Next Article in Journal
Telemedicine in the Management of Parkinson’s Disease: Achievements, Challenges, and Future Perspectives
Previous Article in Journal
Electroacupuncture Alleviates Neuroinflammation by Inhibiting the HMGB1 Signaling Pathway in Rats with Sepsis-Associated Encephalopathy
Previous Article in Special Issue
Effects of a 10-Week Physical Activity Intervention on Asylum Seekers’ Physiological Health
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Review

Effect of Physical Exercise in Real-World Settings on Executive Function of Typical Children and Adolescents: A Systematic Review

1
School of Physical Education, Shanghai University of Sport, Shanghai 200438, China
2
School of Physical Education, Liaoning Normal University, Dalian 116029, China
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Brain Sci. 2022, 12(12), 1734; https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci12121734
Submission received: 10 October 2022 / Revised: 15 November 2022 / Accepted: 22 November 2022 / Published: 18 December 2022
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Physical Exercise for Brain Health and Longevity)

Abstract

:
Objective: The aim of this paper is to provide a systematic review of research on physical exercise in real-world settings on executive function of typical children and adolescents. Methods: The CNKI, WOS, PubMed, ScienceDirect, and SPORTDiscus databases were searched by computer. Two researchers independently screened the literature, extracted data, and evaluated the risk of bias in the included literature. Statistical analysis was performed using frequency and percentage and the χ2 test. Results: A total of 49 articles was included. Acute (moderate intensity lasting 30–50 min) and long-term (interventions of moderate intensity of 30–50 min at least 3 times a week for 17 weeks or more) physical exercises in real-world settings have positive intervention effects on executive function. Furthermore, for acute interventions, closed skills are more efficient for inhibitory control, open skills are more efficient for working memory and cognitive flexibility, and open-continuous and closed-sequential skills are the most efficient; long-term interventions with open skills, sequential skills, and open-sequential skills are more effective. Conclusion: Physical exercise in real-world settings has a good promotion effect on typical children and adolescents, and motor skills with open and/or sequential attributes are more helpful in improving executive function.

1. Introduction

Executive function can be described as a higher-level, top-down thinking process that is closely related to frontal brain activity [1]. It is the process by which individuals coordinate divergent cognitive activities while undertaking a cognitive task, with the objective of enabling individuals to achieve set targets and produce intentional behavior in a flexible and efficient way [2]. In general, the executive function is recognized as a multidimensional structure [3]. Although there is an ongoing debate over the elements of executive function, the general consensus is that executive function includes flexibility, goal-setting and planning, attention and memory systems (such as working memory), and inhibitory control [4,5]. Children and adolescents are at a peak cognitive-development stage. The level of executive-function development during this period is crucial for academic achievement, physical and mental health, and social adaptation [6,7,8]. As a corollary, it is crucial to study the cognitive development and facilitation strategies of children and adolescents for the benefit of their long-term psychological and physiological health.
In sports-science research, the benefits of physical exercise on the physical and mental health of children and adolescents have been a central focus. An increasing number of studies [9,10,11] confirm the positive effects of physical exercise on brain growth and cognitive development in children and adolescents. Physical exercise increases the plasticity of gray- and white-matter structures [12,13], enhances the state of brain activation during specific tasks [14], and strengthens functional brain networks [15], thereby bolstering executive function in children and adolescents. Early research on the effects of exercise interventions on the executive function of children and adolescents was conducted primarily in laboratory settings. Their interventions, which primarily involved treadmills and power bikes, disregarded the complexity of movement in real-world settings and lacked ecological validity [16]. Moreover, positive laboratory results indicate that running and cycling are efficient methods for enhancing executive function. Running and cycling can be monotonous for children and adolescents, who are more likely to engage in other forms of physical activity [17]. The advantage of real-world settings is that equipment requirements are less complicated and easier to incorporate into in- or extra-curricular physical activities [18]. Therefore, Vazou et al. [19] called on researchers to conduct more real-world studies to determine which type of exercise intervention is the most effective. Pesce et al. [20] and Diamond et al. [21] believe that the restriction of focusing solely on quantitative characteristics of sports (intensity, period, frequency, and duration, etc.) should be broken and attention should be shifted to the qualitative aspects of sports (metabolic function, motor skill type, etc.).
Physical exercise is based on motor skills. Motor skills are operational activities that are acquired through learning. Based on the predictability of the surrounding environment, motor skills can be categorized into open and closed skills [22]. Open skills refer to the activities of performing motor tasks in an unpredictably changing environment, requiring individuals to react and adapt their movements. Closed skills refer to motor tasks performed in a stable and predictable environment, where individuals can plan their motor routines in advance [22]. Therefore, there may be a difference in the efficacy of interventions for executive function between open and closed skills. A recent meta-analysis [23] examined the effects of open and closed skills on cognitive function in children, adults, and the elderly and reported that open skills enhanced cognitive function more than closed skills. The dynamic interaction between “individual–environment–task,” in which individuals are required to engage in more cognitive and decision-making processes, strengthens brain structure and function by coordinating and consolidating existing movements and creating novel movements [24,25].
Nevertheless, a number of studies [26,27] have uncovered no distinction between the effects of open and closed skills on executive function. This could be due to the fact that the structure of the movement may modulate the effects of open- and closed-skill interventions on executive functions. The number of joints involved in a movement reflects the complexity of the structure of the movement; the more joints involved in the movement, the more body coordination is facilitated [28]. Multi-articular, cognitively demanding motor-repetition exercises help to activate brain-related neural circuitry [24]. Numerous studies [29,30] have also shown a strong link between motor coordination and executive function in children and adolescents; impaired executive function is the central deficit in developmental coordination disorder (DCD) [31].
In addition, motor skills cannot effectively distinguish between activity tasks through a single dimension. For instance, the motor structure of aerobics and middle-distance running, which are both closed skills, differs significantly. Basketball and Tai Chi, which are both sequence skills, have distinct environmental contexts and cognitive-participation differences. Motor skills can be classified as sequential or continuous, depending on the complexity of the movement structure [22]. Sequential skills are more complex motor sequences that link multiple discrete motors in a particular order. Continuous skills are multiple repetitions of a single discrete motor without a peculiar beginning or end and a relatively uniform motor structure [22]. The classification systems of Schmidt et al. [32] and Voss et al. [33] were applied to develop four types of motor skills for this study: open–sequential skills, open–continuous skills, closed–sequential skills, and closed–continuous skills.
Consequently, this study has the following three research objectives: (1) to systematically review the effects of real-world exercise on the executive function of typical children and adolescents; (2) to explore the moderating effect of quantitative characteristics of interventions on executive function; and (3) to investigate the moderating effect of motor skill types on executive function.

2. Materials and Methods

This review was registered (CRD42022348781) in the International Prospective Register of Systematic Reviews (PROSPERO). The Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA 2020) guidelines [21] were followed for this study.

2.1. Search Strategy

An individual researcher used keywords to search the relevant literature. The China National Knowledge Infrastructure (CNKI), Web of Science (WOS), PubMed, ScienceDirect, and SPORTDiscus databases were scrutinized for pertinent literature. In addition, Google Scholar searches were employed to identify literature that may have been overlooked. The search date ranges from the creation of the database to May 2022. In this study, a combination search with the following three sets of subject terms was conducted: (1) motor skill OR sports skill OR sports items OR sports types OR exercise; (2) executive function OR working memory OR inhibition control OR inhibitory control OR cognitive flexibility OR self-control; and (3) children OR child OR adolescent OR teenagers OR young. The Boolean logical operator “AND” is used to join three groups of subject terms. Similar search terms were applied to search the titles and keywords of the databases listed above. In addition, references in the obtained articles were reviewed.

2.2. Selection Criteria

According to PICOS principles [34], inclusion and exclusion criteria for the literature were formulated. The inclusion criteria were as follows: (1) subjects were typical children and adolescents; (2) exercise interventions in real-world settings were acute and long-term; (3) control measures included traditional physical-education courses, basic academic courses, free activities, or being seated; (4) outcome variables consisted of planning, inhibitory control, working memory, and cognitive flexibility; and (5) study designs included a randomized controlled trial (RCT), randomized crossover design (RCD), non-randomized concurrent control trial (non-RCCT), and before–after study (BAS). The exclusion criteria were as follows: (1) cross-sectional, case-control, and other descriptive studies; (2) reviews, abstracts, letters, and comments lacking a clear description of the study design; (3) screen-based physical games, such as Xbox and Kinect; (4) combined physical exercise and cognitive-therapy interventions; and (5) in the case of duplicate publications, only the superior-quality literature was considered for inclusion. The order of title, abstract, figure, and full text determined the literature-selection procedure. The selection of the literature was carried out independently by two researchers each, with two other researchers conducting a secondary assessment of the selected literature. In the event of a disagreement between the two groups, all researchers would exchange assessments and reach a consensus.

2.3. Data Extraction

First author, publication date, study design, subject characteristics (sample size, age, and percentage of females), interventions (sports items, intervention period, weekly frequency, session duration, and intensity), control measures, and outcome variables (measures and results) were entered into Excel 2010 and stored. In addition, sports activities were categorized into a classification system for motor skills. The data extraction was undertaken independently by two researchers, and the extracted data were reviewed by two additional researchers. In the event of a disagreement between the two groups, all researchers exchanged assessments and reached a consensus.

2.4. Quality Assessment

The quality of RCTs and RCDs was evaluated utilizing the risk-of-bias assessment tool recommended by the Cochrane Collaboration Network [35]. Six aspects of the tool were evaluated: randomization methods, blinding, allocation concealment, completeness of outcome data, selective reporting of study results, and other biases. The methodological index for non-randomized studies (MINORS) [36] scale was utilized to assess the quality of non-RCCTs and BAS. The tool consists of 12 entries, of which 9 to 12 are additional criteria used to evaluate studies with a control group. Each entry is assigned a score of 2, for a total score of 24. A score of 0 indicates “not reported”, 1 means “reported with insufficient information”, and 2 denotes “reported with sufficient information”. Two researchers independently evaluated the assessment tools. In case of significant disagreements, they were discussed with a third researcher.

2.5. Statistical Methods

Due to the differences in research paradigms and measurement tools in the included literature, it was difficult to estimate effect sizes using meta-analysis; therefore, this study only employed a systematic-review approach to evaluate research results in this area. For statistical analysis, the Statistical Product Service Solutions (SPSS) 25.0 software (developed by IBM of New York State, New York, NY, USA) was utilized. Utilizing frequencies and percentages, descriptive statistics on the number of articles with efficient interventions were compiled. The χ2 test was utilized to compare between-group differences in the efficacy of the intervention.

3. Results

3.1. Literature-Selection Results

The search strategy retrieved a total yield of 8010 articles. The retrieved articles were imported into EndNote X9 software for de-duplication, and 869 articles were obtained. After further selection of the articles, a total of 49 articles was finally included. Figure 1 illustrates the study-selection process according to the PRISMA 2020 guideline.

3.2. Data-Extraction Results

A total of 49 articles that satisfied the criteria for inclusion in the systematic review were accepted; among those 49 articles, 14 (28.6%) articles discuss acute interventions for typical children and adolescents and 35 (71.4%) articles delve into long-term interventions for typical children and adolescents. A total of 38 (77.6%) of the included studies were RCTs, 2 (4.1%) were RCDs, 8 (16.3%) were non-RCTs, and 1 (2.0%) was a BAS. There were 6079 children and adolescents aged 3 to 18 included in the 65 articles. A total of 34 (69.4%) articles reported the proportion of female subjects, with two [37,38] interventions and one [39] intervention focusing on boys and girls only, whereas the remainder utilized the entire sample, with the proportion of females ranging from 18.8% to 71.0%. The acute intervention lasted between 10 and 50 min. Ten articles (71.4%) reported the intensity of exercise, and the majority (70%) were moderate. The quantitative features of the long-term intervention were 4~36 weeks, 1~7 times/week, and 20~120 min/time. The majority (77.3%) of the 22 articles that addressed the intensity of the exercise were of moderate intensity. Six (12.3%) of the articles mentioned planning, 37 (75.5%) acknowledged inhibitory control, 31 (63.3%) reported working memory, and 21 (42.9%) discussed cognitive flexibility. In addition, the literature on acute interventions included seven [37,40,41,42,43,44,45] multi-arm studies for a total of 22 studies, whereas the literature on long-term interventions contained 13 [46,47,48,49,50,51,52,53,54,55] multi-arm studies, for a total of 48 studies. The characteristics of the included studies are detailed in Table 1.

3.3. Quality-Assessment Results

Randomization procedures were described in 17 (42.5% of all articles) of the 40 randomized trials (RCTs and RCDs), the majority of which involved block randomization, cluster randomization, and stratified randomization. Sixteen articles (40%) reported blinding strategies, primarily subject blinding, assessor blinding, and double blinding. Only one [48] reported an allocation-concealment strategy. In 25 (62.5%) of the articles, complete outcome data were reported. There was no selective reporting of study results in any of the articles, and the existence of other biases was unidentified (Table 2). The quality scores for the nine non-randomized trials (non-RCCTs and BAS) ranged from 17 to 24; the quality score for BAS was 13, indicating relatively high-quality included literature. The primary reasons for the lower quality score of the included literature were that endpoint-indicator evaluation was not conducted using blinded methods, the sample size was not estimated, and the proportion of lost follow-up was >5% (Table 3).

3.4. Results of the Systematic Review

3.4.1. Acute-Intervention Effects

In the 18, 16, and 13 studies that examined the effects of acute exercise interventions on inhibitory control, working memory, and cognitive flexibility in typical children and adolescents, respectively, 14 (77.8%), 12 (75.0%), and 7 (53.8%) of those studies, respectively, revealed beneficial effects following the intervention. Inhibitory control and working memory were significantly more responsive to acute interventions than cognitive flexibility. The efficiency of the intervention was not statistically significant between these three indicators (χ2 = 2.333, p = 0.331) (Table 4). In terms of inhibitory control and cognitive flexibility, there was no significant difference between pre-school and post-preschool children (p > 0.05). Nonetheless, the literature on acute interventions for preschool-aged children is scant [58,62], necessitating further research. There were no significant differences in efficiency between intensities (p > 0.05). However, the moderate intensity was more than 75% in terms of inhibitory control, working memory, and cognitive flexibility. In addition, working memory (χ2 = 6.857, p < 0.01) and cognitive flexibility (χ2 = 4.550, p < 0.05) showed significant differences between the intervention efficiency at 30–50 min per session compared to 10–20 min per session. Closed skills demonstrated significantly greater intervention efficiency on inhibitory control than open skills (χ2 = 4.018, p < 0.05). However, the intervention efficiency of closed skills in terms of working memory and cognitive flexibility was marginally inferior to that of open skills. Between continuous and sequential skills, there was no discernible difference in executive function (p > 0.05). There was a significant difference in the efficient intervention rate for inhibitory control by various types of motor skills under the two-dimensional skill-type system (χ2 = 8.839, p < 0.05); among which the most efficient intervention rates were exhibited by open–continuous and closed–sequential skills. The overall efficient intervention rate was highest for open–continuous skills, followed by closed–sequential skills.

3.4.2. Long-Term Intervention Effects

Of the 7, 36, 31, and 20 studies that examined the effects of long-term exercise interventions on planning, inhibitory control, working memory, and cognitive flexibility, respectively, 4 (57.1%), 25 (69.4%), 19 (61.3%), and 12 (60.0%) studies, respectively, revealed beneficial effects following the intervention. Long-term interventions were slightly more effective on inhibitory control and working memory compared to cognitive flexibility and planning. The efficiency of the intervention was not statistically significant between these four indicators (χ2 = 0.841, p = 0.840) (Table 5). There was no significant difference between pre-school and post-preschool children (p > 0.05). However, the literature on long-term interventions for preschool-aged children is scant [47,65,70,74,80,83], thus necessitating additional research. The efficiency of different intensities of intervention on the executive function of children and adolescents did not differ significantly (p > 0.05). The efficiency of moderate-intensity interventions on inhibitory control, working memory, and cognitive flexibility exceeded 75%. In terms of intervention efficiency, there were no significant differences between acute and long-term interventions for inhibitory control (χ2 = 0.415), working memory (χ2 = 0.883), and cognitive flexibility (χ2 = 0.122) in typical children and adolescents (p > 0.05). Moreover, there were no statistically significant differences in executive function in terms of the intervention period (p > 0.05). At 17 weeks or more, however, the intervention was more efficient for inhibitory control and working memory. There were no significant differences in intervention efficiency between different intervention frequencies for executive function (p > 0.05). Although the efficiency of ≥3 x/week in inhibitory control, working memory, and cognitive flexibility was over 70%, the efficiency of different durations on the executive function of typical children and adolescents was not statistically significant (p > 0.05). However, interventions lasting 30–50 min were more efficient on average. In terms of skill type, open skills were more efficient at intervening in executive function than closed skills, particularly in the dimension of inhibitory control (χ2 = 5.740, p < 0.05). Though there was no statistically significant difference between continuous and sequential skills in terms of the efficiency of executive-function interventions (p > 0.05), sequential skills were more efficient overall. Under the two-dimensional skill-type system, there were significant differences in the intervention efficiency of different types of motor skills on inhibitory control (χ2 = 9.555, p < 0.05). In general, open–sequential skills were the most efficient form of intervention.

4. Discussion

4.1. Overall Effect of Physical Exercise in Real-World Settings

In typical children and adolescents, acute and long-term physical exercise in real-world settings was more than 50% efficient in all aspects of executive function and more efficient in inhibitory control and working memory than in cognitive flexibility and planning. Several researchers [86,87] differentiated executive function into three substructures: inhibitory control, working memory, and cognitive flexibility. However, according to Smith and Jonides [88], executive function extends far beyond these low-level structures and should include highly relevant real-world components such as task management, goal planning, monitoring, and coding. Compared to inhibitory control and working memory, cognitive flexibility is a more complex executive function based on the development and coordination of inhibitory control and working memory [89]. Likewise, planning and problem-solving require the collaborative processing of numerous higher-order cognitive processes. For instance, the Rey Complex Figure Test (RCFT) and tower tasks (e.g., the Tower of Hanoi and the Tower of London) are typically controlled by goal orientation, planning and organization, maintaining goals in working memory, and monitoring behavioral performance [90,91]. Consequently, advancements in the higher structures of executive function are dependent on lower structures. Further improvements in cognitive flexibility and planning are only possible through the efficient development of inhibitory control and working memory. Several studies [41,50,51,54,55,61,62,65,66,70,75] also suggest that the effects of exercise interventions on cognitive flexibility and planning are insignificant in the absence of efficient improvements in inhibitory control or working memory.

4.2. Demographic Variables

There were no significant differences in the effectiveness of the intervention between the pre-school and post-preschool groups. This result contradicts the findings of a prior study on cognitive training, which concluded that younger children had greater potential for training-induced improvement and demonstrated greater training benefits on cognitive tasks [18]. However, executive function is accompanied by staged maturation and development of the prefrontal lobe, with the fastest-developing stages occurring between 0 and 2 years of age, 7 and 9 years of age, and 16 and 19 years of age [92]. Considering the lengthy maturation period of the prefrontal cortex, one study [89] revealed that executive functions may not be fully developed until after the age of 20. According to the findings of a recent meta-analysis, age factors did not have any influence on the facilitative effect of physical activity on executive function [93]. Consequently, the intervention was equally efficient with older adolescent age groups.
Only three articles [37,38,39] involved interventions with children of a single gender, and the complexity and inconsistency of the interventions made it difficult to draw conclusions about gender. Based on the findings of one study [94], short bursts of aerobic exercise at varying intensities exhibited selectively beneficial effects on the executive function that did not differ by gender. However, girls typically display a greater level of executive function and may have less room for growth than boys [18]. In addition, boys are more likely to engage in more intense open-ended activities, which may be more advantageous in terms of executive function. Nevertheless, no comparative studies on the effects of real-world environmental exercise on executive function in different age and gender groups were retrieved.

4.3. Quantitative Characteristics of the Intervention

Acute or long-term interventions of moderate intensity were generally more efficient than those of low or high intensity, similar to the findings of previous studies [41,47] evaluating dose–effect relationships. In accordance with the model of self-control strength, self-control energy is constrained. If the energy expended by previous self-control cannot be recovered in a timely manner, it may result in self-depletion, which will affect future self-control [95]. This study lends support to the self-control strength model, which posits a U-shaped relationship between intervention intensity and executive function. The intervention over 17 weeks has the highest overall intervention efficiency. Short-term interventions can bolster levels of activation in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and boost blood flow to the brain, according to neuroimaging studies [11]. Sustained interventions can enhance the structural plasticity of the brain’s gray and white matter and augment the functionality of its neural networks [10]. Consequently, the latter features a greater intervention effect [12]. In terms of frequency, interventions that occur more frequently than three times per week have a greater impact on executive function overall. A linear dose–effect relationship between exercise frequency and cognitive benefits was discovered [96,97]; that is, the greater the frequency of exercise, the greater the cognitive benefits. Acute interventions lasting 30–50 min were more efficient than those lasting 10–20 min. Interventions lasting 30–50 min were more efficient than those lasting less than 30 min or longer than 50 min. This result is in line with previous research [60,98]. A possible explanation is that the duration of the intervention has an inverted U-shaped dose–effect relationship with executive function, with both longer and shorter interventions likely to diminish cognitive benefits [98]. Specifically, sustained interventions can deplete resources for self-control and result in a decline in cognitive tasks [99].

4.4. Types of Skills of the Intervention

The improvement of executive function benefits from the qualitative characteristics of exercise interventions. First, closed skills were more efficient in acute interventions for inhibitory control and open skills for working memory and cognitive flexibility, whereas open skills were more efficient in long-term interventions. The differential effects of various types of motor skills on brain tissue and neural activation may account for the selective enhancement of executive function by acute interventions. Closed skills, such as jogging, cycling, and aerobics, improve a person’s cardiorespiratory fitness, increase the capillary density of brain tissue, and activate the sensorimotor network involved in the regulation of response inhibition [100]. Open skills such as basketball and table tennis stimulate individual perceptual–motor coordination, expand the amount of Purkinje neurons and synapses, and activate the visuospatial network involved in attention control and working memory [100,101]. Long-term interventions in open skills necessitate not only an abundance of environmental stimulation but also an emphasis on cardiorespiratory fitness [102,103]. They were able to effectively activate sensorimotor and visuospatial networks, induce better neurological remodeling, and lead to greater improvements in executive function.
Second, sequential and continuous skills were equally efficient in the acute intervention, whereas sequential skills were generally more efficient in the long-term intervention. Sequential skills include a more complex movement structure, and the multi-limb involvement in the task necessitates more mental-manipulation processes [102]. This makes it easier to improve executive function than with purely continuous activities. A neuroimaging study [104] also revealed that complex random movements induce neurogenesis in the hippocampus, cerebellum, and cerebral cortex more readily than simple repetitive movements.
Finally, this study discovered that open–continuous and closed–sequential skills were the most efficient interventions in acute intervention, premised on a two-dimensional classification system for motor skills. In the long-term intervention, open-sequential skills were the most efficient interventions. The outcome is similar to the findings of the comparison of the one-dimensional skill-type system. To summarize, not all forms of physical exercise offer the same advantages for executive function [15,105]. Most studies [37,42,50,51,53] indicated that complex motor skills with open and/or sequential characteristics are more beneficial for enhancing executive function in children and adolescents. Krafft et al. [106] also evidenced a favorable intervention effect of aerobics (closed–sequential) on executive function rated by teachers. Based on this result, we should design intervention programs based on selective facilitation of executive function by different types of motor skills. For intervention, it is recommended to combine skills with a diversified and cognitively challenging environment.

4.5. Limitations

First, there is a possibility of bias in the selection of studies, which can compromise the reliability of the findings. Second, despite the overwhelming interest in the effect sizes of exercise interventions, it is a daunting task to test for combined effects due to the high heterogeneity in the evaluation criteria (response time, accuracy, and score) of the measurement instruments used in the studies. Finally, the limitations of primary data have prevented researchers of this paper from investigating the time-course effects of motor-skill interventions on executive function in children and adolescents; hence, it is undetermined how long the cognitive benefits generated by the interventions may be sustained.

5. Conclusions

This systematic review demonstrates the beneficial effects of physical exercise on executive function in children and adolescents, both short-term and long-term, in real-world settings. Exercise interventions exhibit comparable effects in pre-school and post-preschool age groups. The more efficient acute interventions are moderately intense and last 30–50 min. Interventions of moderate intensity that last 30–50 min at least three times a week for 17 weeks or more are more effective. In addition, for acute interventions, closed skills are more efficient for inhibitory control, open skills are more efficient for working memory and cognitive flexibility, continuous and sequential skills were similarly efficient, and open–continuous and closed–sequential skills are the most efficient. Long-term interventions with open skills, sequential skills, and open-sequential skills are more effective. The above results provide a basis for the development of well-designed interventions that can contribute to effective practice in school exercise settings.

Author Contributions

P.S. wrote the article and reviewed the relevant literature. Y.T. designed this study. X.F., Z.Z. and C.L. proofread the manuscript and searched for relevant literature. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was supported by The Youth Humanities and Social Sciences Project of the Liaoning Provincial Department of Education (WQ2020012).

Institutional Review Board Statement

Not applicable.

Informed Consent Statement

Not applicable.

Data Availability Statement

No data were used to support this study.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Friedman, N.P.; Robbins, T.W. The role of prefrontal cortex in cognitive control and executive function. Neuropsychopharmacology 2022, 47, 72–89. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Ferguson, H.J.; Brunsdon, V.E.A.; Bradford, E.E.F. The developmental trajectories of executive function from adolescence to old age. Sci. Rep. 2021, 11, 1–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Baddeley, A. Exploring the central executive. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. Sect. A 1996, 49, 5–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Hofmann, W.; Schmeichel, B.J.; Baddeley, A.D. Executive functions and self-regulation. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2012, 16, 174–180. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Baggetta, P.; Alexander, P.A. Conceptualization and operationalization of executive function. Mind Brain Educ. 2016, 10, 10–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Montoya, M.F.; Susperreguy, M.I.; Dinarte, L.; Morrison, F.J.; San Martin, E.; Rojas-Barahona, C.A.; Förster, C.E. Executive function in Chilean preschool children: Do short-term memory, working memory, and response inhibition contribute differentially to early academic skills? Early Child. Res. Q. 2019, 46, 187–200. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Allan, J.L.; McMinn, D.; Daly, M. A bidirectional relationship between executive function and health behavior: Evidence, implications, and future directions. Front. Neurosci. 2016, 10, 386–398. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  8. Liu, C.; Dai, J.; Chen, Y.; Qi, Z.; Xin, F.; Zhuang, Q.; Zhou, X.; Zhou, F.; Luo, L.; Huang, Y.; et al. Disorder-and emotional context-specific neurofunctional alterations during inhibitory control in generalized anxiety and major depressive disorder. Neuro Image Clin. 2021, 30, 102661–102668. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Thomas, A.G.; Dennis, A.; Bandettini, P.A.; Johansen-Berg, H. The effects of aerobic activity on brain structure. Front. Psychol. 2012, 3, 86–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  10. Berchtold, N.C.; Castello, N.; Cotman, C.W. Exercise and time-dependent benefits to learning and memory. Neuroscience 2010, 167, 588–597. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Hillman, C.H.; Logan, N.E.; Shigeta, T.T. A review of acute physical activity effects on brain and cognition in children. Transl. J. Am. Coll. Sport. Med. 2019, 4, 132–136. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Weinstein, A.M.; Voss, M.W.; Prakash, R.S.; Chaddock, L.; Szabo, A.; White, S.M.; Wojcicki, T.; Mailey, E.; McAuley, E.; Kramer, A.F.; et al. The association between aerobic fitness and executive function is mediated by prefrontal cortex volume. Brain Behav. Immun. 2012, 26, 811–819. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  13. Xiong, X.; Zhu, L.N.; Dong, X.X.; Wang, W.; Yan, J.; Chen, A.G. Aerobic exercise intervention alters executive function and white matter integrity in deaf children: A randomized controlled study. Neural Plast. 2018, 30, 3735208. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  14. Chaddock-Heyman, L.; Erickson, K.I.; Voss, M.W.; Knecht, A.M.; Pontifex, M.B.; Castelli, D.M.; Charles, H.H.; Kramer, A.F. The effects of physical activity on functional MRI activation associated with cognitive control in children: A randomized controlled intervention. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2013, 7, 72–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  15. Meijer, A.; Königs, M.; Pouwels, P.J.; Smith, J.; Visscher, C.; Bosker, R.J.; Hartman, E.; Oosterlaan, J. Effects of aerobic versus cognitively demanding exercise interventions on brain structure and function in healthy children—Results from a cluster randomized controlled trial. Psychophysiology 2022, 3, e14034. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Ludyga, S.; Gerber, M.; Kamijo, K. Exercise types and working memory components during development. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2022, 26, 191–203. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Chai, J.; Lin, J.B. Changing Law of Primary and Secondary School Students’ Interest in Sports Events. J. Shenyang Sport Univ. 2018, 37, 80–88. [Google Scholar]
  18. Zhao, X.; Chen, L.; Fu, L.; Maes, J.H. “Wesley says”: A children’s response inhibition playground training game yields preliminary evidence of transfer effects. Front. Psychol. 2015, 6, 207–213. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  19. Vazou, S.; Pesce, C.; Lakes, K.; Smiley-Oyen, A. More than one road leads to Rome: A narrative review and meta-analysis of physical activity intervention effects on cognition in youth. Int. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 2019, 17, 153–178. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Pesce, C.; Croce, R.; Ben-Soussan, T.D.; Vazou, S.; McCullick, B.; Tomporowski, P.D.; Horvat, M. Variability of practice as an interface between motor and cognitive development. Int. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 2019, 17, 133–152. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Diamond, A.; Ling, D.S. Conclusions about interventions, programs, and approaches for improving executive functions that appear justified and those that, despite much hype, do not. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 2016, 18, 34–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  22. Zhang, Y.B. Athletic Skill Study Theory and Practice; Higher Education Press: Beijing, China, 2012; pp. 4–12. [Google Scholar]
  23. Zhu, H.; Chen, A.; Guo, W.; Zhu, F.; Wang, B. Which type of exercise is more beneficial for cognitive function? A meta-analysis of the effects of open-skill exercise versus closed-skill exercise among children, adults, and elderly populations. Appl. Sci. 2020, 10, 2737. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  24. Tomporowski, P.D.; Pesce, C. Exercise, sports, and performance arts benefit cognition via a common process. Psychol. Bull. 2019, 145, 929–951. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Sale, A.; Berardi, N.; Maffei, L. Enrich the environment to empower the brain. Trends Neurosci. 2009, 32, 233–239. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  26. Chueh, T.Y.; Huang, C.J.; Hsieh, S.S.; Chen, K.F.; Chang, Y.K.; Hung, T.M. Sports training enhances visuo-spatial cognition regardless of open-closed typology. PeerJ 2017, 5, e3336. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  27. Becker, D.R.; McClelland, M.M.; Geldhof, G.J.; Gunter, K.B.; MacDonald, M. Open-skilled sport, sport intensity, executive function, and academic achievement in grade school children. Early Educ. Dev. 2018, 29, 939–955. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Wheat, J.S.; Glazier, P.S. Chapter 9—Measuring coordination and variability in coordination. In Movement System Variability; Human Kinetics: Champaign, IL, USA, 2006; pp. 167–181. [Google Scholar]
  29. Rigoli, D.; Piek, J.P.; Kane, R.; Oosterlaan, J. An examination of the relationship between motor coordination and executive functions in adolescents. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 2012, 54, 1025–1031. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  30. Michel, E.; Molitor, S.; Schneider, W. Motor coordination and executive functions as early predictors of reading and spelling acquisition. Dev. Neuropsychol. 2019, 44, 282–295. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Bernardi, M.; Leonard, H.C.; Hill, E.L.; Botting, N.; Henry, L.A. Executive functions in children with developmental coordination disorder: A 2-year follow-up study. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 2018, 60, 306–313. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  32. Schmidt, R.A.; Wrisberg, C.A. Motor Learning and Performance: A Situation-Based Learning Approach; Human Kinetics: Champaign, IL, USA, 2008; pp. 17–18. [Google Scholar]
  33. Voss, M.W.; Kramer, A.F.; Basak, C.; Prakash, R.S.; Roberts, B. Are expert athletes ‘expert’in the cognitive laboratory? A meta-analytic review of cognition and sport expertise. Appl. Cogn. Psychol. 2010, 24, 812–826. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Costantino, G.; Montano, N.; Casazza, G. When should we change our clinical practice based on the results of a clinical study? Searching for evidence: PICOS and PubMed. Intern. Emerg. Med. 2015, 10, 525–527. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Cumpston, M.; Li, T.; Page, M.J.; Chandler, J.; Welch, V.A.; Higgins, J.P.; Thomas, J. Updated guidance for trusted systematic reviews: A new edition of the Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions. Cochrane Database Syst. Rev. 2019, 10, 14651858. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  36. Slim, K.; Nini, E.; Forestier, D.; Kwiatkowski, F.; Panis, Y.; Chipponi, J. Methodological index for non-randomized studies (MINORS): Development and validation of a new instrument. ANZ J. Surg. 2003, 73, 712–716. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. O’Brien, J.; Ottoboni, G.; Tessari, A.; Setti, A. Multisensory Perception, Verbal, Visuo-spatial and motor working memory modulation after a single open-or closed-skill exercise session in children. J. Cogn. Enhanc. 2021, 5, 141–154. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Alesi, M.; Bianco, A.; Luppina, G.; Palma, A.; Pepi, A. Improving children’s coordinative skills and executive functions: The effects of a football exercise program. Percept. Mot. Ski. 2016, 122, 27–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Manjunath, N.K.; Telles, S. Improved performance in the Tower of London test following yoga. Indian J. Physiol. Pharmacol. 2001, 45, 351–354. [Google Scholar]
  40. Yan, J.; Wang, Y.; Chen, A.G.; Ma, D.J. Empirical study of the impact of various school-term physical activity of moderate intensity on the executive function of children in their preadolescence. J. Sport. Sci. 2014, 35, 94–100. [Google Scholar]
  41. Chen, A.G.; Zhao, L.; Li, H.Y.; Yan, J.; Yin, H.C. Effects of acute basketball dribbling training of different intensity on executive function of primary students. J. TUS 2014, 29, 352–355. [Google Scholar]
  42. Chen, A.G.; Zhao, Z.Y.; Yan, J. Effects of rope skipping with different forms of organization on the executive function of preadolescent children: A school-based experimental study. Chin. J. Sport. Med. 2015, 34, 886–890. [Google Scholar]
  43. Jäger, K.; Schmidt, M.; Conzelmann, A.; Roebers, C.M. The effects of qualitatively different acute physical activity interventions in real-world settings on executive functions in preadolescent children. Ment. Health Phys. Act. 2015, 9, 1–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Gallotta, M.C.; Emerenziani, G.P.; Franciosi, E.; Meucci, M.; Guidetti, L.; Baldari, C. Acute physical activity and delayed attention in primary school students. Scand. J. Med. Sci. Sport. 2015, 25, 331–338. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  45. Ottoboni, G.; Ceciliani, A.; Tessari, A. The effect of structured exercise on short-term memory subsystems: New insight on training activities. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18, 7545–7555. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  46. Davis, C.L.; Tomporowski, P.D.; McDowell, J.E.; Austin, B.P.; Miller, P.H.; Yanasak, N.E.; Allison, J.D.; Naglieri, J.A. Exercise improves executive function and achievement and alters brain activation in overweight children: A randomized, controlled trial. Health Psychol. 2011, 30, 91–97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  47. Chang, Y.K.; Tsai, Y.J.; Chen, T.T.; Hung, T.M. The impacts of coordinative exercise on executive function in kindergarten children: An ERP study. Exp. Brain Res. 2013, 225, 187–196. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  48. Telles, S.; Singh, N.; Bhardwaj, A.K.; Kumar, A.; Balkrishna, A. Effect of yoga or physical exercise on physical, cognitive and emotional measures in children: A randomized controlled trial. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry Ment. Health 2013, 7, 37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Yin, H.C.; Chen, A.G.; Ma, Z.; Li, X.N.; Liu, M. A follow-up study on two kinds of exercise intervention programs for children’s executive functions. China Sport Sci. 2014, 34, 24–28. [Google Scholar]
  50. Yin, H.C.; Li, X.N.; Chen, A.G.; Song, X.Q.; Du, Y.; Pan, J.L.; Wang, C. Experimental study on the effects of five kinds of exercise intervention programs on brain executive functions of primary students. J. TUS 2015, 30, 7–10. [Google Scholar]
  51. Schmidt, M.; Jäger, K.; Egger, F.; Roebers, C.M.; Conzelmann, A. Cognitively engaging chronic physical activity, but not aerobic exercise, affects executive functions in primary school children: A group-randomized controlled trial. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 2015, 37, 575–591. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Gallotta, M.C.; Emerenziani, G.P.; Iazzoni, S.; Meucci, M.; Baldari, C.; Guidetti, L. Impacts of coordinative training on normal weight and overweight/obese children’s attentional performance. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2015, 9, 577–595. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  53. Koutsandreou, F.; Wegner, M.; Niemann, C.; Budde, H. Effects of Motor versus Cardiovascular Exercise Training on Children’s Working Memory. Med. Sci. Sport. Exerc. 2016, 48, 1144–1152. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Oppici, L.; Rudd, J.R.; Buszard, T.; Spittle, S. Efficacy of a 7-week dance (RCT) PE curriculum with different teaching pedagogies and levels of cognitive challenge to improve working memory capacity and motor competence in 8–10 years old children. Psychol. Sport Exerc. 2020, 50, 101675–101723. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Meijer, A.; Königs, M.; van der Fels, I.M.; Visscher, C.; Bosker, R.J.; Hartman, E.; Oosterlaan, J. The effects of aerobic versus cognitively demanding exercise interventions on executive functioning in school-aged children: A cluster-randomized controlled trial. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 2020, 43, 1–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Budde, H.; Voelcker-Rehage, C.; Pietraßyk-Kendziorra, S.; Ribeiro, P.; Tidow, G. Acute coordinative exercise improves attentional performance in adolescents. Neurosci. Lett. 2008, 441, 219–223. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Niemann, C.; Wegner, M.; Voelcker-Rehage, C.; Holzweg, M.; Arafat, A.M.; Budde, H. Influence of acute and chronic physical activity on cognitive performance and saliva testosterone in preadolescent school children. Ment. Health Phys. Act. 2013, 6, 197–204. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Palmer, K.K.; Miller, M.W.; Robinson, L.E. Acute exercise enhances preschoolers’ ability to sustain attention. J. Sport. Exerc. Psychol. 2013, 35, 433–437. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Chen, A.G.; Yan, J.; Yin, H.C.; Pan, C.Y.; Chang, Y.K. Effects of acute aerobic exercise on multiple aspects of executive function in preadolescent children. Psychol. Sport Exerc. 2014, 15, 627–636. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Chen, A.G.; Feng, L.; Zhu, L.N.; Yan, J. Effect of moderate intensity basketball dribbling teaining of different durations on children’ executive function. J. Cap. Univ. Phys. Educ. Sport. 2015, 27, 223–227. [Google Scholar]
  61. Cooper, S.B.; Bandelow, S.; Nute, M.L.; Dring, K.J.; Stannard, R.L.; Morris, J.G.; Nevill, M.E. Sprint-based exercise and cognitive function in adolescents. Prev. Med. Rep. 2016, 4, 155–161. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Stein, M.; Auerswald, M.; Ebersbach, M. Relationships between motor and executive functions and the effect of an acute coordinative intervention on executive functions in kindergartners. Front. Psychol. 2017, 8, 859–872. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  63. Lakes, K.D.; Hoyt, W.T. Promoting self-regulation through school-based martial arts training. J. Appl. Dev. Psychol. 2004, 25, 283–302. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Kamijo, K.; Pontifex, M.B.; O’Leary, K.C.; Scudder, M.R.; Wu, C.T.; Castelli, D.M.; Hillman, C.H. The effects of an afterschool physical activity program on working memory in preadolescent children. Dev. Sci. 2011, 14, 1046–1058. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  65. Fisher, A.; Boyle, J.M.; Paton, J.Y.; Tomporowski, P.; Watson, C.; McColl, J.H.; Reilly, J.J. Effects of a physical education intervention on cognitive function in young children: Randomized controlled pilot study. BMC Pediatr. 2011, 11, 97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  66. Lakes, K.D.; Bryars, T.; Sirisinahal, S.; Salim, N.; Arastoo, S.; Emmerson, N.; Kang, D.; Shim, L.; Wong, D.; Kang, C.J. The healthy for life taekwondo pilot study: A preliminary evaluation of effects on executive function and BMI, feasibility, and acceptability. Ment. Health Phys. Act. 2013, 6, 181–188. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  67. Crova, C.; Struzzolino, I.; Marchetti, R.; Masci, I.; Vannozzi, G.; Forte, R.; Pesce, C. Cognitively challenging physical activity benefits executive function in overweight children. J. Sport. Sci. 2014, 32, 201–211. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  68. Hillman, C.H.; Pontifex, M.B.; Castelli, D.M.; Khan, N.A.; Raine, L.B.; Scudder, M.R.; Drolette, E.S.; Moore, R.D.; Wu, C.T.; Kamijo, K. Effects of the FITKids randomized controlled trial on executive control and brain function. Pediatrics 2014, 134, 1063–1071. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  69. Krafft, C.E.; Schwarz, N.F.; Chi, L.; Weinberger, A.L.; Schaeffer, D.J.; Pierce, J.E.; Rodrigue, A.L.; Yanasak, N.E.; Miller, P.H.; Tomporowski, P.D.; et al. An 8-month randomized controlled exercise trial alters brain activation during cognitive tasks in overweight children. Obesity 2014, 22, 232–242. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  70. Jiang, D.L.; Zeng, C.Z. The effect of 8-week soccer exercise with moderate intensity on executive function in preschool children. China Sport Sci. Technol. 2015, 51, 43–50. [Google Scholar]
  71. Martín-Martínez, I.; Chirosa-Ríos, L.J.; Reigal-Garrido, R.E.; Hernández-Mendo, A.; Juárez-Ruiz-de-Mier, R.; Guisado-Barrilao, R. Efectos de la actividad física sobre las funciones ejecutivas en una muestra de adolescentes. An. Psicol. 2015, 31, 962–971. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  72. Chen, A.G.; Liang, H.Y.; Yan, J.; Yin, H.C. The developmental features of executive functions of left-at-home children and the interventions by mind-body exercise. Chin. J. Spec. Educ. 2016, 32, 69–74. [Google Scholar]
  73. Pesce, C.; Masci, I.; Marchetti, R.; Vazou, S.; Sääkslahti, A.; Tomporowski, P.D. Deliberate play and preparation jointly benefit motor and cognitive development: Mediated and moderated effects. Front. Psychol. 2016, 7, 349–356. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  74. Robinson, L.E.; Palmer, K.K.; Bub, K.L. Effect of the children’s health activity motor program on motor skills and self-regulation in head start preschoolers:an efficacy trial. Front. Public Health 2016, 4, 173–181. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  75. Van der Niet, A.G.; Smith, J.; Oosterlaan, J.; Scherder, E.J.; Hartman, E.; Visscher, C. Effects of a cognitively demanding aerobic intervention during recess on children’s physical fitness and executive functioning. Pediatr. Exerc. Sci. 2016, 28, 64–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  76. Chen, A.G.; Chen, L.P.; Yan, J. Exeperimental study on the effect of eight-week football program on executive function among left-behind children. J. Shandong Sport Univ. 2017, 33, 85–89. [Google Scholar]
  77. Cho, S.Y.; So, W.Y.; Roh, H.T. The effects of taekwondo training on peripheral neuroplasticity-related growth factors, cerebral blood flow velocity, and cognitive functions in healthy children: A randomized controlled trial. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2017, 14, 454. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  78. Xiong, S.; Li, X.; Tao, K. Effects of structured physical activity program on Chinese young children’s executive functions and perceived physical competence in a day care center. BioMed Res. Int. 2017, 23, 5635070. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  79. Hsieh, S.S.; Lin, C.C.; Chang, Y.K.; Huang, C.J.; Hung, T.M.; City, T. Effects of childhood gymnastics program on spatial working memory. Med. Sci. Sport. Exerc. 2017, 49, 2537–2547. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  80. Mulvey, K.L.; Taunton, S.; Pennell, A.; Brian, A. Head, toes, knees, SKIP! Improving preschool children’s executive function through a motor competence intervention. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 2018, 40, 233–239. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  81. Lo, W.L.A.; Liang, Z.; Li, W.; Luo, S.; Zou, Z.; Chen, S.; Yu, Q. The effect of judo training on set-shifting in school children. BioMed Res. Int. 2019, 14, 2572016. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  82. Dai, C. Effects of soccer exercise and stop practice on executive function of school age children. J. Chengdu Sport Univ. 2020, 46, 109–113. [Google Scholar]
  83. Lai, Y.; Wang, Z.; Yue, G.H.; Jiang, C. Determining whether tennis benefits the updating function in young children: A functional near-Infrared spectroscopy study. Appl. Sci. 2020, 10, 407. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  84. Chou, C.C.; Chen, K.C.; Huang, M.Y.; Tu, H.Y.; Huang, C.J. Can movement games enhance executive function in overweight children? A randomized controlled trial. J. Teach. Phys. Educ. 2020, 39, 527–535. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  85. Ma, Y.D.; Xu, L.; Fu, Q. An empirical study of the impact of campus football training on children’s mental health. J. Shenyang Sport Univ. 2022, 41, 58–66. [Google Scholar]
  86. Pennington, B.F.; Ozonoff, S. Executive functions and developmental psychopathology. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 1996, 37, 51–87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  87. Miyake, A.; Friedman, N.P.; Emerson, M.J.; Witzki, A.H.; Howerter, A.; Wager, T.D. The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cogn. Psychol. 2000, 41, 49–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  88. Smith, E.E.; Jonides, J. Storage and executive processes in the frontal lobes. Science 1999, 283, 1657–1661. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  89. Diamond, A. Executive functions. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2013, 64, 135–168. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  90. Anderson, P.; Anderson, V.; Garth, J. Assessment and development of organizational ability: The Rey complex figure organizational strategy score (RCF-OSS). Clin. Neuropsychol. 2001, 15, 81–94. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  91. Chang, Y.K.; Tsai, C.L.; Hung, T.M.; So, E.C.; Chen, F.T.; Etnier, J.L. Effects of acute exercise on executive function: A study with a Tower of London Task. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 2011, 33, 847–865. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  92. Anderson, V.; Northam, E.; Wrennall, J. Developmental Neuropsychology: A Clinical Approach; Routledge: New York, NY, USA, 2018; pp. 88–92. [Google Scholar]
  93. Wang, J.F.; Qi, C.Z.; Wei, X.N. Effects of physical activity on executive function: A meta-analysis. J. Cap. Univ. Phys. Educ. Sport. 2019, 31, 375–384. [Google Scholar]
  94. Chen, A.G.; Yin, H.C.; Yan, J.; Yang, Y. Effects of acute aerobic exercise of different intensity on executive function. Acta Psychol. Sin. 2011, 43, 1055–1062. [Google Scholar]
  95. Baumeister, R.F.; Vohs, K.D.; Tice, D.M. The strength model of self-control. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2007, 16, 351–355. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  96. Larson, E.B.; Wang, L.I.; Bowen, J.D.; McCormick, W.C.; Teri, L.; Crane, P.; Kukull, W. Exercise is associated with reduced risk for incident dementia among persons 65 years of age and older. Ann. Intern. Med. 2006, 144, 73–81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  97. De Souto Barreto, P.; Delrieu, J.; Andrieu, S.; Vellas, B.; Rolland, Y. Physical activity and cognitive function in middle-aged and older adults: An analysis of 104,909 people from 20 countries. Mayo Clin. Proc. 2016, 91, 1515–1524. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  98. Chang, Y.K.; Chu, C.H.; Wang, C.C.; Wang, Y.C.; Song, T.F.; Tsai, C.L.; Etnier, J.L. Dose-response relation between exercise duration and cognition. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 2015, 47, 159–165. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  99. Audiffren, M.; André, N. The strength model of self-control revisited: Linking acute and chronic effects of exercise on executive functions. J. Sport Health Sci. 2015, 4, 30–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  100. Voelcker-Rehage, C.; Godde, B.; Staudinger, U.M. Cardiovascular and coordination training differentially improve cognitive performance and neural processing in older adults. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2011, 5, 26–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  101. Teitelbaum, P.; Teitelbaum, O. FMRI study: Visuospatial processing and the function of the prefrontal-parietal network in autism-type spectrum disorders. Clin. Med. Eng. 2007, 12, 44–45. [Google Scholar]
  102. Mavilidi, M.F.; Okely, A.D.; Chandler, P.; Cliff, D.P.; Paas, F. Effects of integrated physical exercises and gestures on preschool children’s foreign language vocabulary learning. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 2015, 27, 413–426. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  103. Moreau, D.; Morrison, A.B.; Conway, A.R.A. An ecological approach to cognitive enhancement: Complex motor training. Acta Psychol. 2015, 157, 44–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  104. Carey, J.R.; Bhatt, E.; Nagpal, A. Neuroplasticity promoted by task complexity. Exerc. Sport Sci. Rev. 2005, 33, 24–31. [Google Scholar]
  105. Diamond, A. Effects of physical exercise on executive functions: Going beyond simply moving to moving with thought. Ann. Sport. Med. Res. 2015, 2, 1011–1106. [Google Scholar]
  106. Krafft, C.E.; Schaeffer, D.J.; Schwarz, N.F.; Chi, L.; Weinberger, A.L.; Pierce, J.E.; Rodrigue, A.L.; Allison, J.D.; Yanasak, N.E.; Liu, T.; et al. Improved frontoparietal white matter integrity in overweight children is associated with attendance at an after-school exercise program. Dev. Neurosci. 2014, 36, 1–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Figure 1. Literature-selection process according to the PRISMA 2020 guideline.
Figure 1. Literature-selection process according to the PRISMA 2020 guideline.
Brainsci 12 01734 g001
Table 1. Characteristics of included studies.
Table 1. Characteristics of included studies.
Included Article
Study Design
Patients (N/Age/F%)Interventions and ControlsOutcome
Outcome MeasuresResults
Budde et al. [56], 2008
RCT
E = 47/15.04 ± 0.87y/22.9%
C = 52/15.04 ± 0.87y/15.4%
10 min moderate-intensity soccer exercise (O, S) (E) vs. general activity (C)②d2-test+
Niemann et al. [57], 2013
RCT
E = 27/9.7 ± 0.4y/NC
C = 15/9.7 ± 0.5y/NC
12 min high-intensity (85–90% HRmax) track-and-field run (Cl, Co) (E) vs. sitting (C)②d2-test+
Palmer et al. [58], 2013
RCD
16/49.4 ± 5.3 m/18.8%30 min of passing, dribbling, and throwing activities (O, S) (E) vs. sitting (C)②PDTP0
Yan et al. [40], 2014
RCT
E1 = 52/9.8 ± 0.3y/53.8%
E2 = 51/9.7 ± 0.3y/49.0%
C = 51/9.8 ± 0.3y/49.0%
30 min moderate-intensity (60–69% HRmax) aerobics (Cl, S) (E1) vs. obstacle run (O, Co) (E2) vs. sitting (C)②Flanker (E1 > E2)
③1-back (E1 > E2)
④More-odd shifting (E2 > E1)
+&
+&
+&
Chen et al. [41], 2014a
RCT
E1 = 30/9.8 ± 0.3y/50.0%
E2 = 30/9.8 ± 0.3y/53.3%
E3 = 32/9.7 ± 0.3y/46.9%
C = 28/9.8 ± 0.3y/50.0%
30 min low-intensity (50–59% HRmax) basketball high dribbling and dribbling between runs (Cl, S) (E1) vs. moderate intensity (60–69% HRmax) (E2) vs. high intensity (70–79% HRmax) (E3) vs. free activities in their classroom (C)②Flanker (E2 > E1 = E3 > C)
③1-back (E2 = E3 > E1 = C)
④More-odd shifting (E2 > E3 = C > E1)
+&
+0&
+−0&
Chen et al. [59], 2014b
RCT
E = 44/3~5g/47.7%
C = 38/3~5g/55.3%
30 min moderate-intensity (60–70% HRmax) track-and-field run (Cl, Co) (E) vs. sedentary reading (C)②Flanker
③2-back
④More-odd shifting
+
+
+
Chen et al. [42], 2015a
RCT
E1 = 39/9.1 ± 0.3y/48.7%
E2 = 38/9.1 ± 0.3y/44.7%
C = 38/9.2 ± 0.4y/77.7%
30 min moderate-intensity (60–69% HRmax) cooperative rope skipping (O, Co) (E1) vs. single rope skipping (Cl, Co) (E2) vs. sedentary reading (C)②Flanker (E1 > E2)
③1-back (E1 > E2)
④More-odd shifting (E1 > E2)
+&
+&
+&
Chen et al. [60], 2015b
RCT
E = 24/9.5 ± 0.3y/NC
C = 22/9.5 ± 0.3y/NC
30 min moderate-intensity (60–69% HRmax) basketball high dribbling and dribbling between runs (Cl, S) (E) vs. free activities in their classroom (C)②Flanker (E1 > C)
③1-back (E1 > C)
④More-odd shifting (E1 > C)
+
+
+
Jäger et al. [43], 2015
RCT
E1 = 54/134.6 ± 6.6 m/64.8%
E2 = 62/135.3 ± 6.5 m/45.2%
C = 58/135.8 ± 6.3 m/56.9%
20 min moderate-intensity (70% HRmax) cognitive-involvement skill games (O, S) (E1) vs. aerobic exercise without cognitive involvement (Cl, Co) (E2) vs. sitting without cognitive involvement (C)②Flanker (E1 = E2)
③1-back (E1 = E2)
④More-odd shifting (E1 = E2)

0&
0&
0&
Gallotta et al. [44], 2015a
RCT
E1 = 31/8~11y/NC
E2 = 46/8~11y/NC
C = 39/8~11y/NC
50 min traditional PE course (brisk walking, jogging, jumping, etc.) (Cl, Co) (E1) vs. basketball-skills acquisition practice (O, S) (E2) vs. basic academic course (C)②d2-test (E1 > C > E2)+−&
Cooper et al. [61], 2016
RCD
44/12.6 ± 0.6/52.3%10 min high-intensity interval sprint in athletics hall (Cl, Co) (E) vs. sitting (C)②Stroop
③Corsi blocks test
④DSST
+
0
0
Stein et al. [62], 2017
RCT
E = 48/72.2 ± 5.2 m/50.0%
C = 53/72.3 ± 6.9 m/52.8%
20 min motor-skill-learning practice based on coordination of both sides of the body (Cl, S) (E) vs. board game (C)②Simon-says task
②Hearts and Flowers task-incongruent block
④Hearts and Flowers task-mixed block
+
0

0
O’Brien et al. [37], 2021
RCT
E1 = 16/7.0 ± 0.5y/0.0%
E2 = 16/6.7 ± 0.1y/0.0%
C = 19/7.0 ± 0.5y/0.0%
30 min open-skills activities such as basketball, football, tennis (O, S) (E1) vs. closed-skills activities such as race, rope skipping, circuit training (Cl, Co) (E2) vs. free activities in their classroom (C)③Backward Digit Span (E1 > E2)
③Corsi blocks test
③Motor span task (E2 > E1)
+&

0
+&
Ottoboni et al. [45], 2021
RCT
125/7~10y/NC30 min high-intensity (170~180 bpm) team ball games (O, S) (E1) vs. agility obstacle run (O, Co) (E2) vs. basic academic course (C)③Digit Span (E1 > E2)
③Corsi blocks test (E1 > E2)
+&
+&
Manjunath et al. [39], 2001
RCT
E = 10/10~13y/100.0%
C = 10/10~13y/100.0%
4 weeks (7 x/week) yoga intervention (Cl, S), 75 min/time (E) vs. traditional PE course (C)①Tower of London+
Lakes et al. [63], 2004
RCT
207/Kindergarten to Primary 5/NC12 weeks (2–3 x/week) martial-arts intervention (Cl, S), 45 min/time (E) vs. traditional PE course (C)③Digit Span0
Davis et al. [46], 2011
RCT
E1 = 55/7~11y/NC
E2 = 56/7~11y/NC
C = 60/7~11y/NC
13 weeks (7 x/week) moderate-intensity (>150 bpm) running games, rope skipping, football and basketball exercise intervention, 20 min/time (E1) vs. 40 min/time (E2) vs. blank control (C)①Cognitive Assessment System-Planning (E2 > E1 = C)+0&
Kamijo et al. [64], 2011
RCT
E = 20/8.9 ± 0.5y/55.0%
C = 16/9.1 ± 0.6y/50.0%
36 weeks (7 x/week) moderate- to high-intensity physical training combined with dribbling-skills practice, 70 min/time (E) vs. blank control (C)③Sternberg+
Fisher et al. [65], 2011
RCT
E = 34/6.1 ± 0.3y/53.0%
C = 30/6.2 ± 0.3y/58.0%
10 weeks aerobic program, 1~2 h/week (E) vs. traditional PE course (C)①Cognitive Assessment System-Planning
②Attention Network Test (ANT)
③Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Battery(CANTAB)-Spatial working memory
0

0

+


Chang et al. [47], 2013
non-RCCT
E1 = 13/7.2 ± 0.3/46.2%
E2 = 13/7.0 ± 0.3/53.9%
8 weeks (2 w/week) low-intensity (40–50% HRmax) football learning practice (O, S), 2 sessions/week, 35 min/time (E1) vs. moderate intensity (60–70% HRmax) (E2) vs. pre-test②Flanker
(E1: Post > Pre; E2; Post > Pre)
+
Lakes et al. [66], 2013
RCT
E = 50/12.2y/52.00%
C = 31/12.3y/48.00%
36 weeks (2 x/week) Taekwondo (Cl, S), 45 min/time (E) vs. traditional PE course (C)②Hearts and Flowers task-incongruent block
④Hearts and Flowers task-mixed block
0

0
Telles et al. [48], 2013
RCT
E1 = 49/10.4 ± 1.2y/30.6%
E2 = 49/10.5 ± 1.3y/46.9%
12 weeks (5 x/week) yoga (Cl, S), 45 min/time (E1) vs. physical exercise such as jogging, sprint running, relay races (Cl, Co) (E2) vs. pre-test②Stroop (E1 < E2)+&
Crova et al. [67], 2014
RCT
E = 37/9.6 ± 0.5y/46.0%
C = 33/9.6 ± 0.5y/54.6%
21 weeks (1 x/week) moderate-intensity (150.5 ± 6.4 bpm) tennis (O, S), 120 min/time (E) vs. traditional PE course (C)②RNG-inhibition of mental routines
③RNG-working memory updating
+

0
Yin et al. [49], 2014
RCT
326/3~5 g/47.9%20 weeks (3 x/week) moderate-intensity (120–140 bpm) martial arts + rope skipping+ 8 word run (Cl), 30 min/time (E1) vs. pattern running (O, Co), 5 x/week (E2) vs. blank control (C)②Flanker (E1 > E2 = C)
③2-back (E2 > E1)
④More-odd shifting (E1 = E2)
+0&
+&
+&
Hillman et al. [68], 2014
RCT
E = 109/8.7~8.9y/49.0%
C = 112/8.7~8.9y/44.0%
36 weeks (7 x/week) moderate-intensity (137.0 ± 68.3 bpm) aerobic exercise intervention, 120 min/time (E) vs. wait control (C)②Flanker
③Switch task
+
+
Krafft et al. [69], 2014
RCT
E = 24/9.7 ± 0.8y/71%
C = 19/9.9 ± 0.9y/58%
32 weeks (7 x/week) moderate intensity (161 ± 9 bpm) rope skipping and tag games, 40 min/time (E) vs. sedentary attention control (C)②Flanker
②Antisaccade task
+
+
Yin et al. [50], 2015
RCT
610/3~5 g/46.9%10 weeks (5 week) 40–80% HRmax pattern running (O, Co), 30 min/time (E1) vs. fun track-and-field games (O, Co), 3 x/week (E2) vs. small handball and physical-fitness exercises, 3 x/week (E3) vs. shuttlecock and games (E4) vs. martial arts, rope skipping, 8-word run (Co), 3 x/week (E5) vs. regular extracurricular physical activity (C)②Flanker
(E5 > E4 > E2 > E1 = E3 = C)
③2-back
(E1 = E5 > E2 = E3 = E4 = C)
④More-odd shifting
(E1 = E5 > E3 > E2 = E4 = C)
+0&

+0&

+0&
Jiang et al. [70], 2015
RCT
E = 31/5~6y/NC
C = 30/5~6y/NC
8 weeks (2 x/week) moderate-intensity (60–70% HRmax) football games (O, S), 35 min/time (E) vs. blank control (C)②Panda-Lion task
②Snow-Grass task
③Corsi blocks test
③Reverse Corsi blocks test
④Flexible Item Selection task
+
+
0
0
0
Schmidt et al. [51], 2015
RCT
E1 = 69/11.3 ± 0.6y/62.3%
E2 = 57/11.3 ± 0.6y/50.9%
C = 55/11.4 ± 0.6y/49.1%
6 weeks (2 x/week) high-intensity soft hockey and basketball games (O, S), 45 min/time (E1) vs. 200 m round-trip run (Cl, Co) (E2) vs. traditional PE course (C)②Flanker (E1 > E2 = C)
③2-back (E1 > E2 = C)
④More-odd shifting (E1 > E2 = C)
+0&
+0&
+0&
Martín-Martínez et al. [71], 2015
non-RCCT
54/15~16y/25.9%8 weeks (2 x/week) moderate-intensity (RPE = 13.36 ± 1.39) group ball (football, basketball, and handball) games (O, S), 30–60 min/time (E) vs. aerobic exercise and modern dance (C)②Stroop
③WISC-IV-digital and letter Span
④Trail Making Test
+
+

+
Gallotta et al. [52], 2015b
RCT
E1 = 56/8~11y/NC
E2 = 59/8~11y/NC
C = 41/8~11y/NC
20 weeks (2 x/week) moderate-intensity (RPE = 5~8) traditional PE course focusing on cardiovascular fitness, 60 min/time (E1) vs. activities focusing on improving coordination and flexibility (E2) vs. blank control (C)②d2-test (E2 > E1)+&
Chen et al. [72], 2016a
RCT
E = 20/11.4 ± 0.6y/NC
C = 20/11.3 ± 06y/NC
8 weeks (3 x/week) moderate-intensity (60–69% HRmax) mind–body aerobics (Cl, S), 40 min/time (E) vs. regular academics (C)②Flanker
③1-back
④More-odd shifting
0
+
+
Koutsandreou et al. [53], 2016
RCT
E1 = 27/9.3 ± 0.6y/NC
E2 = 23/9.4 ± 0.7y/NC
C = 21/9.3 ± 0.6y/NC
10 weeks (3 x/week) moderate-intensity (60–70% HRmax) aerobic exercise, 45 min/time (E1) vs. moderate intensity (55–65% HRmax) skill practice focused on improving coordination (S) (E2) vs. doing their homework (C)③Letter Digit Span (E2 > E1)+&
Alesi et al. [38], 2016
non-RCCT
E = 24/8.8 ± 1.1y/0.0%
C = 20/9.3 ± 0.9y/0.0%
24 weeks (2 x/week) football intervention (O, S), 75 min/time (E) vs. traditional PE course, 1 x/week, 60 min/time (C)①Tower of London
③Forward Digit Span
③Backward Digit Span
③Corsi blocks test
+
0
0
+
Pesce et al. [73], 2016
RCT
E = 232/5~10y/50.4%
C = 228/5~10y/49.6%
24 weeks (1 x/week) moderate intensity (131.9 ± 17.4 bpm) skill games (O, S) focusing on motor coordination and cognitive engagement, 60 min/time (E) vs. traditional PE course (C)②RNG-inhibition of mental routines
③RNG-working memory updating
+

0
Robinson et al. [74], 2016
RCT
E = 68/52.4 ± 5.2 m/39.7%
C = 45/51.6 ± 5.2 m/40.0%
5 weeks (3 x/week) Children’s Health Activity Programme (motor-skills-led intervention), 40 min/time (E) vs. outdoor free play (C)②Delay of gratification snack task+
van der Niet et al. [75], 2016
non-RCCT
E = 53/8.8 ± 0.8y/64.2%
C = 52/8.9 ± 1.2y/38.5%
22 weeks (2 x/week) of moderate- to high-intensity running games, circuit training, and football with cognitive participation, 30 min/time (E) vs. blank control (C)①Tower of London
②Stroop
③Visual Memory Span
③Digit Span
④Trailmaking test
0
+
0
+
0
Chen et al. [76], 2017
RCT
E = 21/9.4 ± 0.5y/47.6%
C = 20/9.2 ± 0.4y/50.0%
8 weeks (2 x/week) moderate-intensity (60–69% HRmax) football intervention (O, S), 40 min/time (E) vs. traditional PE course (C)②Flanker
③1-back
④More-odd shifting
+
+
+
Cho et al. [77], 2017
RCT
E = 15/11.2 ± 0.8y/40.0%
C = 15/11.3 ± 0.7y/40.0%
16 weeks (5 x/week) moderate intensity (RPE = 11~15) taekwondo intervention (O, S), 60 min/time (E) vs. blank control (C)②Stroop+
Xiong et al. [78], 2017
non-RCCT
39/4.67y/48.7%12 weeks (7 x/week) structured motor-skills intervention, 30 min/time (E) vs. unstructured free play (C)①WCST+
Hsieh et al. [79], 2017
non-RCCT
E = 24/8.7 ± 1.1y/NC
C = 20/8.6 ± 1.1y/NC
8 weeks (2 x/week) moderate-intensity (136.4 ± 16.8 bpm) gymnastics intervention (Cl, S), 90 min/time (E) vs. blank control (C)③Delayed matching-to-sample test+
Mulvey et al. [80], 2018
RCT
T = 50/3~6y/NC
C = 57/3~6y/NC
6 weeks (2 x/week) SKIP program, 30 min/time (E) vs. rest as usual (C)②HTKS+
Lo et al. [81], 2019
BAS
E = NC/13.36 ± 1.15y/NC
C = NC/13.47 ± 1.24y/NC
8 weeks judo (3 x/week) (O, S), 60 min/time (E) vs. never trained in judo (C)④spatial task-switching+
Dai et al. [82], 2020
non-RCCT
E = 46/10.5 ± 0.3y/NC
C = 43/10.4 ± 0.3y/NC
24 weeks (5 x/week) moderate-intensity (60~69% HRmax) football intervention (O, S), 120 min/time (E) vs. blank control (C)②Flanker
③2-back
④Salthouse
+
+
+
Lai et al. [83], 2020
RCT
E = 10/5~7y/50.0%
C = 10/5~7y/50.0%
8 weeks (2 x/week) moderate-intensity (60~69% HRmax) tennis intervention (O, S), 60 min/time (E) vs. basic academic course (C)③1-back+
Oppici et al. [54], 2020
RCT
E1 = 30/8.8 ± 0.5y/62.0%
E2 = 30/8.7 ± 0.7y/59.0%
C = 20/8.9 ± 0.7y/63.0%
7 weeks (2 x/week) high-cognitive dance practice (Cl, S), 60 min/time (E1) vs. low-cognitive dance practice (Cl, S) (E2) vs. blank control (C)②Flanker
③List Sorting Working Memory test
④Dimensional Change Card Sort test
0
0

0
Chou et al. [84], 2020
RCT
E = 44/12.3 ± 0.7y/38.6%
C = 40/12.1 ± 0.7y/37.5%
8 weeks (3 x/week) moderate- to high-intensity (60–80% HRmax) running games, jump-rope games, baseball, football, and basketball (O), 40 min/time (E) vs. traditional PE course (C)②Stroop+
Meijer et al. [55], 2021
RCT
E1 = 206/9.3 ± 0.7y/50.5%
E2 = 235/9.0 ± 0.6y/53.6%
C = 415/9.2 ± 0.7y/49.6%
14 weeks (4 x/week) aerobic-exercise intervention (Cl, Co), 30 min/time (E1) vs. team games with cognitive participation (O, S) (E2) vs. traditional PE sessions (C)②Attention Network Test-interference control
②Stop Signal Task
③Digit Span
③Grid Task
0

0
0
0
Ma et al. [85], 2022
non-RCCT
E = 40/9.2 ± 0.3y/NC
C = 40/9.2 ± 0.3y/NC
16 weeks (3 x/week) football intervention (O, S), 40 min/time (E) vs. blank control (C)②GO/NO GO
③1-back
③2-back
④More-odd shifting
+
+
+
+
Abbreviations and notes: RCT: randomized controlled trial; RCD: randomized crossover design; non-RCCT: non-randomized concurrent control trial; BAS: before–after study; E: experimental group; C: control group; y: year; m: month; g: grade; F%: females as a percentage of subjects; O: open skills; Cl: closed skills; Co: continuous skills; S: sequential skills; NC: not clear; HRmax: maximum heart rate; RPE: rate of perceived exertion; ①: planning; ②; inhibitory control; ③: working memory; ④: cognitive flexibility; +: beneficial to experimental group; −: beneficial to control group; 0: no significant difference between the experimental and control groups; &: comparison of intervention results between experimental groups; Pre: pre-test; Post: post-test; PDTP: Picture Deletion Task for Preschoolers; DSST: Digit Symbol Substitution Test; RNG: Random Number Generation task; WCST: Wisconsin Card Sorting Test; WISC-IV: Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Fourth Edition; HTKS: Head Toe Knee Shoulder test.
Table 2. Results of the quality assessment of the included RCTs and RCDs.
Table 2. Results of the quality assessment of the included RCTs and RCDs.
Included ArticlesRandomization MethodsBlindingAllocation ConcealmentCompleteness of Outcome DataSelective Reporting of Study ResultsOther Biases
Budde et al. [56], 2008NCNCNCCompleteNoNC
Niemann et al. [57], 2013NCSubject blindingNCCompleteNoNC
Palmer et al. [58], 2013NCNCNCCompleteNoNC
Yan et al. [40], 2014NCSubject blindingNCCompleteNoNC
Chen et al. [41], 2014aCluster randomizationNCNCCompleteNoNC
Chen et al. [59], 2014bStratified randomizationNCNC5 lost to follow-upNoNC
Chen et al. [42], 2015aCluster randomizationNCNCCompleteNoNC
Chen et al. [60], 2015bNCNCNCCompleteNoNC
Jäger et al. [43], 2015NCAssessor blindingNCCompleteNoNC
Gallotta et al. [44], 2015aCluster randomizationNCNCCompleteNoNC
Cooper et al. [61], 2016NCSubject blindingNCCompleteNoNC
Stein et al. [62], 2017NCNCNCCompleteNoNC
O’Brien et al. [37], 2021NCNCNC5 lost to follow-upNoNC
Ottoboni et al. [45], 2021Stratified randomizationNCNCCompleteNoNC
Manjunath et al. [39], 2001Random number tableNCNCCompleteNoNC
Lakes et al. [63], 2004NCAssessor blindingNC12 lost to follow-upNoNC
Davis et al. [46], 2011Stratified randomizationDouble blindingNCCompleteNoNC
Kamijo et al. [64], 2011NCNCNC7 lost to follow-upNoNC
Fisher et al. [65], 2011Cluster randomizationSubject blindingNC8 lost to follow-upNoNC
Lakes et al. [66], 2013NCAssessor blindingNCCompleteNoNC
Telles et al. [48], 2013Cmputer-generated random numbersAssessor blindingpaper hideCompleteNoNC
Crova et al. [67], 2014Cluster randomizationNCNCCompleteNoNC
Yin et al. [49], 2014NCNCNCCompleteNoNC
Hillman et al. [68], 2014NCSubject blindingNC15 lost to fllow-upNoNC
Krafft et al. [69], 2014NCNCNC12 lost to follow-upNoNC
Yin et al. [50], 2015Stratified randomizationNCNCCompleteNoNC
Jiang et al. [70], 2015NCNCNCCompleteNoNC
Schmidt et al. [51], 2015NCAssessor blindingNC8.6% lost to follow-upNoNC
Gallotta et al. [52], 2015bCluster randomizationNCNCCompleteNoNC
Chen et al. [72], 2016aDrawing lotsDouble blindingNCCompleteNoNC
Koutsandreou et al. [53], 2016NCAssessor blindingNC28 lost to follow-upNoNC
Pesce et al. [73], 2016Stratified randomizationNCNC17 lost to follow-upNoNC
Robinson et al. [74], 2016NCNCNC42.5% lost to follow-upNoNC
Chen et al. [76], 2017Drawing lotsDouble blindingNC7 lost to follow-upNoNC
Cho et al. [77], 2017NCNCNCCompleteNoNC
Mulvey et al. [80], 2018Random number tableNCNCCompleteNoNC
Lai et al. [83], 2020NCDouble blindingNC12 lost to follow-upNoNC
Oppici et al. [54], 2020Stratified randomizationSubject blindingNC2 lost to follow-upNoNC
Chou et al. [84], 2020NCNCNCCompleteNoNC
Meijer et al. [55], 2021Cluster randomizationNoNC35 lost to follw-upNoNC
Abbreviations and notes: NC: not clear.
Table 3. Results of the quality assessment of the included non-RRCTs and BAS.
Table 3. Results of the quality assessment of the included non-RRCTs and BAS.
Included Articles(1)(2)(3)(4)(5)(6)(7)(8)(9)(10)(11)(12)Total
Chang et al. [47], 201322220220221219
Martín-Martínez et al. [71], 201522220220222220
Alesi et al. [38], 201621220220222219
van der Niet et al. [75], 201622220200221217
Xiong et al. [78], 201721220200221216
Hsieh et al. [79], 201722222222222224
Lo et al. [81], 201922220221NANANANA13
Dai et al. [82], 202021220200222217
Ma et al. [85], 202220220210222217
Abbreviations and notes: (1) the study purpose is clearly given; (2) consistency of the included subjects; (3) collection of expected data; (4) the outcome indicators can properly reflect the study purpose; (5) objectivity of outcome index evaluation; (6) whether the follow-up time is sufficient; (7) the rate of lost visits is lower than 5%; (8) whether the sample size has been estimated; (9) whether the control group has been properly selected; (10) whether the control group is synchronized; (11) whether the baseline between groups is comparable; (12) whether the statistical analysis is appropriate; NA: not applicable.
Table 4. Summary of intergroup comparisons of acute-intervention effectiveness.
Table 4. Summary of intergroup comparisons of acute-intervention effectiveness.
CharacteristicInhibitory ControlWorking MemoryCognitive Flexibility
Effective Rate (%)χ2Effective Rate (%)χ2Effective Rate (%)χ2
Total14/18 (77.8) 12/16 (75.0) 7/13 (53.8)
Age
Pre-school1/25 (0.0)1.004 0/1 (0.0)1.264
Post-preschool13/16 (81.3) 11/15 (73.3) 7/12 (58.3)
Intensity
Low1/1 (100.0)1.2310/1 (0.0)2.7030/1 (0.0)5.600
Moderate9/12 (77.0) 7/9 (77.7) 7/9 (77.8)
High3/3 (100.0) 3/4 (75.0) 0/2 (0.0)
Duration
10~20 min3/5 (60.0)2.7140/2 (0.0)6.857 **0/3 (0.0)4.550 *
30~50 min12/13 (92.3) 12/14 (85.7) 7/10 (70.0)
Skill type
Open skills3/6 (50.0)4.018 *5/6 (83.3)0.3562/3 (66.7)0.258
Closed skills11/12 (91.7) 7/10 (70.0) 5/10 (50.0)
Contionous skills7/8 (87.5)0.7886/8 (75.0)0.0004/6 (66.7)0.343
Sequential skills7/10 (70.0) 6/8 (75.0) 3/6 (50.0)
Open–Continous2/2 (100.0)8.839 *3/3 (100.0)1.7782/2 (100.0)2.940
Open–Sequential1/4 (25.0) 2/3 (66.7) 0/1 (0.0%)
Closed–Contionous5/6 (83.3) 3/5 (60.0) 2/4 (50.0)
Closed–Sequential6/6 (100.0) 4/5 (80.0) 3/6 (50.0)
Abbreviations and notes: For the measurement of the same dimension using multiple tools, as long as one positive benefit is achieved, it will be included in the effective intervention; * p < 0.05; ** p < 0.01.
Table 5. Summary of intergroup comparisons of long-term intervention effectiveness.
Table 5. Summary of intergroup comparisons of long-term intervention effectiveness.
CharacteristicPlanningInhibitory ControlWorking MemoryCognitive Flexibility
Effective Rate (%)χ2Effective Rate (%)χ2Effective Rate (%)χ2Effective Rate (%)χ2
Total4/7 (57.1) 25/36 (69.4) 19/31 (61.3) 12/20 (60.0)
Age
Pre-school1/2 (50.0)0.0584/5 (80.0)0.3542/3 (66.7)0.0020/1 (0.0)1.579
Post-preschool3/5 (60.0) 20/30 (66.7) 17/26 (65.4) 12/19 (63.2)
Intensity
Low 1/1 (100.0)1.9200.7621.148
Moderate1/2 (50.0) 13/15 (86.7) 11/14 (78.6) 6/7 (85.7)
High 1/2 (50.0) 1/2 (50.0) 1/2 (50.0)
Period
≤8 weeks1/1 (100.0)0.8758/12 (66.7)1.8335/9 (55.6)2.2455/9 (55.6)0.185
9~16 weeks2/4 (50.0) 7/12 (58.3) 6/12 (50.0) 4/6 (66.7)
≥17 weeks1/2 (50.0) 10/12 (83.3) 8/10 (80.0) 3/5 (60.0)
Frequency
<3 x/week1/1 (100.0)0.6008/12 (66.7)0.2035/12 (41.7)2.8013/8 (37.5)2.036
≥3 x/week3/5 (60.0) 17/23 (73.9) 13/18 (72.2) 9/12 (75.0)
Duration
≤30 min1/3 (33.3)1.5566/10 (60.0)1.1905/10 (50.0)0.8835/8 (62.5)0.173
30~50 min1/1 (100.0) 12/15 (80.0) 7/10 (70.0) 4/7 (57.1)
>50 min2/3 (66.7) 7/10 (70.0) 7/11 (63.6) 2/4 (50.0)
Skill type
Open skills1/1 (100.0)0.00013/16 (81.3)5.740 *9/14 (64.3)1.4738/10 (80.0)3.484
Closed skills1/1 (100.0) 3/9 (33.3) 3/8 (37.5) 2/6 (33.3)
Contionous skills 3/7 (42.9)1.6273/6 (50.0)0.4033/5 (60.0)0.019
Sequential skills2/2 (100.0) 12/17 (70.6) 11/17 (64.7) 7/11 (63.6)
Open–Continous 0.0002/3 (66.7)9.555 *1/3 (33.3)3.3112/3 (66.7)5.526
Open–Sequential1/1 (100.0) 11/12 (91.7) 7/11 (63.6) 6/7 (85.7)
Closed–Contionous 1/3 (33.3) 0/2 (0.0) 0/1 (0.0)
Closed–Sequential1/1 (100.0) 1/5 (20.0) 2/5 (40.0) 1/4 (25.0)
Abbreviations and notes: for the measurement of the same dimension using multiple tools, as long as one positive benefit is achieved, it will be included in the effective intervention; * p < 0.05.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Shi, P.; Tang, Y.; Zhang, Z.; Feng, X.; Li, C. Effect of Physical Exercise in Real-World Settings on Executive Function of Typical Children and Adolescents: A Systematic Review. Brain Sci. 2022, 12, 1734. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci12121734

AMA Style

Shi P, Tang Y, Zhang Z, Feng X, Li C. Effect of Physical Exercise in Real-World Settings on Executive Function of Typical Children and Adolescents: A Systematic Review. Brain Sciences. 2022; 12(12):1734. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci12121734

Chicago/Turabian Style

Shi, Peng, Yan Tang, Ziyun Zhang, Xiaosu Feng, and Chenyang Li. 2022. "Effect of Physical Exercise in Real-World Settings on Executive Function of Typical Children and Adolescents: A Systematic Review" Brain Sciences 12, no. 12: 1734. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci12121734

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop