Next Article in Journal
Effect of Sensory Deprivation of Nasal Respiratory on Behavior of C57BL/6J Mice
Previous Article in Journal
Cerebrolysin Prevents Brain Injury in a Mouse Model of Liver Damage
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Body Schema Self-Awareness and Related Dream Content Modifications in Amputees Due to Cancer

1
Centro di Medicina del Sonno, Dipartimento di Neuroscienze, Università di Torino, 10126 Torino, Italy
2
SC Chirurgia Oncologica Ortopedica, AOU Città della Salute e della Scienza di Torino, Presidio CTO, 10126 Torino, Italy
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Brain Sci. 2021, 11(12), 1625; https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci11121625
Submission received: 29 October 2021 / Revised: 6 December 2021 / Accepted: 8 December 2021 / Published: 9 December 2021
(This article belongs to the Section Neurorehabilitation)

Abstract

:
Purpose: the evaluation of body image perception, pain coping strategies, and dream content, together with phantom limb and telescoping phenomena in patients with sarcoma who underwent surgery for limb amputation. Material and Methods: consecutive outpatients were evaluated at T0 (within 3 weeks after surgery) and T1 (4–6 months after surgery) as follows: demographic and clinical data collection; the Groningen Questionnaire Problems after Arm Amputation; the West Haven-Yale Multidimensional Pain Inventory; the Body Image Concern Inventory, a clinical trial to identify telescoping; and a weekly diary of dreams. Dream contents were coded according to the Hall and Van de Castle coding system. Results: Twenty patients completed the study (15 males and 5 females, mean age: 53.9 ± 24.6, education: 7.8 ± 3.4). All subjects experienced phantom limb and 35% of them experienced telescoping soon after surgery, and 25% still after 4–6 months. Both at T0 and T1, that half of the subjects reported dreams about still having their missing limbs. At T1 the patients’ perceptions of being able to deal with problems were lower, and pain and its interference in everyday life were higher yet associated with significant engagement in everyday activities and an overall good mood. The dream content analysis highlighted that males were less worried about health problems soon after amputation, and women showed more initial difficulties that seemed to be resolved after 4–6 months after surgery. Conclusions: The dream content analysis may improve clinicians’ ability to support their patients during their therapeutic course.

1. Introduction

Over the years, many authors have defined body schema, body image, and body awareness in different ways. Body image is referred to the mental representation of the body and its physical features [1,2]. It is a conscious image or representation of the self, a subjective individual’s picture of their own body. The concept of body image is referred to the body as it is perceived in the immediate consciousness. It includes the conceptual construction of the body, and the emotional attitude and conscious feelings about the body itself [3]. The construction and the modification of the body’s internal representation are linked with the surrounding environment components as either cognitive, perceptual, affective, or behavioral [4]. The body schema is a non-conscious performance of the body [3] that is actively triggered by certain internal or environmental events and cues [5]. Indeed, in this performance the body acquires a certain organization or style in its relation with its environment. For example, it appropriates certain habitual postures and movements. The body schema is an active, operative performance of the body rather than an image of the existing parts of the body. “It is the body as it actively integrates it position and responses in the environment” [3].
The self-concept schema depends on “cognitive generalizations about oneself derived from past experiences”. Such experiences lead and guide the processing of self-related information acquired from social knowledge [6]. Self-schemas ‘‘reflect one’s core and affect laden assumptions or beliefs about the importance and influence of one’s appearance in life, including the centrality of appearance to one’s sense of self’’ [1,2].
Differences have been found among categories in determining self-schema. Men’s and women’s awareness levels of body parts are diverse. For men, body awareness has more permanence compared to women’s, meaning that they are more aware of their body parts. Additionally, left-handed people seem to be also more certain in perceiving their body senses, compared to right-handed people, while the latter showed less body divergence, meaning they have fewer clearness levels of images of different body parts [7]. Body awareness may be considered the conceptualization of body image or the complexity of the principles of body schema, body posture, and body position concerning spatial motions; it is the basis of personality [8].
Information about body image is differently interpreted by each individual, also concerning a potentially worrisome physical appearance [5]. Some people cognitively interpret information in accordance with their psychologically important body-related concerns, and, conversely, cognitive dysfunctions can lead to medical problems with a bidirectional relationship. Information processing can be dysfunctional when influenced by medical pathologies such as eating disorders and gender dysphoria [9,10], oncologic disease [11,12,13,14], HIV/AIDS [15,16], dermatological disturbances [17], burns [18], amputations [19,20,21,22], permanent scars [23] or interesting phenomena such as phantom limb [24], and telescoping [8,25]. Phantom limb sensations (PLS) are a well-known phenomenon [26,27] in which an amputee continues to experience a normally limbed body with the sensation that an amputated or missing limb is still part of the body. Telescoping is characterized by the experience of a gradual proximal migration or shortening of the amputated limb, as perceived by the patient.
These phenomena can significantly alter the function of the body and can affect the body image and, ultimately, psychosocial well-being [28] and the quality of life. For these reasons, understanding perceptual distortions in pathological conditions is important to addressing body image concerns and helping suffering people in having a content and productive life [3,29,30].
In amputees, phantom limb sensations and telescoping are widely present with most individuals experiencing some type of phantom phenomena at some point, post-amputation [26,27,31,32,33,34,35,36].
The phenomenon of the phantom limb may be considered as an alteration of the sensory-motor pathway. Its origins are not fully understood. The contributions of the central nervous system (CNS) and/or the peripheral nervous system (PNS) are still under debate. Recent findings suggest that both peripheral and central mechanisms, including neuroplastic changes in cortical neural circuits, can contribute to PLS and in particular to phantom limb pain (PLP) [37]. Currently, the most commonly suggested CNS theory is the cortical remapping theory (CRT), in which the brain is believed to respond to limb loss by reorganizing somatosensory maps [38], and the PNS may work in conjunction with the CNS to cause and maintain the persistence of PLP.
The human brain has the power to undergo plastic changes after an alteration in cortical circuits, which are, for a large part, responsible for the conscious awareness of the body, so-called body image. Can unconscious or less conscious processes, that contribute defining body schema, undergo similar adapting changes [39], and how can non-conscious awareness be investigated?
Dream content analysis may be considered an indicator of the internalization of the above-mentioned modifications.
A single definition of dream and dreaming is not possible. Dreaming is, at least in part, a mental experience that can be described during waking consciousness [40].
Dreaming should not be exclusively defined as a non-conscious electrophysiological state. Neurobiological theories of dreams state that the meaning of dreams should be considered clearly “transparent” and not elaborated [41,42]. Hobson affirmed that there are common formal aspects concerning everybody’s dreams, and there are some content differences related to age, gender, personality characteristics, and cross-cultural differences [43].
Domhoff hypothesized a continuity between dreams and waking life in the sense that “the concerns people express in their dreams are the concerns they have in waking life” [44,45]. Additionally, Revonsuo argued that dream content is not random, but organized and selective, and indicated that dream content is consistently and powerfully modulated by certain types of waking experiences [46].
Following Domhoff’s hypothesis, the analysis of dream reports may be useful in evaluating the changes of mental body image and self-perception in dreams after patients have experienced different kinds of disease-implicating body disfigurements. Calvin S. Hall and Robert L. Van de Castle [47,48] developed a specific coding system to study dream content. The Hall and Van de Castle system treats a dream report as a story or play in which there are several empirical categories.
Considering that both innate factors and life experiences may affect the body representation in dreams, they assumed that “the frequency with which a dream element appears reveals the concerns and interests of the dreamer”, providing the opportunity “to link dream content with the waking thoughts and behavior of the dreamer” [49].
Many studies evaluated the dream content in healthy volunteers to find common features in people’s dream content [44,45,49,50].
Women have been reported to recall their dreams more often than men. Sexual themes arise more often in men’s dreams, as do aggressions, whereas women’s dreams have more depressive contents [51,52].
Studies on dream content in amputated or surgically treated patients, analyzing the alterations of the physical aspect and their impact on the quality of life, mostly reported that a majority of amputees continue to dream of themselves with an intact body [39,53,54,55,56]. Our previous research demonstrated that dream content in patients who have recently undergone surgery for breast cancer is different both from healthy subjects dream content and from that of the same patients 3 months after surgery, especially for what concerns body image. This evidence underlines the importance of considering the timing of the assessment after amputation [14].
The aim of this study is the evaluation of body image perception, pain coping strategies, dream content, and their possible modifications over time, together with phantom limb and telescoping phenomena in patients affected by sarcoma who have undergone surgery for limb amputation, within six months after surgery.

2. Materials and Methods

Sleep medicine experts evaluated all consecutive outpatients referred to the SC Chirurgia Oncologica Ortopedica- Città della Salute e della Scienza over one year. Inclusion criteria were the diagnosis of bone or soft tissue sarcoma, minimum age 14 years old. Exclusion criteria were impaired cognitive ability (MMSE < 24), psychiatric disease, ray amputations (or smaller), or previous amputations.
Subjects were evaluated at T0 (within 3 weeks after surgery) and T1 (4–6 months after surgery). The evaluation included the following: the Groningen Questionnaire Problems after Arm Amputation (GQPAA), to describe frequency and type of phantom sensation, phantom pain, and stump pain, medical treatment received and their effect, and prosthetic use [26]; a weekly diary of dreams; and the West Haven-Yale Multidimensional Pain Inventory (WHYMPI), an inventory used in the assessment of clinical pain to examine the impact of pain on patients’ lives, the responses of others to the patients communications of pain, and the extent to which patients participate in common daily activities [57]. The WHYMPI is a 52-item, 12-scale inventory that is divided into three parts. Part I includes five scales designed to measure important dimensions of the chronic pain experience including (1) perceived interference of pain in vocational, social/recreational, and family/marital functioning, (2) support or concern from spouse or significant other, (3) pain severity, (4) perceived life control, and (5) affective distress. Part II assesses patients’ perceptions of the degree to which spouses or significant others display Solicitous, Distracting, or Negative responses to their pain behaviors and complaints. Part III assesses patients report of the frequency with which they engage in four categories of common everyday activities: Household Chores, Outdoor Work, Activities Away from Home, and Social Activities. An additional General Activity scale score, obtained from the combination of all four activity scale scores, has been added.
The Body Image Concern Inventory (BICI), a brief self-report measure of dysmorphic concern [58] a clinical trial to examine the location of the phantom limb useful to identify telescoping and changes in body schema [8]. The first evaluation of all patients also included demographic (age, instruction level, employment, family) and clinical (treatments, oncologic diagnosis) data collection
Dream contents were coded according to the Hall and Van de Castle (1966) coding system. Contents are summarized in the following output categories: Male/Female, Familiarity Friends Family Dead & Imaginary Animal Aggression/Friendliness Befriender Aggressor Physical Aggression Indoor Setting Familiar Setting, Self-Negativity, Bodily Misfortunes, Negative Emotions, Dreamer-Involved Success, Torso/Anatomy, Aggression, Friendliness, Sexuality, Misfortune, Good Fortune, Success, Failure, and Striving.
Dreams were recorded using a seven-day dream diary and blindly scored (mixing T0 and T1 dream reports) by a sleep medicine board-certified psychologist; difficult or ambiguous issues were resolved during in-group discussions. The alphanumeric codes were uploaded into a DreamSAT spreadsheet, which provided frequencies for the total series of dream reports and percentage calculations [49,59]. Coded dream data also were statistically compared to normative data using Adam Schneider’s DreamSAT (available online: www.dreamresearch.net, accessed on: 14 January 2021).
The Local Ethical Committee approved the protocol, and all patients (or parents whether under the age) signed the informed consent form.
Statistical analyses were carried out with IBM SPSS 27.0 for Windows, performing a Wilcoxon Test, the Chi-square test (X2), and contingencies tables. Results are presented in terms of mean ± SD with significance levels at p0.05.

3. Results

Twenty-seven subjects were enrolled. Seven out of 27 patients did not complete the T1 evaluation—two due to death, two did not show up at the appointment, and three were unable to attend the interview due to their disability. Twenty patients completed the study (15 males and 5 females). The mean age was 53.9 ± 24.6, education (years) 7.8 ± 3.4, and 10 patients had a diagnosis of soft tissue sarcoma and 10 of bone sarcoma. Eleven underwent lower limb amputation and 9 upper limb amputation, and pre-surgery pain was present in 13 out of 20 subjects. The mean number of weeks after amputation was at T0 2.6 ± 1.0 and at T1 19.5 ± 4.1.
Additional clinical data and Wilcoxon test results performed on BICI and WHYMPI are shown in Table 1.
All our subjects experience PLS at some point in their clinical history, and 1/3 of them experienced telescoping soon after surgery, with 1/4 still after 4–6 months. Both at T0 and T1, that half of the subjects reported dreams about still having their missing limbs.
Results showed significant differences at some West Haven-Yale Multidimensional Pain Inventory subscales.
Four-six months after surgery, significantly higher scores were obtained on two scales measuring important dimensions of how pain affects patient lives (interference and pain severity) and on patients’ reports of the frequency with which they engage in common everyday activities (house chores, activity away, social activity, and general activity).
Additionally, scales about patients perception of being able to deal with and control their problems (life control) and affective distress, and about the support or concern from spouse or significant other (support), obtained lower scores, along with patients’ perceptions of the degree to which significant others react and respond to them when they know they are in pain, and display negative response to their behaviors and complaints (negative response).
Crossing pre-surgery pain, amputation types and stump pain with telescoping both at T0 and T1, significance (p = 0.016), has only been found between pre-surgery pain and early post-amputation telescoping.
DreamSAT
One hundred and twenty-three dreams have been collected and codified using the Hall and Van de Castle method. Statistical analyses were performed by the automated Dream Data Entry System and statistical analysis tool DreamSat (spreadsheet for Microsoft Excel).
Crossing the number of reported dreams at T0 and T1 in both males and females showed that a statistically significant (p < 0.05) higher number of dreams was reported by males (T0M/F = 44/18, T1M/F = 47/14) both at T0 and T1.
Table 2 and Table 3 show DreamSAT dream contents results for Males and Females vs. norms, both at T0 and T1, and Males and Females T0 vs. T1.
We found that males at both T0 and T1 had fewer negative features (such as aggression and negative emotions) and sexuality and more good fortune- and family-related content in their dreams compared to normative data. In addition, at T1 they showed no torso-anatomy references.
Females at T0 showed more familiar settings in bodily misfortune and dreamer-involved success, and less friends, friendliness, aggression, and failure, compared to norms. Besides, there were no torso-anatomy references.
Females, some months after surgery, showed less aggression, self-negativity, negative emotions, torso-anatomy content, success, and dreamer’s success compared to norms.
Males showed, substantially, no significant differences between T0 and T1, whereas women at T0 showed more bodily misfortune, aggression, and familiar settings.

4. Discussion

The present study analyzes the dream content in amputees, using the Hall and Van de Castle coding system, to understand the possible internalization of body modifications due to amputations and some interesting phenomena such as phantom limb and telescoping that may have significant implications on the quality of life. The results of this study mostly confirm previous literature data on phantom limb phenomenology: all our subjects experience PLS at some point in their clinical history, and 1/3 of them experienced telescoping soon after surgery, with 1/4 still after 4–6 months.
The pain inventory showed that, at T1, the patients’ perception of being able to deal with problems is lower and pain and its interference on everyday life is higher, meaning that physical sensation and phantom limb pain are still important issues also after 4–6 months from surgery. Nevertheless, the patients’ reports of the frequency with which they engage in four categories of common everyday activities (house chores, activity away, social activities, and general activities) is higher at 4–6 months after surgery, as they report an overall good mood. That is probably also because significant others display less negative responses to their pain complaints.
Besides, the significant association between pre-surgery pain and telescoping at T0 may mean that pre-surgery pain is predictive of telescoping soon after amputation.
In literature, we found suggestions that pre-amputation pain might predispose amputees to PLP [60]. Recent work has suggested that features related to the amputation, such as pre-amputation pain, stump pain, sleep disturbances, and/or diabetic/traumatic causes of amputation, may be associated with PLP development [35]. These data may support an association also between pre-surgery pain and telescoping, an association that, however, needs to be further evaluated.
In normal conditions, various factors may influence body image: Body mass index (BMI), family, peers, society, media, culture, self-esteem, gender, age, marital status, education level, smoking status, alcohol consumption, physical activity, weight control behavior, and spirituality [61,62]. For example, as we previously stated, for men, body awareness has more permanence compared to for women, meaning that they are more aware of their body parts. In addition, left-handed people seem to be also more certain in perceiving their body senses, compared to right-handed people, while the latter showed less body divergence, meaning that they have fewer clearness levels of images of different body parts [8].
Pathological conditions can have a significant impact on the body’s internal representation, as well. Indeed, body representation and self-perception rely on cognitive schema and emotional experiences that are strongly linked with what happens in everyday life.
For this reason, self-awareness can change during life due to life experiences and pathological conditions that can occur.
Although there has been much progress in the surgical treatment of bone and soft tissue sarcomas, amputation is unfortunately still needed in 5–10% of cases [63] of bone and soft tissue tumors.
As we previously said, phantom limb sensations are very common in amputees [8,26,31,32,33,34,35], and post-amputation pain such as phantom limb pain (PLP) and residual limb pain (RLP) may have negative effects on a person’s well-being.
Phantom limb sensations may include pain, itching, movements, abnormal shape or position, warmth, and cold or electric sensations. Some kind of phantom sensations are present in virtually all patients with limb amputation and may be explained by three theories: the peripheral one theorizes that the impulses carried to the central nervous system, coming from the nerve endings in the stump, are perceived as pertaining to the amputated limb [8,64]; the central theory affirms that visual sensory, tactile and postural impressions contribute to developing a life-long body schema independently from peripheral sensory impulses and define phantom limb as a conscious process [8,65,66]; and the mixed theory proposes that both these factors are combined [67]. Following these theories, the perception of the phantom limb phenomenon may be considered as an alteration of the sensory-motor pathway.
If PLS involve the majority of amputees and are still an important issue several months after amputation, as we found in our study, a different and deeper way to analyze the impact of these phenomena on patients life may be useful.
If, during waking life, an amputee still feels the presence of the amputated limb, what happens while dreaming? Can the dream be considered a reliable and precocious indicator of what happens in the dreamer’s emotional life?
Over the years, some studies have analyzed the dream content using a standardized coding method [51,68,69,70,71]. The continuity hypothesis of dreams suggests that the presence of aversive experiences in the waking state should be reflected in dream content [55,72].
In literature, most studies reported that a majority of amputees continue to dream about themselves with an intact body [36,45,46,47,48] even if, in some cases, there is a difference between how the phantom limb is perceived in the waking state and in dreams [73].
Bekrater-Bodmann and colleagues in 2015, demonstrated that, although only a minority of dreams in their sample contained bodily impairments, there is a positive relationship between post-amputation pain during wake and the recall of an impaired body representation in dreams [55]. Our previous research demonstrated that dream contents in subjects who have recently undergone surgery for breast cancer are different from that of healthy subjects and from that of the same patients three months after the surgery [14].
The dream analysis, in the present study, showed both at T0 and T1 that half of the subjects reported dreams about still having their missing limbs. As we previously said, this evidence underlines the importance of considering the timing of the assessment after amputation. Males both at T0 and T1 reported a statistically significantly higher number of dreams. That is maybe because, as we previously said, men’s body awareness has more permanence compared to women’s [74].
We also found that males, at both T0 and T1, had fewer negative features (such as aggression and negative emotions) and more good fortune and an abundance of family members in their dreams compared to normative data indicating fewer concerns about the surgery, the clinical outcome, and the physical appearance. In addition, at T1 they showed no torso-anatomy references, an indicator of possible disturbance of bodily image (denial) or a real absence of body-image issues.
Females at T0 showed more familiar settings, bodily misfortune, and dreamer-involved success, and fewer friends, friendliness, aggression, and failure compared to norms. Besides, there were no torso-anatomy references.
Female, some months after surgery, showed fewer aggression, self-negativity, negative emotions, torso-anatomy contents, dreamer’s success, and more good fortune compared to norms. Women’s self-perception appears less frightened, and dominated by negative emotions, maybe suggesting a good self-restructuring which could be tied to the ordeal of overcoming cancer.
Yet the complete absence of body-related topics in women’s dreams both at T0 and T1 could reflect clear neglect about the body-shape change.
T0 and T1 in males showed, substantially, no significant differences, whereas women at T0 showed more bodily misfortune, aggression, and familiar settings. According to Domhoff’s perspective, the presence of an abundance of family members and settings and increased misfortunes could be related to a dreamer’s maladjustment problems. As we previously said, these concerns seemed to be better managed some months after surgery.
It seems possible that males were less worried about the health problem from the beginning, and women showed more initial difficulties that seemed to be resolved after 4–6 months after surgery.
As explained by Revonsuo’s (2000) perspective and Ernest Hartmann’s (1996) theory, the function of dreaming is to “contextualize a dominant emotion of the dreamer”, making “connections more broadly than waking in the nets of mind” [14,75]. All these considerations are in agreement with Domhoff’s assertion that “dreams express several key aspects of people’s conceptual systems” [44].
Some limits in our study should be underlined. Difficulties in recruitment in this research field resulted in a small sample of patients and consequent limited statistical analysis power. Additionally, the wider SD we obtained at T1 was because we tried to reevaluate the patients during medical follow-up visits that were differently distributed in a large period of time. It was impossible to reach the patients during an additional visit. This could have missed possible additional changes in the body perception at 4 and 6 months after the amputation.
We also did not consider the possible prosthesis reconstruction, which could affect some variables mostly regarding the subjective sensation of a possible return to “integrity”. Some of the subjects in our study refused prosthesis, some were not eligible, and some of them were not in the process yet, so we did not have enough data to collect and analyze.
In addition, considering the persistence, in half of the patients, of the amputated limb in their dreams, it would be important to further re-test the patients and plan an extended follow-up.

5. Conclusions

The analysis of dream content, which could reveal the expression of inner self-perception through a novel analysis method, could significantly improve psycho-oncologists’ and clinicians’ ability to support their patients during their therapeutic course. The persistence of an intact self-image, even 4–6 months after surgery, in some of our patients’ dreams may suggest an ongoing plasticity process that has to be better evaluated to implement a timely intervention. Indeed, providing proper psychological support is essential in patients who have to face important challenges in self-perception and possible significant changes in the quality of their lives.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, A.C., A.G. and R.P.; methodology, M.B.; formal analysis, A.G. and A.C.; investigation, A.G. and M.B.; resources, R.M.; data curation A.C. and A.G.; writing—original draft preparation, A.G.; writing—review and editing, A.G. and A.C.; supervision, A.C. and R.P. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted according to the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the Local Ethics Committee of Città della Salute e della Scienza di Torino (Prot. N. 0009841 09/02/2012).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The data presented in this study are available on request from the corresponding author.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to express their gratitude to the patients who have agreed to participate in the study.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Cash, T.F. Cognitive-behavioral perspectives on body image. In Encyclopedia of Body Image and Human Appearance; Elsevier Inc.: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2012; Volume 1, pp. 334–342. [Google Scholar]
  2. Cash, T.F.; Smolak, L. Body Image: A handbook of Science, Practice, and Prevention; Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 2011. [Google Scholar]
  3. Gallagher, S. Body image and body schema: A conceptual clarification. J. Mind Behav. 1986, 7, 541–554. Available online: https://www.jstor.org/stable/43853233?seq=1#metadata_info_tab_contents (accessed on 25 November 2021).
  4. Yamamotova, A.; Bulant, J.; Bocek, V.; Papezova, H. Dissatisfaction with own body makes patients with eating disorders more sensitive to pain. J. Pain Res. 2017, 10, 1667–1675. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  5. Jakatdar, T.A.; Cash, T.F.; Engle, E.K. Body-image thought processes: The development and initial validation of the Assessment of Body-Image Cognitive Distortions. Body Image 2006, 3, 325–333. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Markus, H. Self-schemata and processing information about the self. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1977, 35, 63–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Fisher, S.; Greenberg, R.P.; Reihman, J. Body Perception and Somatic Discomfort in Relation to Right- Versus Left-Handedness. Cortex 1984, 20, 285–294. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Mayer, Á.; Kudar, K.; Bretz, K.; Tihanyi, J. Body schema and body awareness of amputees. Prosthet. Orthot. Int. 2008, 32, 363–382. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  9. McLean, S.A.; Paxton, S.J. Body Image in the Context of Eating Disorders. Psychiatr. Clin. N. Am. 2019, 42, 145–156. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Milano, W.; Ambrosio, P.; Carizzone, F.; de Biasio, V.; Foggia, G.; Capasso, A. Gender Dysphoria, Eating Disorders and Body Image: An Overview. Endocrine Metab. Immune Disord. Drug Targets 2019, 20, 518–524. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Graboyes, E.M.; Hill, E.G.; Marsh, C.H.; Maurer, S.; Day, T.A.; Sterba, K.R. Body Image Disturbance in Surgically Treated Head and Neck Cancer Patients: A Prospective Cohort Pilot Study. Otolaryngol. Head Neck Surg. 2019, 161, 105–110. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Kolodziejczyk, A.; Pawlowski, T. Negative body image in breast cancer patients. Adv. Clin. Exp. Med. 2019, 28, 1137–1142. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Fingeret, M.C.; Teo, I.; Epner, D.E. Managing body image difficulties of adult cancer patients: Lessons from available research. Cancer 2014, 120, 633–641. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  14. Giordano, A.; Francese, V.; Peila, E.; Tribolo, A.; Airoldi, M.; Torta, R.; Mutani, R.; Cicolin, A. Dream content changes in women after mastectomy: An initial study of body imagery after body-disfiguring surgery. Dreaming 2012, 22, 115–123. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  15. Martinez, S.M.; Kemper, C.A.; Diamond, C.; Wagner, G. Body image in patients with HIV/AIDS: Assessment of a new psychometric measure and its medical correlates. AIDS Patient Care STDS 2005, 19, 150–156. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Martins, C.; Coelho, F.M.D.C.; Pinheiro, R.T.; Motta, J.V.D.S.; De Souza, L.D.M.; Pinheiro, C.A.T.; Kelbert, E.F.; Souza, M.S.; Pinheiro, L.M.D.S.; Pinheiro, K.A.T. People living with HIV/AIDS: Body image and its important associations with mental health and BMI. Psychol. Health Med. 2020, 25, 1020–1028. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Cotterill, J.A. Dermatological non-disease: A common and potentially fatal disturbance of cutaneous body image. Br. J. Dermatol. 1981, 104, 611–619. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Pellard, S. Body image and acute burn injuries: A literature review. J. Wound Care 2006, 15, 129–132. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Gozaydinoglu, S.; Hosbay, Z.; Durmaz, H. Body image perception, compliance with a prosthesis and cognitive performance in transfemoral amputees. Acta Orthop. Traumatol. Turc. 2019, 53, 221–225. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Senra, H.; Oliveira, R.A.; Leal, I.; Vieira, C. Beyond the body image: A qualitative study on how adults experience lower limb amputation. Clin. Rehabil. 2012, 26, 180–191. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Burçak, B.; Kesikburun, B.; Köseoğlu, B.F.; Öken, Ö.; Doğan, A. Quality of life, body image, and mobility in lower-limb amputees using high-tech prostheses: A pragmatic trial. Ann. Phys. Rehabil. Med. 2021, 64, 101405. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Akkaya, N.; Atalay, N.Ş.; Selcuk, S.T.; Akkaya, S.; Ardiç, F. Impact of body image on quality of life and mood in mastectomized patients and amputees in turkey. Asian Pacific J. Cancer Prev. 2011, 12, 2669–2673. Available online: https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/22320972/ (accessed on 11 October 2021).
  23. Bennis, I.; Thys, S.; Filali, H.; de Brouwere, V.; Sahibi, H.; Boelaert, M. Psychosocial impact of scars due to cutaneous leishmaniasis on high school students in Errachidia province, Morocco. Infect. Dis. Poverty 2017, 6, 46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  24. Price, E.H. A critical review of congenital phantom limb cases and a developmental theory for the basis of body image. Conscious. Cogn. 2006, 15, 310–322. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Montoya, P.; Larbig, W.; Grulke, N.; Flor, H.; Taub, E.; Birbaumer, N. The relationship of phantom limb pain to other phantom limb phenomena in upper extremity amputees. Pain 1997, 72, 87–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Kooijman, C.M.; Dijkstra, P.U.; Geertzen, J.H.B.; Elzinga, A.; van der Schans, C.P. Phantom pain and phantom sensations in upper limb amputees: An epidemiological study. Pain 2000, 87, 33–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Ehde, D.M.; Czerniecki, J.M.; Smith, D.G.; Campbell, K.M.; Edwards, W.T.; Jensen, M.P.; Robinson, L.R. Chronic phantom sensations, phantom pain, residual limb pain, and other regional pain after lower limb amputation. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 2000, 81, 1039–1044. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Cash, T.F. Body image: Past, present, and future. Body Image 2004, 1, 1–5. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Hosseini, S.A.; Padhy, R.K. Body Image Distortion. StatPearls 2019. Available online: https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/31536191/ (accessed on 11 October 2021).
  30. Sadibolova, R.; Ferrè, E.R.; Linkenauger, S.A.; Longo, M.R. Distortions of perceived volume and length of body parts. Cortex 2019, 111, 74–86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Kaur, A.; Guan, Y. Phantom limb pain: A literature review. Chin. J. Traumatol. Engl. Ed. 2018, 21, 366–368. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Weeks, S.R.; Anderson-Barnes, V.C.; Tsao, J.W. Phantom limb pain: Theories and therapies. Neurologist 2010, 16, 277–286. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Finnoff, J. Differentiation and treatment of phantom sensation, phantom pain, and residual-limb pain. J. Am. Podiatr. Med. Assoc. 2001, 91, 23–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Kern, U.; Busch, V.; Rockland, M.; Kohl, M.; Birklein, F. Prävalenz und Risikofaktoren von Phantomschmerzen und Phantomwahrnehmungen in Deutschland: Eine bundesweite Befragung. Schmerz 2009, 23, 479–488. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Stankevicius, A.; Wallwork, S.B.; Summers, S.J.; Hordacre, B.; Stanton, T.R. Prevalence and incidence of phantom limb pain, phantom limb sensations and telescoping in amputees: A systematic rapid review. Eur. J. Pain 2021, 25, 23–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Saetta, G.; Cognolato, M.; Atzori, M.; Faccio, D.; Giacomino, K.; Hager, A.G.M.; Tiengo, C.; Bassetto, F.; Müller, H.; Brugger, P. Gaze, behavioral, and clinical data for phantom limbs after hand amputation from 15 amputees and 29 controls. Sci. Data 2020, 7, 60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  37. Flor, H.; Andoh, J. Origin of phantom limb pain: A dynamic network perspective. Neuroforum 2017, 23, 111–116. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Flor, H.; Elbert, T.; Knecht, S.; Wienbruch, C.; Pantev, C.; Birbaumers, N.; Larbig, W.; Taub, E. Phantom-limb pain as a perceptual correlate of cortical reorganization following arm amputation. Nature 1995, 375, 482–484. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Mulder, T.; Hochstenbach, J.; Dijkstra, P.U.; Geertzen, J.H.B. Born to adapt, but not in your dreams. Conscious. Cogn. 2008, 17, 1266–1271. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  40. Pagel, J.F.; Blagrove, M.; Levin, R.; States, B.; Stickgold, B.; White, S. Definitions of Dream: A Paradigm for Comparing Field Descriptive Specific Studies of Dream. Dreaming 2001, 11, 195–202. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Hobson, J.A. REM sleep and dreaming: Towards a theory of protoconsciousness. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2009, 10, 803–814. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Hobson, J.A. Ego ergo sum: Toward a psychodynamic neurology. Contemp. Psychoanal. 2013, 49, 142–164. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Hobson, A.; Kahn, D. Dream content: Individual and generic aspects. Conscious. Cogn. 2007, 16, 850–858. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Domhoff, G.W. The Scientific Study of Dreams: Neural Networks, Cognitive Development, and Content Analysis; APA Press: Washington, DC, USA, 2003; Volume 209. [Google Scholar]
  45. Domhoff, G.W.; Meyer-Gomes, K.; Schredl, M. Dreams as the Expression of Conceptions and Concerns: A Comparison of German and American College Students. Imagin. Cogn. Personal. 2016, 25, 269–282. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Revonsuo, A. The reinterpretation of dreams: An evolutionary hypothesis of the function of dreaming. Behav. Brain Sci. 2000, 23, 877–901. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Hall, C.S.; van de Castle, R.L. The Content Analysis of Dreams; Appleton-Century-Crofts: New York, NY, USA, 1966. [Google Scholar]
  48. Domhoff, G.W. New Directions in the Study of Dream Content Using the Hall and Van de Castle Coding System. Dreaming 1999, 9, 115–137. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Domhoff, G.W.; Hall, C.S. Finding meaning in Dreams: A Quantitative Approach; Plenum Press: New York, NY, USA, 1996; p. 356. [Google Scholar]
  50. DreamResearch.net: A Widower’s Dreams. Available online: https://dreams.ucsc.edu/Library/domhoff_2008b.html (accessed on 13 October 2021).
  51. Schredl, M.; Doll, E. Emotions in Diary Dreams. Conscious. Cogn. 1998, 7, 634–646. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Mathes, J.; Schredl, M. Gender differences in dream content: Are they related to personality? Int. J. Dream Res. 2013, 6, 34–39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Brugger, P. The phantom limb in dreams. Conscious. Cogn. 2008, 17, 1272–1278. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  54. Vetrugno, R.; Arnulf, I.; Montagna, P. Disappearance of ‘phantom limb’ and amputated arm usage during dreaming in REM sleep behaviour disorder. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 2008, 79, 481–483. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Bekrater-Bodmann, R.; Schredl, M.; Diers, M.; Reinhard, I.; Foell, J.; Trojan, J.; Fuchs, X.; Flor, H. Post-amputation pain is associated with the recall of an impaired body representation in dreams-results from a nation-wide survey on limb amputees. PLoS ONE 2015, 10, e0119552. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  56. Alessandria, M.; Vetrugno, R.; Cortelli, P.; Montagna, P. Normal body scheme and absent phantom limb experience in amputees while dreaming. Conscious. Cogn. 2011, 20, 1831–1834. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Kerns, R.D.; Turk, D.C.; Rudy, T.E. The West Haven-Yale Multidimensional Pain Inventory (WHYMPI). Pain 1985, 23, 345–356. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Littleton, H.L.; Axsom, D.; Pury, C.L.S. Development of the body image concern inventory. Behav. Res. Ther. 2005, 43, 229–241. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  59. Domhoff, G.W.; Schneider, A. New Rationales and Methods for Quantitative Dream Research Outside the Laboratory. Sleep 1998, 21, 398–404. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  60. Limakatso, K.; Bedwell, G.J.; Madden, V.J.; Parker, R. The prevalence of phantom limb pain and associated risk factors in people with amputations: A systematic review protocol. Syst. Rev. 2019, 8, 17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  61. Shoraka, H.; Amirkafi, A.; Garrusi, B. Review of body image and some of contributing factors in Iranian population. Int. J. Prev. Med. 2019, 10, 19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Jang, H.Y.; Ahn, J.W.; Jeon, M.K. Factors affecting body image discordance amongst Korean adults aged 19–39 years. Osong Public Health Res. Perspect. 2018, 9, 197–206. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Mavrogenis, A.F.; Coll-Mesa, L.; Gonzalez-Gaitan, M.; Ucelay-Gomez, R.; Fabri, N.; Ruggieri, P.; Papagelopoulos, P.J. Criteria and outcome of limb salvage surgery. J. BUON 2011, 16, 617–626. Available online: https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/22331712/ (accessed on 13 October 2021).
  64. Carlen, P.L.; Wall, P.D.; Nadvorna, H.; Steinbach, T. Phantom limbs and related phenomena in recent traumatic amputations. Neurology 1978, 28, 211–217. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Head, H.; Holmes, G. Sensory disturbances from cerebral lesions. Brain 1911, 34, 102–254. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  66. Jacome, D. Phantom Itching Relieved by Scratching Phantom Feet. JAMA J. Am. Med. Assoc. 1978, 240, 2432. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Shukla, G.D.; Sahu, S.C.; Tripathi, R.P.; Gupta, D.K. Phantom limb: A phenomenological study. Br. J. Psychiatry 1982, 141, 54–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Fantini, M.L.; Corona, A.; Clerici, S.; Ferini-Strambi, L. Aggressive dream content without daytime aggressiveness in REM sleep behavior disorder. Neurology 2005, 65, 1010–1015. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Bugalho, P.; Ladeira, F.; Barbosa, R.; Marto, J.P.; Borbinha, C.; Salavisa, M.; da Conceição, L.; Saraiva, M.; Fernandes, M.; Meira, B. Do dreams tell the future? Dream content as a predictor of cognitive deterioration in Parkinson’s disease. J. Sleep Res. 2021, 30, e13163. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Guralnik, O.; Levin, R.; Schmeidler, J. Dreams of personality disordered subjects. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 1999, 187, 40–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Bugalho, P.; Paiva, T. Dream features in the early stages of Parkinson’s Disease. J. Neural Transm. 2011, 118, 1613–1619. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Cicolin, A.; Boffano, M.; Beccuti, G.; Piana, R.; Giordano, A. End-of-life in oncologic patients’ dream content. Brain Sci. 2020, 10, 505. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  73. Frank, B.; Lorenzoni, E. Experiences of phantom limb sensations in dreams. Psychopathology 1989, 22, 182–187. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  74. Fisher, S. Development and Structure of the Body Image; Psychology Press: New York, NY, USA, 2014. [Google Scholar]
  75. Hartmann, E. Outline for a theory on the nature and functions of dreaming. Dreaming 1996, 6, 147–170. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Table 1. Clinical data percentage and Wilcoxon Test results between T0 and T1 questionnaires subscales.
Table 1. Clinical data percentage and Wilcoxon Test results between T0 and T1 questionnaires subscales.
T0 %T1 %
PLS §100100
Stump pain4045
Telescoping3525
Limb in dreams5050
Mean T0Std. Dev. T0Mean T1Std. Dev. T1Sign.
BICI §§_30.4013.2637.6515.780.052
WHY §§§_interference0.870.861.701.620.0009 *
WHY_support3.851.713.431.900.006 *
WHY_pain_sev1.961.372.461.640.002 *
WHY_life_contr2.951.952.281.190.049 *
WHY_aff_distr2.971.811.340.880.000 *
WHY_neg_resp0.660.800.430.660.012 *
WHY_solic_resp3.841.403.491.990.078
WHY_dist_resp2.571.322.751.690.959
WHY_house_chores1.401.572.212.120.039 *
WHY_out_work0.360.900.500.750.064
WHY_act_away1.111.371.911.390.017 *
WHY_soc_act2.131.713.281.270.005 *
WHY_gen_act1.181.132.111.150.005 *
* Statistically significant; § PLS: Phantom Limb Sensations; §§ BICI: Body Image Concern Inventory; §§§ WHY: West Haven-Yale Multidimensional Pain Inventory (WHYMPI) subscales.
Table 2. Male at T0 vs. norms and Female at T0 vs. norms and Male at T1 vs. norms and Female at T1 vs. norms.
Table 2. Male at T0 vs. norms and Female at T0 vs. norms and Male at T1 vs. norms and Female at T1 vs. norms.
Male T0Male Normsp vs.
Male
Female T0Female Normsp vs.
Female
Male
T1
Male Normsp vs.
Males
Female T1Female Normsp vs.
Female
§Characters
Male/Female75%67%0.37113%48%* 0.02278%67%0.23950%48%0.915
Familiarity53%45%0.28250%58%0.50256%45%0.17060%58%0.900
Friends24%31%0.2966%37%** 0.00127%31%0.53440%37%0.802
Family22%12%* 0.04944%19%* 0.03324%12%* 0.03613%19%0.533
Dead and Imaginary2%0%0.2546%1%0.2305%0%* 0.0416%1%0.221
Animal6%6%0.9416%4%0.7935%6%0.66812%4%0.240
§Social Interaction Percents
Aggression/Friendliness57%59%0.82971%51%0.27455%59%0.6930%51%** 0.001
Befriender20%50%0.15250%47%0.93580%50%0.15850%47%0.909
Aggressor64%40%0.11440%33%0.74450%40%0.5570%33%0.223
Physical Aggression23%50%* 0.04433%34%0.9808%50%** 0.0000%34%0.080
§Settings
Indoor Setting38%48%0.34550%61%0.58037%48%0.18767%61%0.737
Familiar Setting73%62%0.340100%79%* 0.03481%62%0.08563%79%0.315
§Self-Concept Percents
Self-Negativity59%65%0.46560%66%0.69867%65%0.71338%66%* 0.046
Bodily Misfortunes48%29%0.097100%35%** 0.00019%29%0.29533%35%0.953
Negative Emotions36%80%** 0.00250%80%0.19277%80%0.75843%80%* 0.036
Dreamer-Involved Success50%51%0.959100%42%* 0.01640%51%0.4120%42%* 0.016
Torso/Anatomy20%31%0.4360%20%* 0.0410%31%** 0.0000%20%** 0.006
§Dreams with at Least One:
Aggression20%47%** 0.00022%44%* 0.04717%47%** 0.00000%44%** 0.000
Friendliness20%38%* 0.01211%42%** 0.00219%38%** 0.00521%42%0.096
Sexuality0%12%** 0.0000%4%0.1120%12%** 0.0000%4%0.159
Misfortune30%36%0.36722%33%0.29638%36%0.77621%33%0.319
Good Fortune20%6%** 0.0056%6%0.99421%6%** 0.00236%6%** 0.003
Success7%15%0.08911%08%0.61413%15%0.6720%8%* 0.039
Failure11%15%0.4500%10%** 0.00819%15%0.51521%10%0.229
Striving18%27%0.17811%15%0.65828%27%0.92321%15%0.515
§ In bold: DreamSAT output categories; * Statistically Significant; ** Highly statistically significant.
Table 3. Male T0 vs. T1 and Female T0 vs. T1.
Table 3. Male T0 vs. T1 and Female T0 vs. T1.
Males T0Males T1p: Males T0 vs.
Males T1
Females T0Females T1p: Females T0 vs. Females T1
§Characters
Male/Female75%78%0.78413%50%0.166
Familiarity53%55%0.85550%64%0.481
Friends24%28%0.7476%55%** 0.003
Family22%23%0.99544%0%** 0.000
Dead and Imaginary2%2%0.8926%9%0.721
Animal6%5%0.8076%0%0.214
§Social Interaction Percents
Aggression/Friendliness57%55%0.89471%0%0.060
Befriender20%80%* 0.04250%100%0.200
Aggressor64%50%0.55240%0%0.211
Physical Aggression23%8%0.26233%0%0.254
§Settings
Indoor Setting38%39%0.93350%71%0.426
Familiar Setting73%83%0.528100%67%* 0.042
§Self-Concept Percents
Self-Negativity59%66%0.56560%50%0.734
Bodily Misfortunes48%20%0.078100%0%** 0.000
Negative Emotions36%78%0.05450%25%0.459
Dreamer-Involved Success50%63%0.640100%25%0.087
Torso/Anatomy20%0%0.0600%0%1.000
§Dreams with at Least One:
Aggression20%24%0.69322%0%* 0.013
Friendliness20%27%0.48611%10%0.927
Sexuality0%0%1.0000%0%1.000
Misfortune30%36%0.52822%20%0.890
Good Fortune20%18%0.8036%10%0.671
Success7%15%0.23911%0%0.085
Failure11%9%0.7440%0%1.000
Striving18%18%1.00011%0%0.085
§ In bold: DreamSAT output categories; * Statistically Significant; ** Highly statistically significant.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Giordano, A.; Boffano, M.; Piana, R.; Mutani, R.; Cicolin, A. Body Schema Self-Awareness and Related Dream Content Modifications in Amputees Due to Cancer. Brain Sci. 2021, 11, 1625. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci11121625

AMA Style

Giordano A, Boffano M, Piana R, Mutani R, Cicolin A. Body Schema Self-Awareness and Related Dream Content Modifications in Amputees Due to Cancer. Brain Sciences. 2021; 11(12):1625. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci11121625

Chicago/Turabian Style

Giordano, Alessandra, Michele Boffano, Raimondo Piana, Roberto Mutani, and Alessandro Cicolin. 2021. "Body Schema Self-Awareness and Related Dream Content Modifications in Amputees Due to Cancer" Brain Sciences 11, no. 12: 1625. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci11121625

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop