Next Article in Journal
Repetitive Anodal TDCS to the Frontal Cortex Increases the P300 during Working Memory Processing
Next Article in Special Issue
The Genetics of Intellectual Disability
Previous Article in Journal
The Effect of COVID-19 on Sleep Quality and Mental Health: Adolescents Are More at Risk Than the Elderly
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Review

Wechsler Scale Intelligence Testing in Males with Dystrophinopathies: A Review and Meta-Analysis

by
Pien M. M. Weerkamp
1,2,*,†,
Eva M. Mol
1,3,†,
Dirk J. J. Sweere
1,2,
Debby G. M. Schrans
4,
R. Jeroen Vermeulen
2,5,
Sylvia Klinkenberg
1,2,5,
Petra P. M. Hurks
2,5 and
Jos G. M. Hendriksen
1,2
1
Expert Centre for Neurological and Developmental Learning Disabilities, Kempenhaeghe, Sterkselseweg 65, 5591 VE Heeze, The Netherlands
2
School for Mental Health and Neuroscience, Maastricht University, Universiteitssingel 40, UNS40, 6229 ER Maastricht, The Netherlands
3
Klimmendaal Academy, Klimmendaal Rehabilitation Specialists, Heijenoordseweg 5, 6813 GG Arnhem, The Netherlands
4
Mutsaersstichting, Postweg 88, 5915 HB Venlo, The Netherlands
5
Department of Neurology, Maastricht University Medical Centre, 6229 ER Maastricht, The Netherlands
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
These authors contributed equally to this work.
Brain Sci. 2022, 12(11), 1544; https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci12111544
Submission received: 20 October 2022 / Revised: 9 November 2022 / Accepted: 11 November 2022 / Published: 14 November 2022
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Reviews on Developmental Brain Disorders)

Abstract

:
Background: Intelligence scores in males with Duchenne Muscular Dystrophy (DMD) and Becker Muscular Dystrophy (BMD) remain a major issue in clinical practice. We performed a literature review and meta-analysis to further delineate the intellectual functioning of dystrophinopathies. Method: Published, peer-reviewed articles assessing intelligence, using Wechsler Scales, of males with DMD or BMD were searched from 1960 to 2022. Meta-analysis with random-effects models was conducted, assessing weighted, mean effect sizes of full-scale IQ (FSIQ) scores relative to normative data (Mean = 100, Standard Deviation = 15). Post hoc we analysed differences between performance and verbal intelligence scores. Results: 43 studies were included, reporting data on 1472 males with dystrophinopathies; with FSIQ scores available for 1234 DMD (k = 32) and 101 BMD (k = 7). DMD males score, on average, one standard deviation below average (FSIQ = 84.76) and significantly lower than BMD (FSIQ = 92.11). Compared to a previous meta-analysis published in 2001, we find, on average, significantly higher FSIQ scores in DMD. Conclusion: Males with Duchenne have, on average, significantly lower FSIQ scores than BMD males and the general population. Clinicians must consider lower intelligence in dystrophinopathies to ensure good clinical practice.

1. Introduction

Dystrophinopathies are X-linked recessive progressive neuromuscular disorders caused by mutations in the dystrophin gene. Due to dystrophin deficiency in muscles, patients with dystrophinopathies show progressive muscular weakness, cardiomyopathy, motor delay, and respiratory impairment [1]. The most common and severe type of dystrophinopathy, Duchenne Muscular Dystrophy (DMD), is characterized by a complete disruption of the genetic code in the dystrophin protein and has an estimated birth prevalence of 1 per 3500 to 1 per 9300 males (OMIM: 310,200, ORPHA: 98,896). Boys with DMD usually become wheelchair bound early in the second decade of life [2]. The milder but more variable type of dystrophinopathy, Becker Muscular Dystrophy (BMD), is characterized by a partial disruption of the genetic code in this protein and has a prevalence of 5 per 100,000 males [3]. Motor symptoms in BMD vary from isolated muscle cramps with few effects on ambulation, to being wheelchair bound at the end of the second decade of life [2,3].
Dystrophin can be found in the central nervous system (CNS), muscle cells, and endothelial cells. In the brain, dystrophin is found in regions such as the hippocampus, amygdala, cerebral cortex, prefrontal cortex, and in the Purkinje cells of the cerebellum [4,5,6,7,8]. Its absence is thought to be responsible for the presence of a diversity of neurocognitive symptoms [9] often found in patients with dystrophinopathies [1,5,10,11,12,13,14,15,16,17,18,19]. Previous research has shown an association between the location of the dystrophin gene mutation, affecting specific dystrophin isoforms in the brain (i.e., Dp140 and Dp71), and cognitive impairments. However, the exact phenotype-genotype relationship in both DMD and BMD remains unclear [11,13,20,21,22,23,24]. The study by Cotton, Voudouris, and Greenwood, published in 2001 [25], was the first meta-analysis describing (impaired) intelligence in DMD. Intelligence is the ability to understand and adapt to the environment and to learn from experience [26]. The meta-analysis of Cotton is often used as a key reference in literature on DMD [5,8,12,27,28,29]. It is based on 32 studies published between 1960 and 1999, reporting data on 1224 boys with DMD (mean age = 12.26, SD = 4.06, range 2–27). The data presented are based on scores from eight or more (in some articles, the authors did not explain which instrument(s) is (are) used) different types of intelligence tests (Inc. Stanford-Binet Intelligence Scale, Griffiths Scales, Raven’s Progressive Matrices, Rorsach Developmental Index, Cattel Infant Intelligence Scale, Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test, Wechsler scales, Bayley Scales). Mean full-scale IQ (FSIQ) was 80.20 (SD = 19.3), which is more than one standard deviation below the normal population (M = 100; SD = 15).
In contrast to DMD, less research was conducted on measuring intelligence in BMD boys. A recent review by Ferrero and Rossi (2022) [30] reported on six articles since 1995 on intelligence in BMD. They reported mean FSIQ scores of males with BMD to be (low) average [31,32], where the FSIQ ranged from 82.8 to 95.6. An explanation for this limited number of studies on intelligence in BMD, as opposed to DMD, could be that BMD is characterized by, in general, a milder yet more heterogenous phenotype compared to DMD [3].
In research and clinical practice, Wechsler Intelligence Scales are internationally the most frequently used tests to determine intelligence [33], even in DMD males with motor impairments [34]. The Wechsler Intelligence Scales are based on Spearman’s taxonomy of intelligence (1923, 1927), which argued that there are two factors underlying intelligence test performances: (1) a general intelligence factor (g-factor) that determines an individual’s “general” abilities level and (2) specific cognitive functions (s-factors) that are used to solve specific problems, tasks, and (sub)tests. The current paper aims to review the results of the Wechsler Intelligence Scales administered to males with dystrophinopathies. To our knowledge, this will be the first meta-analysis on Wechsler test scores in BMD.
The main reason to focus on Wechsler Intelligence Scales alone is to ensure consistency of test scores and therefore minimalizing variation of methods/instruments used. For example, some instruments, e.g., the Raven Standard Progressive Matrices, show only moderate correlation with the Wechsler Intelligence Scales (e.g., the correlation between the Raven Standard Progressive Matrices and Wechsler Intelligence Scales is r = 0.55) [35]. Analysing scores from one measure as opposed to comparing scores of different instruments that are, in some cases, not that comparable in what they measure (for instance, they measure different specific cognitive functions), will help uniform data on IQ in DMD and BMD. Therefore, we will re-analyse the data on intelligence test scores—while including studies published until 2022—using only data from studies using the Wechsler Intelligence scales.
Nonetheless, it should be noted that the Wechsler Intelligence scales have been revised and updated with new subtests and new normative data several times, resulting in a variability of versions since the first (adult) version of this test was published in 1955. There is a separate Wechsler Intelligence scales version for adults (Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale: WAIS), children (Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children: WISC), and preschoolers (Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence: WPPSI) (Wechsler, 2012; Wechsler, 2018; Wechsler, 2020) [35,36,37], covering the whole age range from 2½ years into late adulthood (until 90 years). Moreover, the internal structure of Wechsler Intelligence Scales has also been updated as knowledge of intelligence theory, psychometrics, and diagnostics developed [35]. For example, earlier versions of the Wechsler scales provided FSIQ scores as well as the indices verbal IQ (VIQ) and performance IQ (PIQ). While most recent versions of the Wechsler scales (WAIS-IV, WISC-V, and WPPSI-IV) are based on the theoretical model of Cattell, Horn, Carol (CHC) theory, which is based on Spearman’s two-factor theory. Next to FSIQ (g-factor), the WISC-V and WPPSI-IV administer five index scores (s-factors): verbal comprehension, visuospatial functioning, fluid reasoning, working memory, and information processing speed, and the WAIS-IV four factors: verbal comprehension, perceptual reasoning, working memory, and processing speed. Additionally, subtests have been removed and introduced in the updated versions. Despite the variability between the structure of the different Wechsler Intelligence scales, correlations between FSIQ scores of different versions (e.g., WISC-V and WAIS-V (r = 0.89), WISC-III and WISC-V (r = 0.87), WISC-V and WPPSI-III (r = 0.78)) are high, indicating that scores on different versions are strongly related within the general population [36]. Nevertheless, in our meta-analyses, we will take the different versions into account as a possible moderator effect.
Despite the abandonment of the PIQ-VIQ split in the newest versions of the Wechsler scales, we decided to do a tentative analysis of PIQ and VIQ in our sample of studies. Cotton et al. (2001) [25] reported, on average, a discrepancy between VIQ (80.40, SD = 18.44) and PIQ (85.40, SD = 16.90) in DMD, which is in accordance with other research and clinical practice showing more language-related problems and dyslexia in DMD. In BMD, there seems to be no significant mean difference in VIQ and PIQ [11,38]. We aim to give an update on the VIQ-PIQ split in DMD and BMD, as this is a clinically relevant split that is reformulated in the newest versions as verbal comprehension and perceptual organization. VIQ and PIQ in dystrophinopathies are still reported in recent studies [23,24,39].
Despite the current sizable literature (on mainly DMD), intelligence scores in dystrophinopathies are still a matter of debate. The main purpose of this meta-analysis is to evaluate the intelligence scores of patients with DMD and BMD, through a systematic review and meta-analysis of published scientific articles over the last decades. To provide better insight into the intelligence scores distribution of the different phenotypes, test scores of, respectively the DMD and BMD population will be compared to normative data of the typically developing population. We will take the mean age of the participants, the publication year of the study, the continent where the study took place, the type of Wechsler Intelligence scale used (WPPSI, WISC, WAIS), and the version of Wechsler Intelligence scale (e.g., WISC-V) into account as possible moderator effects. Results from this review will also be compared to a meta-analysis in 2001 [25] to give an update on the intelligence scores of male patients diagnosed with DMD and BMD. Next to that, we will make a comparison between the intelligence scores of males with DMD and BMD as it is believed that in Becker, FSIQ falls within the normal range and males of DMD are more at risk for lower intelligence.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Protocol and Registration

A research PROSPERO protocol was submitted (CRD42020194365).

2.2. Search Strategy

We searched for studies that examined or described standardized intelligence scores of boys and young men with Duchenne Muscular Dystrophy (DMD) and Becker Muscular Dystrophy (BMD) on the Wechsler Intelligence scales, published from 1960 through 2022, and written in the English language. Non-peer-reviewed articles, single case studies, dissertations/thesis, or proceedings of the conference were excluded. To address the risk of bias at the study level: solely Wechsler Intelligence scales were compared and all studies with a biased study sample were excluded (e.g., studies that included only DMD patients with intellectual disabilities or only DMD patients with a certain gene mutation, as those limited samples may not accurately represent the characteristics of the larger population). Studies that did not use Wechsler Intelligence scales to determine intelligence scores or studies that did not publish standardized intelligence scores were excluded. Relevant studies were identified by searching the electronic database PubMed/Medline, Embase, PsychInfo, and Web of Science. Articles were identified by search terms ‘Duchenne’; ‘Muscular Dystrophy, Duchenne’; ‘Becker Muscular Dystrophy’; ‘intelligence’ OR ‘IQ’; ‘intelligence tests’; ‘Wechsler’; ‘WAIS; ‘WPPSI; ‘WISC; ‘TIQ’, ‘FSIQ’. Each search was limited to ‘English’. The first search was set from 1960 to 2020. As there was a delay in our study, we did a second search with the same search strategy from 1960 to March 2022.
All identified articles were independently reviewed by the first authors (EM, PW) using the Rayyan app (Cambridge, MA, USA) for systematic reviews [40]. Full-text articles were searched and inspected if inclusion or exclusion could not be determined by reading the title or abstract. Cohen’s Kappa statistical analysis was used to measure the inter-rater reliability (≥0.60) of the independent study selection. Disagreements between the two raters after full-text review were resolved by the consensus and use of the last author (JH). PRISMA-P guidelines on meta-analysis [41] were followed.

2.3. Data Abstraction

The first author (EM) extracted data from included studies using a standardized form. Data were collected on population (dystrophinophaties, DMD, BMD), sample size (n), age (range, mean, SD), location (continent, country), Wechsler Intelligence scale being used (WPPSI, WISC, WAIS, and the different versions). Additionally, results of intelligence testing that were reported as an intelligence quotient (Mean = 100; SD = 15) were searched for. Range, mean, and standard deviation of scores were reported for full-scale (FSIQ), verbal (VIQ), and performance intelligence quotients (PIQ). The first author (PW) checked the extracted data to maximize reliability. To reduce the risk of bias and increase the directness of evidence, in- and exclusion criteria of the individual study samples were assessed. Inclusion criteria that selected a biased sample of the dystrophinopathies were eliminated from our statistical analyses. For example, studies that reported intelligence quotient scores of only a subgroup of the DMD population, such as boys with DMD with comorbid Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD), were excluded.

2.4. Quality Assessment of Studies

The quality of evidence within the included studies was independently assessed by two authors (PW and DS) with “The Joanna Briggs Institute (JBI) Critical Appraisal Tool for Analytical Cross Sectional Studies [42]. The study design, and analysis method are rated to judge the methodological quality and the extent to which a study has addressed the possibility of bias. This method is recommended for observational, cross-sectional studies [43]. Differences in opinion between the reviewers were discussed until a consensus was reached.

2.5. Statistical Analysis

Descriptive statistics were calculated using IBM SPSS statistics 27 (IBM corp, Armonk, NY, USA). Meta-analyses were performed in RStudio (Posit, PBC, Boston, MA, USA) [44] using the ‘meta’ package [45]. First, for each intelligence quotient outcome measure (FSIQ, PIQ, and VIQ) per subgroup (DMD and BMD), a separate random-effects model meta-analysis was performed, resulting in six separate random effects meta-analyses. In all six models, effect sizes were reported in terms of a standardized mean difference (SMD) using Cohen’s d [46] and once again in terms of the mean difference (MD) to be able to report corresponding intelligence quotient scores. Total study heterogeneity was quantified in terms of the I2 value and Cochran’s Q tests were performed for all six models to check for significant between-study heterogeneity. Besides 95% confidence intervals (95% CI), also 95% prediction intervals (95% PI) are reported. The 95% prediction interval reflects the range of expected IQ scores in future DMD and BMD patients [47].
Only a few included studies (k = 9) assessed the FSIQ of both healthy controls and males with DMD or BMD. To analyse a possible deviation of the FSIQ scores in dystrophinopathies from the standardization sample, for each included study Cohen’s d was calculated based on the difference between the reported mean FSIQ score and the mean FSIQ score of 100 (SD = 15) as reported for the standardization sample in the manuals of the Wechsler scales [35,36,37].
As mentioned above, the newest versions of the Wechsler scales report next to FSIQ, scores on five/four index scores. However, as only two studies made use of a newer version (i.e., WISC-IV) we did not further analyse these index scores of males with dystrophinopathies.
Next, post hoc subgroup analyses were conducted using Q-tests for subgroup differences to analyse possible differences in FSIQ between included DMD and BMD studies and possible differences between reported VIQ and PIQ outcomes within both the DMD and BMD subgroups separately. A post hoc independent samples t-test was performed to check whether the reported mean FSIQ scores significantly deviated from the mean FSIQ scores reported by Cotton et al. (2001). Post hoc meta-regression analyses were used to perform moderator analyses for the continent the study took place (i.e., Europe, North America, South America, Asia, and Oceania) to consider possible cross-cultural differences [48,49], the mean age of the included participants, the publication year of the study, the type of Wechsler Intelligence scale used (WPPSI, WISC, WAIS), and the version of Wechsler Intelligence scale (e.g., WISC-V).
Publication bias was assessed by visual inspection of funnel plots and was statistically assessed using Egger’s tests for funnel plot asymmetry [50]. Lastly, missing values analysis at the study level was computed to evaluate whether missing clinical outcomes (from patients that were included but not fully assessed with the Wechsler Intelligence scales) introduced bias in FSIQ of DMD and BMD. Statistical significance for all analyses was set at p < 0.05.

3. Results

3.1. Study Characteristics

The initial data search identified 325 articles, the second 42 (Figure 1). Of the studies, left after duplicates were removed, 90 met the eligibility criteria for full-text 333 assessment. The inter-rater Kappa reliability between the two first authors was 0.96. A third author (JH) was involved to solve two disagreements. Forty-seven full-text articles were excluded for diverse reasons (e.g., the study did not use Wechsler Intelligence scales) [12,14,16,18,21,28,29,51,52,53,54,55,56,57,58,59,60,61,62,63,64,65,66,67,68,69,70,71,72,73,74,75,76,77,78,79,80,81,82,83,84,85,86,87,88,89,90] and 43 studies were included in the meta-analysis [10,11,13,19,20,22,23,24,38,39,91,92,93,94,95,96,97,98,99,100,101,102,103,104,105,106,107,108,109,110,111,112,113,114,115,116,117,118,119,120] (Figure 1). Thirty-nine articles comprised data on DMD (84.78%), seven articles related to BMD (15.22%). Three articles reported data on both DMD and BMD [13,39,108] and were counted twice (in both samples). Publication dates ranged from 1972 to 2022. Twenty-three studies originated from Europe (53.50%), five from Asia (11.60%), nine from North America (20.90%), three from South America (7.00%), and three from Oceania (7.00%).
The 43 studies provided data on 1472 patients with dystrophinopathies, 1354 of them with DMD (91.98%), 118 with BMD (8.20%). Sample sizes differed from 3 to 148. Data on FSIQ were available for 1234 patients with DMD (k = 32) and 101 patients with BMD (k = 7). Data on VIQ were available in 35 studies for DMD (n = 1107) and six BMD studies (n = 91). Data on PIQ were available in 32 studies for DMD (n= 1075) and six BMD studies (n = 91).
Seven studies described a FSIQ of the WPPSI and the WISC (16.30%), three studies of the WPPSI, the WISC, and the WAIS (7.00%), eight studies the WISC and the WAIS (18.60%), twenty-two studies the WISC (51.20%), and three studies the WAIS (7.00%). Different Wechsler versions were used: the WPPSI (WPPSI 70.00%, WPPSI-III 30.00%), the WISC (WISC 32.50%, WISC-R 37.50%, WISC-III 20.00%, WISC and WISC-R combined 2.30%, WISC-III and WISC-IV combined 2.30%, WISC-IV 2.30%), and the WAIS (WAIS 71.40%, WAIS-R 7.10%, WAIS-III 21.40%).
Notation of age data differed per study, 34 studies provided a mean age, age range, and/or SD; therefore, weighted means are reported. The mean age for the total dystrophinopathy group was 11.65 years (SD = 6.61; Mean range = 5.70–38.80) based on 34 studies, for DMD 10.13 years (k = 27; SD = 2.97; Mean range = 5.70–27.30) and for BMD 27.54 years (k = 6; SD = 10.46; Mean range = 11.00–38.80).

3.2. Quality Assessment of Studies

The overall quality of evidence of the 32 included DMD studies was moderate, and for the 7 BMD studies strong. Details of the evaluation results are presented in Appendix A (Table A1 and Table A2).

3.3. Main Effects for FSIQ

Boys and men with DMD (k = 32) had, on average, a significantly lower FSIQ score compared to the means reported for the standardization sample (d = −0.98, Z = −15.26, 95% CI: −1.11, −0.85, p < 0.001), this translates into an average FSIQ of 84.76 points for individuals with DMD (95% CI: 82.81, 86.70; 95% PI: 76.34, 93.17) (Table 1). Heterogeneity between DMD studies was significant and is shown in Figure 2 (Q = 69.09, p < 0.001, I2 = 55.10%).
Boys and men with BMD (k = 7) also had, on average, a significantly lower FSIQ compared to the means reported for the standardization sample (d = −0.52, Z = −2.54, 95%CI: −0.92, −0.12, p = 0.01), this translates into an average FSIQ of 92.11 points (95%CI: 86.71, 97.50; 95%PI: 79.63, 104.59) (Table 1). Heterogeneity between BMD studies was not significant and is shown in Figure 3 (Q = 10.08, p = 0.12, I2 = 40.50%) (Figure 3).
Our results for the random effect model showed that the FSIQ scores of boys and men with BMD were, on average, significantly higher compared to boys and men with DMD (Q = 6.31, on 1 degrees of freedom, p = 0.01).

3.4. Tentative Post Hoc Analysis for VIQ and PIQ

The VIQ in DMD (k = 35) was, on average, significantly lower compared to the means reported for the standardization sample(d = −1.04, Z =−18.33, 95% CI: −1.15, −0.93; 95% PI: −1.42, −0.66), this translates into an average VIQ of 84.21 points (95% CI: 82.63, 85.78; 95% PI: 78.61, 89.81), see Table 1. Heterogeneity among DMD studies was significant (Q = 51.27, p = 0.03, I2 = 33.70%).
The average VIQ in BMD (k = 6) was also, on average, significantly lower compared to normative data (d = −0.56, Z = −1.99, 95% CI: −1.12, −0.01; 95% PI: −2.21, 1.08). This translates into an average VIQ of 90.81 (95% CI: 82.63, 98.99; 95% PI: 66.75, 114.87) (Table 1). Heterogeneity among studies was significant (Q = 13.34, p= 0.02, I2 = 62.50%).
PIQ scores in DMD (k = 32) were, on average, significantly lower compared to the normal population (d = −0.74, Z = −11.54, 95% CI: −0.86, −0.61; 95% PI: −1.24, −0.23). This translates into an average PIQ of 88.82. (95% CI: 82.63, 98.99; 95% PI: 66.75, 114.87), see Table 1. Heterogeneity among studies was significant (Q = 58.88, p < 0.01, I2 = 47.30%). PIQ scores in BMD (k = 6) were, on average, significantly lower compared to the normal population (d = −0.74, Z = −2.40, 95% CI: −1.35, −0.14; 95% PI: −2.60, 1.12). This translates into an average PIQ of 89.18 (95% CI: 81.33, 97.03; 95% PI: 65.29, 113.07) (Table 1). Heterogeneity among studies was significant (Q = 14.02, p = 0.02, I2 = 64.30%).

3.5. Moderator Analyses

For DMD, significant moderator effects were found in the FSIQ data regarding the continent where the study took place (R2 = 60.27). Studies taking place in South America showed significantly lower average FSIQ scores compared to studies from Europe (Z = 4.01, SE = 0.20 p < 0.001), Asia (Z = 2.70, SE = 0.24, p < 0.01), North America (Z = 4.92, SE = 0.17, p < 0.001), and Oceania (Z = 3.08, SE = 0.20, p = 0.002). FSIQ scores from the other continents did not significantly differ between one another, p > 0.05. No significant moderator effect was found for the continent where the study took place when studying the FSIQ data of the BMD group (p > 0.05). In de BMD group, a significant moderator effect was found on FSIQ for the WISC version used (Z = 2.12, SE = 0.47, p = 0.03): FSIQ scores on WISC-III are, on average, significantly higher than those scores on WISC-R, in BMD. No significant moderator effect was found on FSIQ for the other used versions (i.e., the type of the Wechsler Intelligence scale used (WPPSI, WISC, WAIS) and the version of the Wechsler Intelligence scale (e.g., WISC-III and WISC-IV)) in the BMD group (p > 0.05). No type and version effects were found in the DMD sample (p > 0.05).
The other moderator effects being analysed were: the mean age of the participants and the publication year of the study. Both were also not significant in both groups (p > 0.05).

3.6. Publication Bias

Egger’s regression did not indicate a publication bias concerning FSIQ scores in DMD (t = 1.28, p = 0.21). As p > 0.05, an additional trim-and-fill analysis was not conducted. See Figure 4 for the funnel plot.
Following recommendations by Sterne et al. (2011) [41], by default, a test for funnel plot asymmetry is only conducted if the number of studies is ten or more (k·min = 10). Therefore, the funnel plot and Egger’s test of FSIQ in BMD should be considered with caution (k = 7). Our tentative results indicate that there does not seem to be a publication bias for FSIQ in BMD (t = 0.28, p = 0.79) (Figure 5).

3.7. Missing Values Analysis

No significant missing clinical values were indicated for FSIQ scores in DMD (Z = 0.93, Standard Error = 0.01, p = 0.034) nor in BMD (Z = 70.00, Standard Error = 0.53, p = 0.48).

3.8. Comparison with Data from Cotton et al. (2001)

The FSIQ in our DMD sample (Mean = 84.67, SD = 34.86) was significantly higher than reported by Cotton (M = 80.20, SD = 19.3), (t = −3.90, p < 0.001).

4. Discussion

The aim of the current study was to conduct a meta-analysis on the Wechsler Intelligence scale scores of boys and men diagnosed with DMD and BMD. As there is a growing scientific and clinical interest in brain-related comorbidities of DMD and BMD [7], we felt the need to re-analyse available data as published in 2001 (Cotton et al. (ref) who analysed data of 32 studies using more than eight different intelligence tests). Our data describe 42 studies using Wechsler Intelligence scales. We found that males with DMD have a mean FSIQ score of 84.76 (95% CI: 82.81, 86.70; 95% PI: 76.34, 93.17), which confirms the Cotton review that FSIQ in DMD is one standard deviation below the average IQ of the general population and is to be classified as a low average intelligence [31,32]. In comparison with the Cotton review, our data show that the mean intelligence of boys and males with DMD is underestimated with 4.56 FSIQ points. The clinical significance of this statistical significant difference is not yet clear, but is a difference that needs to be taken in consideration when describing new data on intelligence scores in boys and men with dystrophinopathies. Our results predict that there is a 95% chance that a boy or man with DMD will have a FSIQ between 76.34 and 93.17, i.e., a below average to average score. We argue that the current, higher estimate is more robust because of our decision to use Wechsler Intelligence scales only and excluding studies with a selection bias. Furthermore, newer meta-analysis techniques enable us to examine variations in intelligence scores of patients with dystrophinopathy by possible confounding moderator variables such as: age of the participants, publication year of the study, type of Wechsler scale used (WPPSI, WISC, WAIS), version of Wechsler scale (e.g., WISC-V), and the location/continent where the article was published. Except for continent, none of these moderator variables interfered significantly with intelligence scores in our DMD group. In South-America, intelligence scores of boys and young men with DMD seem to be significantly lower than in other continents. However, it should be noted that only two South-American studies were included, both from Brazil, and that one of the two studies is identified as a significant outlier, the study of Rapaport et al. (1991) [93] reports a mean intelligence score of 71.76 (n = 148). However, we did not observe any differences in study methodology to explain this. We found that European studies were overrepresented in this review, which seems to be an overall trend in literature on DMD [121].
This is the first study analysing the intelligence scores of in a total of 101 boys and men with BMD in 7 studies using Wechsler Intelligence scales. Results indicate that BMD boys and men have a mean FSIQ of 92.11, an average intelligence score. The reliability interval of the intelligence scores of boys and men with BMD predicts that there is 95% chance a male with BMD will have a FSIQ on the Wechsler Intelligence scales between 79.63 and 104.59. The mean FSIQ scores of the BMD population were statistically significantly higher than those of boys and men with DMD. Where males with BMD score on average 7.35 FSIQ points more. However, it should be noted that the mean age of males with BMD was higher (27.54 years) than of our DMD population (10.13 years). Previous research has shown intelligence scores to stabilize from the age of 11–12 years [122,123,124], which is a little older than our DMD sample. As age was not a moderator in our meta-regression; therefore, we believe our FSIQ difference between DMD and BMD is representative.
Earlier research reported a statistically significant discrepancy between verbal IQ and performance IQ scores in boys and young men with DMD [25,99,110]. Our tentative analysis also suggests a statistically significant discrepancy between verbal IQ (84.21) and performance IQ (88.82) scores in boys and young men with DMD. The gap reported is similar to that reported by Cotton et al. (verbal IQ: 80.40 and performance IQ: 85.40). Several studies have supported the hypothesis that the lack of dystrophin in the cerebellum and hippocampus may play a role in language, (working) memory, and reading deficits [9,17,54,78,91,125]. In BMD, no discrepancy was found between verbal and performance IQ (mean VIQ: 90.81 and mean PIQ: 89.18). In the more recent theoretical models on intelligence (CHC model; WPPSI-IV, WISC-V and WAIS-V) the VIQ/PIQ structure is abandoned and exchanged for more specific intelligence index scores such as working memory. This an important domain to study within the DMD population since (verbal) working memory problems have been previously reported [51,80,125].
One important moderator factor, that has not been considered in both our review and the Cotton review, is the degree of (fine) motor impairment in boys and young men as some of the subtests of the Wechsler Intelligence scales require motor function and speed of (fine) motor function. Patients with dystrophinopathies may be hindered by progressive muscular weakness and motor delay. Different studies show that patients with impaired motor function are at a disadvantage on some of the subtests of the Wechsler Intelligence scales in which motor skills are required, leading to an underestimation of their intelligence scores [126,127]. Therefore, Pioversana et al. (2019) [128] developed an alternative motor-free Wechsler Intelligence scale measuring method to the ‘traditional’ test administration, calculating the FSIQ using only subtests from the WISC-V that do not have motor involvement: vocabulary and similarities (verbal comprehension index), visual puzzles and figure weights (perceptual reasoning index), digit span and letter number sequencing (working memory). This motor-free short-form of the WISC-V has not been used until now in the dystrophinopathy population, but it has been proven applicable for adolescents with Cerebral Palsy [129]. In future research, we recommend further exploration of the use on the WISC-V motor-free short-form in boys with DMD and BMD.
An important limitation of our work is the variability in versions of Wechsler scales, i.e., some children were assessed with WISC-R and other children with the WISC-V. Therefore, there may be some differences in intelligence scores that are subsequent to the varying versions that are based on different subtests. Our tentative analysis suggests that there are no confounding effects from the variability in versions, except in the BMD sample, the WISC-III (k = 2) differed from the WISC-R (k = 2, with WISC-R showing lower FSIQ scores). However, all other versions (e.g., WAIS or WISC and WISC-III or WISC-IV) did not differ in both samples. Therefore, we believe our overall intelligence scores are representative for our population. This analyses of moderator effects learns us that there were no significant moderator effect for the different versions of the Wechsler scales being used until now (paragraph 3.5). Therefore, it may be expected that the recent conversions into the WISC-V and WPPI-IV will not result in different intelligence scores for the dystrophinopathy populations.
The overall methodological quality of the included studies is for the DMD studies moderate and for the BMD studies strong, Although some included studies did not have a strong quality of evidence, we included all of the studies in our analysis. Considering that multiple studies received a weak or moderate rating because the authors did not identify or deal with confounding factors. However, intelligence scores were most of the time reported as a descriptive and not part of an intervention or manipulation in the reviewed studies. Nonetheless, it should be noted that some available studies did have limitations that could affect our results. In some cases, small sample sizes affect the precision of the estimated estimates and power of the included study. Secondly, in multiple studies information is missing. It was, often unclear in the reviewed studies whether the intelligence testing was carried out by a trained psychologist or not, which may lead to a ‘moderate’ score. Consequently, the study quality might have been under- of overestimated. Especially within the dystrophinopathy population, it should be taken in account by whom intelligence was assessed as the testing of men with dystrophinopathy may require more than average testing skills due to the complexity of their functioning caused by the brain related comorbidities [7].

5. Conclusions

In conclusion, our systematic review and meta-analysis demonstrated that males with DMD may manifest clinically and statistically meaningful reductions in overall intelligence compared to normative data. The mean FSIQ score found in DMD is 4.46 points higher in comparison to the review of the Cotton. On the one hand, our data confirm the findings of the Cotton review; on the other hand, our data further refine and elaborate on intelligence in DMD and BMD. The gap between performance IQ and verbal IQ in boys and men with DMD was not found in the seven studies published on BMD. Males with BMD have average intelligence scores. We recommend to routinely offer intelligence and cognitive functioning screening to patients with DMD and BMD and that the Wechsler Intelligence scales provide a solid instrument to measure intelligence in individuals with dystrophinopathies. These data may contribute to help patients achieve optimal participation in society (e.g., education, work, daily functioning).

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, P.M.M.W., E.M.M., P.P.M.H., and J.G.M.H.; methodology, P.M.M.W., E.M.M., and J.G.M.H.; software, D.J.J.S.; validation, P.M.M.W., and E.M.M.; formal analysis, D.J.J.S.; investigation, P.M.M.W., and E.M.M.; resources, S.K. and J.G.M.H.; data curation, P.M.M.W., E.M.M., and D.J.J.S.; writing—original draft preparation, P.M.M.W., and E.M.M.; writing—review and editing, P.M.M.W., E.M.M., D.J.J.S., D.G.M.S., R.J.V., S.K., P.P.M.H., and J.G.M.H.; visualization, P.M.M.W., and D.J.J.S.; supervision, P.P.M.H., R.J.V., S.K., and J.G.M.H.; project administration, J.G.M.H.; funding acquisition, J.G.M.H. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

European Union: 847826.

Institutional Review Board Statement

Not applicable.

Informed Consent Statement

Not applicable.

Data Availability Statement

Data supporting the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author P.M.M.W. on request.

Acknowledgments

We gratefully acknowledge support to P.W. and J.H. from the European Union Horizon 2020 Framework Programme research and innovation programme ‘Brain Involvement iN Dystrophinopathies’ (grant agreement, 847826). Special thanks to I. Gijselhart for her help creating the searches.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship and/or publication of this article.

Appendix A

Table A1. Quality of evidence of the DMD studies.
Table A1. Quality of evidence of the DMD studies.
AuthorsYear1. Were the Criteria for Inclusion in the Sample Clearly Defined?2. Were the Study Subjects and the Setting Described in Detail?3. Was the Exposure Measured in a Valid and Reliable Way?4. Were Objective, Standard Criteria Used for Measurement of the Condition?5. Were Confounding Factors Identified?6. Were Strategies to Deal with Confounding Factors Stated?7. Were the Outcomes Measured in a Valid and Reliable Way?8. Was Appropriate Statistical Analysis Used?TotalLevel of Quality
Anderson et al.1988NYYYYYUCY13strong
Appleton et al.1991UCYYYNNUCY10weak
Billard et al.1992UCYYYYYUCY14strong
Black & William1973UCYYYUCNUCY11weak
Bresolin et al.1994UCUCYYYUCUCY12moderate
Bunchanan et al.1981UCNYUCNNUCY7weak
Chamova et al.2013NUCYYNNYY9weak
Chieffo et al.2015UCUCYYYNYY12moderate
D’Angelo et al.2011YYYYYYUCY15strong
Da Silva Pereira et al.2005NNYYNNYN6weak
Filippo et al.2012YYYYYYYN14strong
Fu et al.2016YUCYYYYYY15strong
Glaub et al.2005UCUCYUCNNUCY8weak
Glaub et al.1987NUCYUCYYUCY11weak
Karagan & Zellweger1987YYYYYYUCY15strong
Kreis et al.2011NYYUCNYUCY10weak
Leibowitz &Dubowitz1981NYYYYYYY14strong
Lorusso et al.2013YYYYYYYY16strong
Marini et al.2007NYYYYYUCY13strong
Marsh1974NYYYNNUCUC8weak
Marsh & Munsat1974NYYYNNUCY9weak
Mc Donnald et al.1995NYYYUCYUCY12moderate
Nicholson et al.1993NNYUCNNUCY6weak
Ogasawara1989NNYYUCYUCUC9weak
Pane et al.2012YNYYUCNUCY10weak
Rapaport et al.1991UCNYYNNUCY8weak
Sollee et al.1985UCYYUCYYUCY13strong
Taylor et al.2010NYYYNNYY10weak
Wingeier et al.2011YYYYUCNYY13strong
Koken et al.2021YYYYUCNYY13strong
D’Alessandro et al.2021YYYYYYYY16strong
Preethish-Kumar et al.2022YYYYYYUCY15strong
Abbreviations used: Y = Yes criteria fulfilled; N = No criteria were not fulfilled; UC = Unclear if criteria were fulfilled; NA = not applicable.
Table A2. Quality of evidence of the BMD studies.
Table A2. Quality of evidence of the BMD studies.
AuthorsYear1. Were the Criteria for Inclusion in the Sample Clearly Defined?2. Were the Study Subjects and the Setting Described in Detail?3. Was the Exposure Measured in a Valid and Reliable Way?4. Were Objective, Standard Criteria Used for Measurement of the Condition?5. Were Confounding Factors Identified?6. Were Strategies to Deal with Confounding Factors Stated?7. Were the Outcomes Measured in a Valid and Reliable Way?8. Was Appropriate Statistical Analysis Used?TotalLevel of Quality
Chamova et al.2013NUCYYNNYY9weak
Melo et al.1995NYYYYYYY14strong
Mori-Yoshimura et al.2019UCYYUCYYYY14strong
Young et al.2008YYYYYYYY16strong
Koken et al.2021YYYYUCNYY13strong
Hellebrekers et al.2020NAYYYYNAYY12strong
D’alessandro et al.2021YYYYYYYY16strong
Abbreviations used: Y = Yes criteria fulfilled; N = No criteria were not fulfilled; UC = Unclear if criteria were fulfilled; NA = not applicable.

References

  1. Birnkrant, D.J.; Bushby, K.; Bann, C.M.; Apkon, S.D.; Blackwell, A.; Colvin, M.K.; Cripe, L.; Herron, A.R.; Kennedy, A.; Kinnett, K.; et al. Diagnosis and management of Duchenne muscular dystrophy, part 3: Primary care, emergency management, psychosocial care, and transitions of care across the lifespan. Lancet Neurol. 2018, 17, 445–455. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  2. Waldrop, M.; Flanigan, K.M. Update in Duchenne and Becker muscular dystrophy. Curr. Opin. Neurol. 2019, 32, 722–727. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Thangarajh, M. The Dystrophinopathies. CONTINUUM Lifelong Learn. Neurol. 2019, 25, 1619–1639. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Anderson, J.L.; Head, S.; Rae, C.; Morley, J.W. Brain function in Duchenne muscular dystrophy. Brain 2002, 125, 4–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Doorenweerd, N.; Hooijmans, M.; Schubert, S.A.; Webb, A.G.; Straathof, C.S.; van Zwet, E.W.; van Buchem, M.A.; Verschuuren, J.J.; Hendriksen, J.G.; Niks, E.H.; et al. Proton Magnetic Resonance Spectroscopy Indicates Preserved Cerebral Biochemical Composition in Duchenne Muscular Dystrophy Patients. J. Neuromuscul. Dis. 2017, 4, 53–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  6. Hendriksen, R.G.; Hoogland, G.; Schipper, S.; Hendriksen, J.G.; Vles, J.S.; Aalbers, M.W. A possible role of dystrophin in neuronal excitability: A review of the current literature. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2015, 51, 255–262. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Hendriksen, J.G.; Thangarajh, M.; Kan, H.E.; Muntoni, F.; Aoki, Y.; Collin, P.; Colvin, M.; Doorenweerd, N.; Ferlini, A.; Goyenvalle, A.; et al. 249th ENMC International Workshop: The role of brain dystrophin in muscular dystrophy: Implications for clinical care and translational research, Hoofddorp, The Netherlands, November 29th–December 1st 2019. Neuromuscul. Disord. 2020, 30, 782–794. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Snow, W.M.; Anderson, J.E.; Jakobson, L.S. Neuropsychological and neurobehavioral functioning in Duchenne muscular dystrophy: A review. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2013, 37, 743–752. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Cyrulnik, S.E.; Hinton, V.J. Duchenne muscular dystrophy: A cerebellar disorder? Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2008, 32, 486–496. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Kreis, R.; Wingeier, K.; Vermathen, P.; Giger, E.; Joncourt, F.; Zwygart, K.; Kaufmann, F.; Boesch, C.; Steinlin, M. Brain metabolite composition in relation to cognitive function and dystrophin mutations in boys with Duchenne muscular dystrophy. NMR Biomed. 2011, 24, 253–262. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Young, H.K.; Barton, B.A.; Waisbren, S.; Dale, L.P.; Ryan, M.M.; Webster, R.I.; North, K.N. Cognitive and Psychological Profile of Males With Becker Muscular Dystrophy. J. Child Neurol. 2008, 23, 155–162. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. Banihani, R.; Smile, S.; Yoon, G.; Dupuis, A.; Mosleh, M.; Snider, A.; McAdam, L. Cognitive and neurobehavioral profile in boys with duchenne muscular dystrophy. J. Child Neurol. 2015, 30, 472–482. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Chamova, T.; Guergueltcheva, V.; Raycheva, M.; Todorov, T.; Genova, J.; Bichev, S.; Bojinova, V.; Mitev, V.; Tournev, I.; Todorova, A. Association between loss of Dp140 and cognitive impairment in Duchenne and Becker dystrophies. Balk. J. Med. Genet. 2013, 16, 21–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  14. Daoud, F.; Angeard, N.; Demerre, B.; Martie, I.; Benyaou, R.; Leturcq, F.; Cossée, M.; Deburgrave, N.; Saillour, Y.; Tuffery, S.; et al. Analysis of Dp71 contribution in the severity of mental retardation through comparison of Duchenne and Becker patients differing by mutation consequences on Dp71 expression. Hum. Mol. Genet. 2009, 18, 3779–3794. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  15. Darmahkasih, A.J.; Rybalsky, I.; Tian, C.; Shellenbarger, K.C.; Horn, P.S.; Ma, J.T.L.; Wong, B.L. Neurodevelopmental, behavioral, and emotional symptoms common in Duchenne muscular dystrophy. Muscle Nerve 2020, 61, 466–474. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Bardoni, A.; Felisari, G.; Sironi, M.; Comi, G.; Lai, M.; Robotti, M.; Bresolin, N. Loss of Dp140 regulatory sequences is associated with cognitive impairment in dystrophinopathies. Neuromuscul. Disord. 2000, 10, 194–199. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Hendriksen, J.G.; Vles, J.S. Are Males With Duchenne Muscular Dystrophy at Risk for Reading Disabilities? Pediatr. Neurol. 2006, 34, 296–300. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Ricotti, V.; Mandy, W.P.L.; Scoto, M.; Pane, M.; Deconinck, N.; Messina, S.; Mercuri, E.; Skuse, D.H.; Muntoni, F. Neurodevelopmental, emotional, and behavioural problems in Duchenne muscular dystrophy in relation to underlying dystrophin gene mutations. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 2016, 58, 77–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  19. Taylor, P.J.; Betts, G.A.; Maroulis, S.; Gilissen, C.; Pedersen, R.L.; Mowat, D.R.; Johnston, H.M.; Buckley, M.F. Dystrophin gene mutation location and the risk of cognitive impairment in duchenne muscular dystrophy. PLoS ONE 2010, 5, e8803. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Felisari, G.; Boneschi, F.M.; Bardoni, A.; Sironi, M.; Comi, G.; Robotti, M.; Turconi, A.C.; Lai, M.; Corrao, G.; Bresolin, N. Loss of Dp140 dystrophin isoform and intellectual impairment in Duchenne dystrophy. Neurology 2000, 55, 559–564. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Moizard, M.P.; Billard, C.; Toutain, A.; Berret, F.; Marmin, N.; Moraine, C. Are Dp71 and Dp140 brain dystrophin isoforms related to cognitive impairment in Duchenne muscular dystrophy? Am. J. Med. Genet. 1998, 80, 32–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. D’Angelo, M.G.; Lorusso, M.L.; Civati, F.; Comi, G.P.; Magri, F.; Del Bo, R.; Guglieri, M.; Molteni, M.; Turconi, A.C.; Bresolin, N. Neurocognitive profiles in duchenne muscular dystrophy and gene mutation site. Pediatr. Neurol. 2011, 45, 292–299. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  23. Hellebrekers, D.M.J.; Vles, J.S.H.; Klinkenberg, S.; Hendriksen, J.G.M. The Neurocognitive and Behavioral Profiles of 3 Brothers With Becker Muscular Dystrophy. Child Neurol. Open 2020, 7, 2329048X2095721. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. reethish-Kumar, V.; Shah, A.; Polavarapu, K.; Kumar, M.; Safai, A.; Vengalil, S.; Nashi, S.; Deepha, S.; Govindaraj, P.; Afsar, M.; et al. Disrupted structural connectome and neurocognitive functions in Duchenne muscular dystrophy: Classifying and subtyping based on Dp140 dystrophin isoform. J. Neurol. 2022, 269, 2113–2125. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Cotton, S.; Voudouris, N.J.; Greenwood, K.M. Intelligence and Duchenne muscular dystrophy: Full-scale, verbal, and performance intelligence quotients. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 2001, 43, 497–501. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  26. Sternberg, R.J. Intelligence. State of art. Dialogues Clin. Neurosci. 2012, 14, 19–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Bagdatlioglu, E.; Porcari, P.; Greally, E.; Blamire, A.M.; Straub, V.W. Cognitive impairment appears progressive in the mdx mouse. Neuromuscul. Disord. 2020, 30, 368–388. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  28. Doorenweerd, N. Combining genetics, neuropsychology and neuroimaging to improve understanding of brain involvement in Duchenne muscular dystrophy-a narrative review. Neuromuscul. Disord. 2020, 30, 437–442. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Perumal, A.R.; Rajeswaran, J.; Nalini, A. Neuropsychological Profile of Duchenne Muscular Dystrophy. Appl. Neuropsychol. Child 2015, 4, 49–57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Ferrero, A.; Rossi, M. Cognitive profile and neuropsychiatric disorders in Becker muscular dystrophy: A systematic review of literature. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2022, 137, 104648. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Sattler, J.M. Assessment of Children; Cognitive Foundations: San Diego, CA, USA, 2008. [Google Scholar]
  32. Weiss, L.G.; Prifitera, A.; Holdnack, J.A.; Saklofske, D.H. WISC-IV Advanced Clinical Interpretation; Academic Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2006. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Watkins, M.W.; Smith, L.G. Long-term stability of the wechsler intelligence scale for children-fourth edition. Psychol. Assess. 2013, 25, 477–483. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  34. Weerkamp, P.; Chieffo, D.; Collin, P.; Moriconi, F.; Papageorgiou, A.; Vainieri, I.; Miranda, R.; Hankinson, C.; Vogel, A.; Poncet, S.; et al. Psychological test usage in Duchenne muscular dystrophy: An EU Delphi based multi-centre study. (Unpublished manuscript).
  35. Wechsler, D. Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Fifth Edition-Nederlandstalige Bewerking. Technische Handleiding; Pearson: London, UK, 2018. [Google Scholar]
  36. Wechsler, D. WAIS-IV-NL: Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale—Nederlandstalige Bewerking; Pearson: London, UK, 2012. [Google Scholar]
  37. Wechsler, D. Wechsler Preschool and Primary; Pearson: London, UK, 2020. [Google Scholar]
  38. Mori-Yoshimura, M.; Mizuno, Y.; Yoshida, S.; Ishihara, N.; Minami, N.; Morimoto, E.; Maruo, K.; Nonaka, I.; Komaki, H.; Nishino, I.; et al. Psychiatric and neurodevelopmental aspects of Becker muscular dystrophy. Neuromuscul. Disord. 2019, 9, 930–939. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  39. Köken, Ö.Y.; Kucur, Ö.; Taşkiran, C.; Öztoprak, Ü.; Sel, Ç.G.; Aksoy, E.; Aksoy, A.; Yoldaş, T.; Yüksel, D. Clinical features and quality of life in duchenne and becker muscular dystrophy patients from a tertiary center in Turkey. Guncel Pediatri 2021, 19, 15–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Ouzzani, M.; Hammady, H.; Fedorowicz, Z.; Elmagarmid, A. Rayyan—A web and mobile app for systematic reviews. Syst. Rev. 2016, 5, 210. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  41. Sterne, J.A.C.; Sutton, A.J.; Ioannidis, J.P.A.; Terrin, N.; Jones, D.R.; Lau, J.; Carpenter, J.; Rücker, G.; Harbord, R.M.; Schmid, C.H.; et al. Recommendations for examining and interpreting funnel plot asymmetry in meta-analyses of randomised controlled trials. BMJ 2011, 343, d4002. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  42. Moola, S.; Munn, Z.; Tufanaru, C.; Aromataris, E.; Sears, K.; Sfetcu, R.; Currie, M.J.; Lisy, K.; Qureshi, R.; Mattis, P.; et al. Chapter 7: Systematic reviews of etiology and risk. Joanna Briggs Inst. Rev. Man. 2017, 5, 219. [Google Scholar]
  43. Ma, L.-L.; Wang, Y.-Y.; Yang, Z.-H.; Huang, D.; Weng, H.; Zeng, X.-T. Methodological quality (risk of bias) assessment tools for primary and secondary medical studies: What are they and which is better? Mil. Med. Res. 2020, 7, 7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  44. RStudio Team. RStudio: Integrated Development for R. Rstudio Team; PBC: Boston, MA, USA, 2020; Available online: http//www.rstudio.com (accessed on 18 October 2022).
  45. Balduzzi, S.; Rücker, G.; Schwarzer, G. How to perform a meta-analysis with R: A practical tutorial. Evid. -Based Ment. Health 2019, 22, 153–160. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  46. Cohen, J. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences; Routledge: London, UK, 2013. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. IntHout, J.; Ioannidis, J.P.A.; Rovers, M.; Goeman, J.J. Plea for routinely presenting prediction intervals in meta-analysis. BMJ Open 2016, 6, e010247. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Gonthier, C. Cross-cultural differences in visuo-spatial processing and the culture-fairness of visuo-spatial intelligence tests: An integrative review and a model for matrices tasks. Cogn. Res. Princ. Implic. 2022, 7, 1–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Cockcroft, K.; Alloway, T.; Copello, E.; Milligan, R. A cross-cultural comparison between South African and British students on the wechsler adult intelligence scales third edition (WAIS-III). Front. Psychol. 2015, 6, 297. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  50. Egger, M.; Smith, G.D.; Schneider, M.; Minder, C. Bias in meta-analysis detected by a simple, graphical test. BMJ 1997, 315, 629–634. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  51. Thangarajh, M.; Elfring, G.L.; Trifillis, P. Longitudinal evaluation of working memory in duchenne muscular dystrophy. J. Clin. Med. 2020, 9, 2940. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  52. Battini, R.; Lenzi, S.; Lucibello, S.; Chieffo, D.; Moriconi, F.; Cristofani, P.; Bulgheroni, S.; Cumbo, F.; Pane, M.; Baranello, G.; et al. Longitudinal data of neuropsychological profile in a cohort of Duchenne muscular dystrophy boys without cognitive impairment. Neuromuscul. Disord. 2021, 31, 319–327. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  53. Panda, P.K.; Sharawat, I.K. Mental health and behavioral function in children with neuromuscular disorders. Eur. J. Paediatr. Neurol. 2021, 30, 66–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  54. Hoogland, G.; Hendriksen, R.G.F.; Slegers, R.J.; Hendriks, M.P.H.; Schijns, O.E.M.G.; Aalbers, M.W.; Vles, J.S.H. The expression of the distal dystrophin isoforms Dp140 and Dp71 in the human epileptic hippocampus in relation to cognitive functioning. Hippocampus 2019, 29, 102–110. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Lionarons, J.M.; Hellebrekers, D.M.; Klinkenberg, S.; Faber, C.G.; Vles, J.S.; Hendriksen, J.G. Methylphenidate use in males with Duchenne muscular dystrophy and a comorbid attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Eur. J. Paediatr. Neurol. 2019, 23, 152–157. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  56. Bushby, K.M.D.; Appleton, R.; Anderson, L.V.B.; Welch, J.L.; Kelly, P.; Gardner-Medwin, D. Deletion status and intellectual impairment in duchenne muscular dystrophy. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 2008, 37, 260–269. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Rapaport, D.; Passos-Bueno, M.R.; Takata, R.I.; Campiotto, S.; Eggers, S.; Vainzof, M.; Makover, A.; Nudel, U.; Yaffe, D.; Zatz, M. A deletion including the brain promoter of the Duchenne muscular dystrophy gene is not associated with mental retardation. Neuromuscul. Disord. 1992, 2, 117–120. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Moizard, M.-P.; Toutain, A.; Fournier, D.; Berret, F.; Raynaud, M.; Billard, C.; Andres, C.; Moraine, C. Severe cognitive impairment in DMD: Obvious clinical indication for Dp71 isoform point mutation screening. Eur. J. Hum. Genet. 2000, 8, 552–556. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  59. Florek, M.; Karolak, S. Intelligence level of patients with the duchenne type of progressive muscular dystrophy (PMD-D). Eur. J. Pediatr. 1977, 126, 275–282. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  60. Desguerre, I.; Christov, C.; Mayer, M.; Zeller, R.; Becane, H.-M.; Bastuji-Garin, S.; Leturcq, F.; Chiron, C.; Chelly, J.; Gherardi, R.K. Clinical heterogeneity of Duchenne muscular dystrophy (DMD): Definition of sub-phenotypes and predictive criteria by long-term follow-up. PLoS ONE 2009, 4, e4347. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  61. Miller, G.; Tunnecliffe, M.; Psych, M.A.; Douglas, P.S.; Dip, G. IQ, prognosis and duchenne muscular dystrophy. Brain Dev. 1985, 7, 7–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Savage, R.D.; Adams, M. Cognitive functioning and neurological deficit: Duchenne muscular dystrophy and cerebral palsy. Aust. Psychol. 1979, 14, 59–75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Ueda, Y.; Suwazono, S.; Maedo, S.; Higuchi, I. Profile of cognitive function in adults with duchenne muscular dystrophy. Brain Dev. 2017, 39, 225–230. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. HHausmanowa-Petrusewicz, I.; Zaremba, J.; Fidziańska, A.; Zimowski, J.; Bisko, M.; Badurska, B.; Fidziańska, E.; Lusakowska, A.; Borkowska, J. Interrelationship between gene, its product and phenotype in Duchenne and Becker muscular dystrophy. Acta Neurobiol. Exp. (Wars) 1993, 53, 297–303. [Google Scholar]
  65. Parisi, L.; Di Filippo, T.; Glorioso, P.; La Grutta, S.; Epifanio, M.S.; Roccella, M. Autism spectrum disorders in children affected by Duchenne muscular dystrophy. Minerva Pediatr. 2018, 70, 233–239. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Sato, Y.; Yamauchi, A.; Urano, M.; Kondo, E.; Saito, K. Corticosteroid therapy for duchenne muscular dystrophy: Improvement of psychomotor function. Pediatr. Neurol. 2014, 50, 31–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Della Coletta, M.V.; Scola, R.H.; Wiemes, G.R.M.; Fonseca, C.N.; Mäder, M.J.; Freund, A.A.; Werneck, L.C. Event-related potentials (P300) and neuropsychological assessment in boys exhibiting duchenne muscular dystrophy. Arq. Neuropsiquiatr. 2007, 65, 59–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  68. Colombo, P.; Nobile, M.; Tesei, A.; Civati, F.; Gandossini, S.; Mani, E.; Molteni, M.; Bresolin, N.; D’Angelo, M.G. Assessing mental health in boys with Duchenne muscular dystrophy: Emotional, behavioural and neurodevelopmental profile in an Italian clinical sample. Eur. J. Paediatr. Neurol. 2017, 21, 639–647. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Lincoln, N.B.; Staples, D.J. Psychological aspects of some chronic progressive neuromuscular disorders. 1. Cognitive abilities. J. Chronic Dis. 1977, 30, 207–215. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Donders, J.; Taneja, C. Neurobehavioral characteristics of children with duchenne muscular dystrophy. Child Neuropsychol. 2009, 15, 295–304. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  71. Tracey, I.; Thompson, C.; Dunn, J.; Barnes, P.; Styles, P.; Kemp, G.; Rae, C.; Radda, G.; Pike, M.; Scott, R. Brain abnormalities in Duchenne muscular dystrophy: Phosphorus-31 magnetic resonance spectroscopy and neuropsychological study. Lancet 1995, 345, 1260–1264. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Bardoni, A.; Sironi, M.; Felisari, G.; Comi, G.P.; Bresolin, N. Absence of brain Dp140 isoform and cognitive impairment in Becker muscular dystrophy. Lancet 1999, 353, 897–898. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  73. Magri, F.; Govoni, A.; D’Angelo, M.G.; Del Bo, R.; Ghezzi, S.; Sandra, G.; Turconi, A.C.; Sciacco, M.; Ciscato, P.; Bordoni, A.; et al. Genotype and phenotype characterization in a large dystrophinopathic cohort with extended follow-up. J. Neurol. 2011, 258, 1610–1623. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  74. Rabbi-Bortolini, E.; Zatz, M.; Opitz, J.M.; Reynolds, J.F. Investigation on genetic heterogeneity in Duchenne muscular dystrophy. Am. J. Med. Genet. 1986, 24, 111–117. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  75. Rasic, M.V.; Vojinovic, D.; Pešović, J.; Mijalkovic, G.; Lukic, V.; Mladenovic, J.; Kosac, A.; Novakovic, I.; Maksimovic, N.; Romac, S.; et al. Intellectual ability in the duchenne muscular dystrophy and dystrophin gene mutation location. Balk. J. Med. Genet. 2014, 17, 25–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  76. Dorman, C.; Hurley, A.D.; Avignon, J.D. Language and Learning Disorders of Older Boys with Duchenne Muscular Dystrophy. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 2008, 30, 316–327. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  77. Karagan, N.J.; Richman, L.C.; Sorensen, J.P. Analysis of verbal disability in duchenne muscular dystrophy. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 1980, 168, 419–423. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  78. Fabbro, F.; Felisari, G.; D’Angelo, M.G.; Turconi, A.C.; Marini, A.; Comi, G.P.; Bresolin, N. Language disturbances in a group of participants suffering from Duchenne Muscular Dystrophy: A pilot study. Percept. Mot. Ski. 2007, 104, 663–676. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  79. Upadhyaya, M.; Smith, R.A.; Thomas, N.S.T.; Norman, A.M.; Harper, P.S. Intragenic deletions in 164 boys with Duchenne muscular dystrophy (DMD) studied with dystrophin cDNA. Clin. Genet. 1990, 37, 456–462. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  80. Hellebrekers, D.M.; Doorenweerd, N.; Sweere, D.J.; van Kuijk, S.M.; Aartsma-Rus, A.M.; Klinkenberg, S.; Vles, J.S.; Hendriksen, J.G. Longitudinal follow-up of verbal span and processing speed in Duchenne muscular dystrophy. Eur. J. Paediatr. Neurol. 2020, 25, 120–126. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  81. Zellweger, H. Psychometric Studies. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 1967, 9, 576–581. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  82. Mento, G.; Tarantino, V.; Bisiacchi, P.S. The neuropsychological profile of infantile Duchenne muscular dystrophy. Clin. Neuropsychol. 2011, 25, 1359–1377. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  83. Astrea, G.; Pecini, C.; Gasperini, F.; Brisca, G.; Scutifero, M.; Bruno, C.; Santorelli, F.M.; Cioni, G.; Politano, L.; Chilosi, A.M.; et al. Reading impairment in Duchenne muscular dystrophy: A pilot study to investigate similarities and differences with developmental dyslexia. Res. Dev. Disabil. 2015, 45–46, 168–177. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  84. Chamova, T.K.; Guerguelcheva, V.; Raycheva, M.; Mihaylova, Z.; Tournev, I. Genotype-phenotype correlations of the cognitive disorders in Bulgarian patients with Duchenne muscular dystrophy. Eur. J. Neurol. 2010, 17, 472. [Google Scholar]
  85. Vojinovic, D.; Rasic, V.M. Cognitive impairment of males with Duchenne muscular dystrophy. J. Neurol. 2013, 260, S260. [Google Scholar]
  86. Battini, R.; Chieffo, D.; Bulgheroni, S.; Piccini, G.; Pecini, C.; Lucibello, S.; Lenzi, S.; Moriconi, F.; Pane, M.; Astrea, G.; et al. Cognitive profile in Duchenne muscular dystrophy boys without intellectual disability: The role of executive functions. Neuromuscul. Disord. 2018, 28, 122–128. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  87. North, K.N.; Miller, G.; Iannaccone, S.T.; Clemens, P.R.; Chad, D.A.; Bella, I.; Smith, T.W.; Beggs, A.; Specht, L.A. Cognitive dysfunction as the major presenting feature of Becker’s muscular dystrophy. Neurology 1996, 46, 461–465. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  88. Colombo, P.; Civati, F.; Mani, E.; Gandossini, S.; Brighina, E.; Comi, G.; Bresolin, N.; Turconi, A.; Molteni, M.; Nobile, M.; et al. G.P.174. Neuromuscul. Disord. 2014, 24, 858. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  89. Ogasawara, A. Downward shift in IQ in persons with Duchenne muscular dystrophy compared to those with spinal muscular atrophy. Am. J. Ment. Retard. 1989, 93, 544–547. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  90. Kozicka, A.; Prot, J.; Wasilewski, R. Mental retardation in patients with Duchenne progressive muscular dystrophy. J. Neurol. Sci. 1971, 14, 209–213. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  91. Lorusso, M.L.; Civati, F.; Molteni, M.; Turconi, A.C.; Bresolin, N.; D’Angelo, M.G. Specific profiles of neurocognitive and reading functions in a sample of 42 Italian boys with Duchenne Muscular Dystrophy. Child Neuropsychol. 2013, 19, 350–369. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  92. Marsh, G.G.; Munsat, T.L. Evidence for early impairment of verbal intelligence in Duchenne muscular dystrophy. Arch. Dis. Child. 1974, 49, 118–122. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  93. Rapaport, D.; Passos-Bueno, M.R.; Brandão, L.; Love, D.; Vainzof, M.; Zatz, M. Apparent association of mental retardation and specific patterns of deletions screened with probes cf56a and cf23a in Duchenne Muscular Dystrophy. Am. J. Med. Genet. 1991, 39, 437–441. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  94. Anderson, S.W.; Routh, D.K.; Ionasescu, V.V. Serial position memory of boys with duchenne muscular dystrophy. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 2008, 30, 328–333. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  95. Al-Qudah, A.A.; Kobayashi, J.; Chuang, S.; Dennis, M.; Ray, P. Etiology of intellectual impairment in Duchenne muscular dystrophy. Pediatr. Neurol. 1990, 6, 57–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  96. Pane, M.; Lombardo, M.E.; Alfieri, P.; D’Amico, A.; Bianco, F.; Vasco, G.; Piccini, G.; Mallardi, M.; Romeo, D.; Ricotti, V.; et al. Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and cognitive function in duchenne muscular dystrophy: Phenotype-genotype correlation. J. Pediatr. 2012, 161, 705–709.e1. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  97. Marsh, G.G. Impaired visual-motor ability of children with Duchenne muscular dystrophy. Percept. Mot. Ski. 1972, 35, 504–506. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  98. Leibowitz, D.; Dubowitz, V. Intellect and Behaviour in Duchenne Muscular Dystrophy. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 2008, 23, 577–590. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  99. Karagan, N.J.; Zellweger, H.U. Early Verbal Disability in Children with Duchenne Muscular Dystrophy. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 2008, 20, 435–441. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  100. Suzuki, Y.; Higuchi, S.; Aida, I.; Nakajima, T.; Nakada, T. Abnormal distribution of GABAA receptors in brain of duchenne muscular dystrophy patients. Muscle Nerve 2017, 55, 591–595. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  101. Nicholson, L.V.; Johnson, M.A.; Bushby, K.M.; Gardner-Medwin, D.; Curtis, A.; Ginjaar, I.B.; Dunnen, J.T.D.; Welch, J.L.; Butler, T.J.; Bakker, E. Integrated study of 100 patients with Xp21 linked muscular dystrophy using clinical, genetic, immunochemical, and histopathological data. Part 2. Correlations within individual patients. J. Med. Genet. 1993, 30, 737–744. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  102. Black, F.W. Intellectual Ability as Related to Age and Stage of Disease in Muscular Dystrophy: A Brief Note. J. Psychol. 1973, 84, 333–334. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  103. Pereira, C.C.D.S.; Kiyomoto, B.H.; Cardoso, R.; Oliveira, A.S.B. Duchenne muscular dystrophy: δ-dystroglycan immunoexpression in skeletal muscle and cognitive performance. Arq. Neuropsiquiatr. 2005, 63, 984–989. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  104. Bresolin, N.; Castelli, E.; Comi, G.; Felisari, G.; Bardoni, A.; Perani, D.; Grassi, F.; Turconi, A.C.; Mazzucchelli, F.; Gallotti, D.; et al. Cognitive impairment in Duchenne muscular dystrophy. Neuromuscul. Disord. 1994, 4, 359–369. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  105. Filippini, M.; Guerra, A.; Negosanti, A.; Santi, S.; Sarajlija, J.; Musti, M.A.; Gobbi, G.; Lassonde, M.; Pini, A. Mismatch Negativity Recording in Children with Duchenne Muscular Dystrophy: A Preliminary Study Integrating Neurophysiological and Neuropsychological Results. J. Child Neurol. 2016, 31, 1468–1474. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  106. Appleton, R.E.; Bushby, K.; Gardner-Medwin, D.; Welch, J.; Kelly, P.J. Head Circumference And Intellectual Performance Of Patients With Duchenne Muscular Dystrophy. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 2008, 33, 884–890. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  107. Ogasawara, A. Similarity of IQs of siblings with Duchenne progressive muscular dystrophy. Am. J. Ment. Retard. 1989, 93, 548–550. [Google Scholar]
  108. D’Alessandro, R.; Ragusa, N.; Vacchetti, M.; Rolle, E.; Rossi, F.; Brusa, C.; Davico, C.; Vitiello, B.; Mongini, T.; Ricci, F.S. Assessing cognitive function in neuromuscular diseases: A pilot study in a sample of children and adolescents. J. Clin. Med. 2021, 10, 4777. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  109. Sollee, N.D.; Latham, E.E.; Kindlon, D.J.; Bresnan, M.J. Neuropsychological impairment in Duchenne Muscular Dystrophy. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 1985, 7, 486–496. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  110. Marini, A.; Lorusso, M.L.; D’Angelo, M.G.; Civati, F.; Turconi, A.C.; Fabbro, F.; Bresolin, N. Evaluation of narrative abilities in patients suffering from Duchenne Muscular Dystrophy. Brain Lang. 2007, 102, 1–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  111. Melo, M.; Lauriano, V.; Gentil, V.; Eggers, S.; Del Bianco, S.S.; Gimenez, P.R.; Akiyama, J.; Okabaiashi, H.; Frota-Pessoa, O.; Passos-Bueno, M.R.; et al. Becker and limb-girdle muscular dystrophies: A psychiatric and intellectual level comparative study. Am. J. Med. Genet. Neuropsychiatr. Genet. 1995, 60, 33–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  112. Wingeier, K.; Giger, E.; Strozzi, S.; Kreis, R.; Joncourt, F.; Conrad, B.; Gallati, S.; Steinlin, M. Neuropsychological impairments and the impact of dystrophin mutations on general cognitive functioning of patients with Duchenne muscular dystrophy. J. Clin. Neurosci. 2011, 18, 90–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  113. Fu, Y.; Dong, Y.; Zhang, C.; Sun, Y.; Zhang, S.; Mu, X.; Wang, H.; Xu, W.; Wu, S. Diffusion tensor imaging study in Duchenne muscular dystrophy. Ann. Transl. Med. 2016, 4, 109. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  114. Buchanan, D.C.; Roelofs, R.I.; White, D.K.; De Arango, G.S. Intellectual function in Duchenne muscular dystrophy: The influence of penicillamine. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 1981, 62, 623–625. [Google Scholar]
  115. Gauld, L.M.; Boynton, A.; Betts, G.A.; Johnston, H. Spirometry is affected by intelligence and behavior in Duchenne muscular dystrophy. Pediatr. Pulmonol. 2005, 40, 408–413. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  116. McDonald, C.M.; Abresch, R.T.; Carter, G.T.; Fowler, W.M.; Johnson, E.R.; Kilmer, D.D.; Sigford, B.J. Duchenne muscular dystrophy. Am. J. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 1995, 74, S70–S92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  117. Glaub, T.; Mechler, F. Intellectual function in muscular dystrophies. Eur. Arch. Psychiatry Neurol. Sci. 1987, 236, 379–382. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  118. Chieffo, D.; Brogna, C.; Berardinelli, A.; D’Angelo, G.; Mallardi, M.; D’Amico, A.; Alfieri, P.; Mercuri, E.; Pane, M. Early neurodevelopmental findings predict school age cognitive abilities in duchenne muscular dystrophy: A longitudinal study. PLoS ONE 2015, 10, e0133214. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  119. Di Filippo, T.; Parisi, L.; Roccella, M. Psychological aspects in children affected by duchenne de boulogne muscular dystrophy. Ment. Illn. 2012, 4, 21–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  120. Billard, C.; Gillet, P.; Signoret, J.; Uicaut, E.; Bertrand, P.; Fardeau, M.; Barthez-Carpentier, M.; Santini, J. Cognitive functions in duchenne muscular dystrophy: A reappraisal and comparison with spinal muscular atrophy. Neuromuscul. Disord. 1992, 2, 371–378. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  121. Crisafulli, S.; Sultana, J.; Fontana, A.; Salvo, F.; Messina, S.; Trifirò, G. Global epidemiology of Duchenne muscular dystrophy: An updated systematic review and meta-analysis. Orphanet J. Rare Dis. 2020, 15, 141. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  122. Canivez, G.L.; Watkins, M.W. Long-term stability of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children—Third Edition. Psychol. Assess 1998, 10, 285. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  123. Resing, W.C.M.; Bleichrodt, N.; Drenth, P.J.D.; Zaal, J.N. RAKIT-2: Revisie Amsterdamse Kinder Intelligentie Test; Pearson: London, UK, 2012. [Google Scholar]
  124. Watkins, M.W.; Glutting, J.J.; Youngstrom, E.A. Issues in subtest profile analysis. In Contemporary Intellectual Assessment: Theories, Tests, and Issues; The Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 2005. [Google Scholar]
  125. Hinton, V.J.; DE Vivo, D.C.; Nereo, N.E.; Goldstein, E.; Stern, Y. Selective deficits in verbal working memory associated with a known genetic etiology: The neuropsychological profile of Duchenne muscular dystrophy. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 2001, 7, 45–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  126. Foo, R.Y.; Guppy, M.; Johnston, L.M. Intelligence assessments for children with cerebral palsy: A systematic review. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 2013, 55, 911–918. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  127. Sherwell, S.; Reid, S.M.; Reddihough, D.S.; Wrennall, J.; Ong, B.; Stargatt, R. Measuring intellectual ability in children with cerebral palsy: Can we do better? Res. Dev. Disabil. 2014, 35, 2558–2567. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  128. Piovesana, A.M.; Harrison, J.L.; Ducat, J.J. The Development of a Motor-Free Short-Form of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children–Fifth Edition. Assessment 2019, 26, 1564–1572. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  129. Coceski, M.; Hocking, D.R.; Abu-Rayya, H.M.; Sherwell, S.; Reid, S.M.; Reddihough, D.S.; Wrennall, J.; Stargatt, R. WISC-V motor-free cognitive profile and predictive factors in adolescents with cerebral palsy. Res. Dev. Disabil. 2021, 113, 103934. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Flowchart of the selected articles.
Figure 1. Flowchart of the selected articles.
Brainsci 12 01544 g001
Figure 2. Forest plot of full-scale intelligence scores in the DMD population. Abbreviations used: CI = confidence interval; p = significance level; SMD = standard mean difference; SD = standard deviation [10,13,24,26,41,96,97,98,99,101,102,103,104,107,108,109,110,113,114,115,117,118,121,122,123,124,125,126,127,128,129].
Figure 2. Forest plot of full-scale intelligence scores in the DMD population. Abbreviations used: CI = confidence interval; p = significance level; SMD = standard mean difference; SD = standard deviation [10,13,24,26,41,96,97,98,99,101,102,103,104,107,108,109,110,113,114,115,117,118,121,122,123,124,125,126,127,128,129].
Brainsci 12 01544 g002
Figure 3. Forest plot of full-scale intelligence scores in the BMD population. Abbreviations used: CI = confidence interval; p = significance level; SMD = standard mean difference; SD = standard deviation [11,13,41,42,105,115,120,].
Figure 3. Forest plot of full-scale intelligence scores in the BMD population. Abbreviations used: CI = confidence interval; p = significance level; SMD = standard mean difference; SD = standard deviation [11,13,41,42,105,115,120,].
Brainsci 12 01544 g003
Figure 4. Funnel plot of full-scale intelligence scores in the DMD population.
Figure 4. Funnel plot of full-scale intelligence scores in the DMD population.
Brainsci 12 01544 g004
Figure 5. Funnel plot of full-scale intelligence scores in the BMD population.
Figure 5. Funnel plot of full-scale intelligence scores in the BMD population.
Brainsci 12 01544 g005
Table 1. Weighted mean effect size meta-analysis.
Table 1. Weighted mean effect size meta-analysis.
95% CI Homogeneity
knMLLULpQI2
DMD FSIQ32123484.7682.8186.70<0.00169.0955.10
BMD FSIQ710192.1186.7197.500.1210.0840.50
DMD VIQ35110784.2183.4485.950.0351.2733.70
BMD VIQ69190.8182.4599.700.0213.3462.50
DMD PIQ32107588.8287.5591.05<0.0158.8847.30
BMD PIQ69189.1877.9395.830.0214.0264.30
Cotton * FSIQ32114580.20
Note: * refers to the review of Cotton et al. (2001). Abbreviations used: BMD = Becker muscular disorder; DMD = Duchenne muscular disorder; FSIQ = full-scale intelligence quotient; LL = lower limit; M = mean; p = significance level; PIQ = performal intelligence quotient; Q = heterogeneity statistic; UL = upper limit; VIQ = verbal intelligence quotient.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Weerkamp, P.M.M.; Mol, E.M.; Sweere, D.J.J.; Schrans, D.G.M.; Vermeulen, R.J.; Klinkenberg, S.; Hurks, P.P.M.; Hendriksen, J.G.M. Wechsler Scale Intelligence Testing in Males with Dystrophinopathies: A Review and Meta-Analysis. Brain Sci. 2022, 12, 1544. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci12111544

AMA Style

Weerkamp PMM, Mol EM, Sweere DJJ, Schrans DGM, Vermeulen RJ, Klinkenberg S, Hurks PPM, Hendriksen JGM. Wechsler Scale Intelligence Testing in Males with Dystrophinopathies: A Review and Meta-Analysis. Brain Sciences. 2022; 12(11):1544. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci12111544

Chicago/Turabian Style

Weerkamp, Pien M. M., Eva M. Mol, Dirk J. J. Sweere, Debby G. M. Schrans, R. Jeroen Vermeulen, Sylvia Klinkenberg, Petra P. M. Hurks, and Jos G. M. Hendriksen. 2022. "Wechsler Scale Intelligence Testing in Males with Dystrophinopathies: A Review and Meta-Analysis" Brain Sciences 12, no. 11: 1544. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci12111544

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop