnosia, and/or memory problems in response to the question ‘Do you have any of the following conditions related to cognitive strengths or difficulties?’. Thus, navigation problems are not included.
Table 5. Developmental comorbidity and s-DP severity.
Table 5. Developmental comorbidity and s-DP severity.
Severity Group Based on PI20Number of Cases Reporting Developmental Comorbidity
(% of Subsample)
Mild DP (n = 15)9 (60.0)
Moderate DP (n = 42)22 (52.4)
Severe DP (n = 58)34 (58.6)
Table 6. Factor loadings based on a principal axis factoring with oblique rotation for 33 items from the Difficulties and Abilities in Developmental Prosopagnosia Questionnaire (N = 115).
Table 6. Factor loadings based on a principal axis factoring with oblique rotation for 33 items from the Difficulties and Abilities in Developmental Prosopagnosia Questionnaire (N = 115).
Difficulties and Abilities in Developmental Prosopagnosia Questionnaire Items Factor 1Factor 2Factor 3Factor 4
PI1. My face recognition is worse than most people0.456 −0.173−0.165
PI2. I have always had a bad memory for faces0.2890.165
PI3. I find it noticeably easier to recognize people who have distinctive facial features −00.331
PI4. I often mistake people I have met before for strangers0.430
PI5. When I was at school I struggled to recognize my classmates0.653 0.1730.106
PI6. When people change their hairstyle, or wear hats, I have problems recognizing them0.478 −0.196
PI7. I sometimes have to warn new people I meet that I am ‘bad with faces’0.411
PI8. I find it easy to picture individual faces in my mind0.154 −0.160
PI9. I am better than most people at putting a ‘name to a face’0.152
PI10. Without hearing people’s voices, I struggle to recognize them0.548 0.115
PI11. Anxiety about face recognition has led me to avoid certain social or professional situations0.377 0.183
PI12. I have to try harder than other people to memorize faces 0.141−0.330
PI13. I am very confident in my ability to recognize myself in photographs 04500.171
PI14. I sometimes find movies hard to follow because of difficulties recognizing characters0.371 0.209−0.166
PI15. My friends and family think I have bad face recognition or bad face memory0.472−0.141
PI16. I feel like I frequently offend by not recognizing who they are0.549−0.141
PI17. It is easy for me to recognize individuals in situations that require people to wear similar clothes (e.g.,suits, uniforms, swimwear)0.464 −0.315−0.102
PI18. At family gatherings, I sometimes confuse individual family members0.553
PI19. I find it easy to recognize celebrities in ‘before-they-were-famous’ photos, even if they have changed considerably0.149 −0.182−0.326
PI20. It is hard to recognize familiar people when I meet them out of context (e.g.,meeting a work colleague unexpectedly while shopping)0.646 0.199
WQ1. I am good at understanding and following route descriptions −0.801 −0.124
WQ2. I can always orient myself quickly and correctly when I am in an unknown environment −0.967
WQ3. I can easily find the shortest route to a known destination −0.911
WQ4. I can usually recall a new route after I have walked it once −0.853−0.102
WQ5. When I am in a building for the first time, I can easily point to the entrance of this building. −0.862
ARHQ1. How much extra help did you need when learning to read in elementary school? 0.767
ARHQ2. How would you compare your reading skill to that of others in your elementary classes?−0.140 0.744
ARHQ3. How much difficulty did you have learning to spell in elementary school?0.166 0.6950.159
ARHQ4. How much difficulty did you have learning to read in elementary school? 0.829
ARHQ5. How would you compare your current reading speed to that of others of the same age and education? 0.1710.558−0.120
MATH1. As a child, did you make careless errors in math, such as adding when the sign indicated subtraction?0.156 0.1080.779
MATH2. As a child, did you have trouble learning new math concepts? 0.128 0.816
MATH3. Do you have difficulties estimating quantities, e.g., how many coins are lying on a table? 0.194 0.653
Note: Values below 0.10 are suppressed. PI = prosopagnosia index, WQ = Wayfinding Questionnaire, ARHQ = Adult Reading History Questionnaire. Bold type: Strong factor loadings of above 0.5 and below −0.5. Factor loadings not meeting the criterion of above 0.32 or below −0.32 are written in grey.
Table 7. Qualitative coding categories (n = 114) a.
Table 7. Qualitative coding categories (n = 114) a.
Difficulties CodesNbQuoteStrengths CodesNbQuote
Aphantasia 24‘I am not very good at visualizing things in my head. I’ve explained that it’s more like when I picture something in my mind, it looks kind of like a Lite Brite toy. With just like colored outlines of things. So imagining The beach, it’s just a yellow line for the sand, some blue blur for the ocean, and an outlined yellow sun. Like a kid drawing.’Good Visual Mental Imagery7‘I have a very strong visual imagination. I can create whole worlds full of detail in my imagination.’
Difficulties with Math 27‘I think I have some dyscalculia. I will transpose numbers even though I am trying to be careful and can do this even in a short string of only 3 numbers. However I don’t have trouble with mathematical concepts or at least not up to the usual school leaver level.’Mathematical Abilities 20‘I am better at math than most people. I enjoy math and I’m good at it. Even when I don’t “feel” like I fully understand it, I perform well on math/stats tests.’
Reading and Spelling Difficulties 8‘Learning a new language is impossible (I live in Indonesia for 23 years). Very slow reader, I easily forget what I have read.’Reading and Spelling Abilities 39‘I’m a very good reader, writer, editor, and proofreader.’
Memory Problems 21‘I have a terrible memory. I feel like I barely remember anything prior to highschool and I am only 29 years old. Even then things are very foggy. I only remember things if I see them in a photograph.’Good Memory 40‘Far above average skilled in remembering fun facts and historical events. Very good memory in order of recalling details of what’s happened in my own life.’
Object Agnosia 7‘I’m ‘car-blind’, and had to memorize my parents’ license plates as a child.’
Synesthesia 2‘See colours when listening to music’
Lack of Musicality 17‘Very tone deaf- measures by up to 2 full notes by a music teacher.’Musicality 26‘I can hear something once and play it on the piano.’
Difficulties with Sports 17‘I am a disaster when it comes to sports or athletics, especially team sports. Partially this is due to my lack of coordination and skill, and partially because I never knew who to pass the ball to!’Sports Skills 12‘I was a highly competitive gymnast for 14 years. I have very good body awareness.’
Motor Coordination and Body–Space Perception 10‘I’m clumsy, can’t catch or throw, and miss judge and catch the edges of things when walking.’
Problems with Inattentiveness, Impulsivity, or Hyperactivity 2 ‘Need more time on tasks, partly because I want to be thorough and am afraid of doing mistakes, partly because of lack of routines. Bad at routines and “what to do next”. Easy to distract. “Colour codes” on buttons etc. doesn’t help me, seems like I don’t notice them (I have normal colour vision).’
Social and Interpersonal Skills 12‘Knowing what other people feel, and why they feel it. “intuitive profiling” so to say’
Skills in Visual Arts 12‘I am very artistic. I have shown my work and been featured in a variety of mediums throughout my whole life. Most specifically I paint faces and have since I was about 12 years old and was fascinated with faces.’
Navigation Problems41‘I am terrible at navigation. Even in a mid-sized town (100,000 people) I have lived for 20 years, I still get lost. I have no clue which is N, S, E, W.’Navigation Abilities13‘Maps, geography.’
Full descriptions of the coding categories are available in the coding manual in the Supplementary Material. a One participant wrote the response in French and was thus excluded from the qualitative analysis. b The frequency is based on the number of participants mentioning content related to the specific code; even if the same person has several codes for the same category, it is only counted once. The background indicates separate interpretations/codings; Italics indicate quotations.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Svart, N.; Starrfelt, R. Is It Just Face Blindness? Exploring Developmental Comorbidity in Individuals with Self-Reported Developmental Prosopagnosia. Brain Sci. 2022, 12, 230. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci12020230

AMA Style

Svart N, Starrfelt R. Is It Just Face Blindness? Exploring Developmental Comorbidity in Individuals with Self-Reported Developmental Prosopagnosia. Brain Sciences. 2022; 12(2):230. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci12020230

Chicago/Turabian Style

Svart, Nanna, and Randi Starrfelt. 2022. "Is It Just Face Blindness? Exploring Developmental Comorbidity in Individuals with Self-Reported Developmental Prosopagnosia" Brain Sciences 12, no. 2: 230. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci12020230

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop