Next Article in Journal
Power Harvested Maximization for Solar Photovoltaic Energy System Under Static and Dynamic Conditions
Previous Article in Journal
Study of Various Types of Glazing in a Building Constructed Using Hybrid Technology with a Large Window Area
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

The Mechanically Destabilizing Effect of Increased Heel Height in Women Is Not Enhanced by Dual-Task Interference

by
Maria-Elissavet Nikolaidou
1,*,
Aikaterini Dervenioti
2,
Arno Schroll
3,4 and
Adamantios Arampatzis
3,4
1
Division of Sport Medicine and Biology of Exercise, Faculty of Physical Education and Sport Science, School of Physical Education and Sport Science, National and Kapodistrian University of Athens, 17237 Athens, Greece
2
School of Physical Education and Sport Science, National and Kapodistrian University of Athens, 17237 Athens, Greece
3
Department of Training and Movement Sciences, Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin, 10115 Berlin, Germany
4
Berlin School of Movement Science, 10115 Berlin, Germany
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Appl. Sci. 2025, 15(8), 4485; https://doi.org/10.3390/app15084485
Submission received: 24 February 2025 / Revised: 15 April 2025 / Accepted: 17 April 2025 / Published: 18 April 2025

Abstract

:
Postural balance deteriorates as heel height increases, but the dual-task interference effect has not been investigated. This observational cross-sectional study investigated the influence of two concurrent cognitive tasks on balance performance while wearing shoes with different heel heights. We hypothesized that the concurrent cognitive demands’ effect on balance deterioration would be more pronounced in shoes with increased heel height. Twenty-one women (21–54 years) were assessed for quiet stance balance performance while wearing shoes with low-, medium-and high-heel height. Cognitive interference on balance performance was investigated by a dual-task condition using a short memory (COGN_A) and verbal fluency (COGN_B) task. Balance performance was quantified by measuring center of pressure (CoP) path length and sway range, while errors made and scores in COGN_A and COGN_B assessed cognitive performance. Postural balance decreased as heel height increased with greater CoP path length and anteroposterior sway for the medium- and high-heeled shoes (p < 0.001). Cognitive load did not affect balance performance (p = 0.234–0.494), nor was there an interaction between cognitive load and balance with increasing heel height (p = 0.281–0.375). The results suggest that cognitive load does not limit postural balance with increasing heel height, and that balance is more affected by the increased mechanical demands of heel height.

1. Introduction

Posture control is required to ensure the body’s balance and safety in challenging conditions as is the case while wearing high heels. Specifically, increased heel height has been shown to negatively affect postural stability, mainly by inducing lumbar flattening and posterior pelvic tilt [1,2]. In addition, a decrease in pelvic, knee and ankle range of motion [3,4,5] is associated with manifestations of significantly increased magnitude and velocity of postural sway in both the anteroposterior and mediolateral directions [6,7,8,9,10]. Further, the potential threat to the body’s postural balance while standing in high-heeled shoes is accompanied by increased lower limb stiffness [11,12] as evidenced by a significant rise in the electromyographic activity of the triceps surae muscles along with increased co-contraction of the tibialis anterior and peroneus longus muscles [13,14].
On the other hand, postural control would provide a flexible and mechanically stable substrate to enable and facilitate the execution of concurrent cognitive tasks [15,16,17]. Within this context, the underlying theoretical assumption is that available resources are limited and they have to be shared between the motor and cognitive tasks or performance will be compromised [18,19]. Balance control engages cognitive resources [20,21] that are required for the continuous utilization and reweighing by the brain of the integrated sensorimotor information provided by the proprioceptive, vestibular and visual channels in order to determine the magnitude of postural sway to efficiently preserve balance and safety [22,23,24]. In healthy individuals, the cognitive resources devoted to postural balance control are largely automatic [25,26] while requiring varying degrees of attention depending on the difficulty of the task [27,28]. The continuous interaction between postural and perception–action tasks, defined as dual-tasking posture–cognition interference, is quite complex, with previous findings of dual-task postural studies being inconsistent [29,30]. Specifically, there have been reported impairments in postural performance resulting from the diversion of the available attentional resources to the secondary cognitive task [31,32,33,34], whereas decrements in cognitive task performance have been found due to the prioritization of balance safety, referred to as the “posture-first strategy” [35,36,37]. The adoption of the “posture-first strategy” depends on several factors such as the nature of the cognitive tasks, the instruction set, the choice of baseline conditions and, most importantly, the perceived level of postural threat [15,38,39]. For example, a “posture-first strategy” has been found during demanding locomotor activities at the expense of cognitive performance [35,36,37], but not during less demanding conditions such as standing [40,41,42]. Therefore, the issue of dual-task cognitive–postural interference still remains perplexing. To the best of our knowledge, there is no study where the effect of dual tasking has been considered in a challenging postural configuration like standing in high heels.
The goal of this study was to investigate the influence of concurrent demanding cognitive tasks on postural balance performance while wearing shoes with different heel heights in women. We hypothesized that heel height would impair postural balance performance and that the interferences of the concurrent cognitive demands on balance performance would be more pronounced in shoes with increased heel height. We used two challenging cognitive tasks on the recruited participants, as they require different types of brain processing and cognitive resources to perform. The first task required the repetition of a series of digits and is considered a mental tracking and working memory task [43,44], where the individual has to remember the exact order of appearance of a 10-digit string. The second cognitive task required the generation of a 4-word sentence under pre-specified search conditions and is classified as a verbal fluency task involving executive function and semantic memory processes [43].

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants

This was an observational cross-sectional study conducted in a laboratory setting. Participants were contacted by the use of printed announcements posted in the School’s main facilities and through personal communication by the researchers (M.-E.N. and A.D.). Twenty-two subjects responded positively to the study’s call, one subject was excluded due to not meeting the inclusion criteria and twenty-one subjects were deemed eligible and were included for analysis. Thus, a total of 21 healthy, physically active women aged 21 to 54 years old volunteered to participate. Inclusion criteria to the study were defined as follows: (a) participants had to be non-wearers of high-heels, as determined by the frequency of wearing high-heeled shoes <2 days per week during the previous year [9], and (b) participants had to have not been systematically engaged in ballroom dancing activities (e.g., latin, flamengo) for the past 5 years, in order to eliminate the previously observed effect of habitual high-heel experience on postural balance [8,13]. Participants were asked about their medical history and were excluded if they reported a history of neuromuscular diseases, musculoskeletal disorders, cardiovascular or severe systemic diseases, severe arthritis, or if they had been taking any medication for the above diseases, or any other medication with side-effects for postural balance in the last 6 months. Further, they were asked to complete the short self-answered International Physical Activity Questionnaire (IPAQ) [45] in order to assess their physical activity level. The study’s experimental procedures were approved by the Ethics Committee of the School of Physical Education and Sport Science of the National and Kapodistrian University of Athens (approval number: 1542/09-10-2023), and all participants gave their written informed consent in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki.

2.2. Experimental Protocol

Participants’ postural balance was assessed in a quiet stance (a) without any further cognitive load (control condition) (Figure 1, left) and (b) separately with two different concurrent cognitive tasks (dual-task condition) (Figure 1, right). To complete those assessments, participants were required to participate in a 90 min single session, which took place in a quiet indoor space with appropriate light and temperature conditions in the university’s facilities. A wash-out period of 45 min between the control and dual-task conditions was used to limit any possible learning effect. Participants were instructed to abstain from any strenuous cognitive or physical activity 24 h prior to the experimental session. All procedures were safe and conducted under supervision.

2.2.1. Postural Balance Assessment

Assessment of postural balance in the control condition included open-eye quiet stance trials while wearing shoes of different heel heights (Figure 1, upper left), namely (i) a low heel (1–2 cm), (ii) a medium heel (7 cm), and (iii) a high heel (10.1 cm) (Figure 2). In the dual-task conditions, postural balance assessment was carried out with the same quiet stance trials with the medium- and high-heel-height shoes (Figure 1, right). Participants stood on a force plate (Wii, A/D converter, 24-bit resolution, 1000 Hz, Biovision, Wehrheim, Germany) while maintaining a straight body posture with their arms hanging relaxed by their sides and their feet hip-width apart. They had their gaze fixed on an imaginary point on the wall 2–3 m in front of them, while their heads were kept parallel to ground level. The order of shoe wearing was randomized in both the control and the dual-task condition. In each shoe wearing condition, three trials lasting 30 s were recorded with a 1 min rest between shoes. For hygiene reasons, participants wore light stockings that were disposed afterwards. For the low-heel height, 3–4 different pairs of moccasin-style leather shoes with rubber soles and wooden heels were used (Figure 2A), whereas the medium- and high-heeled shoes were purchased by the same manufacturer in order to ensure the same type of shoe style (i.e., dress shoes with pump heels) and sole materials (Figure 2B,C). Heel base dimensions were 1 cm in width and 1 cm in length for both the medium- and high-heel heights, respectively. Prior to starting the experimental protocol, a brief shoe-fitting session was performed to ensure that all shoes were in appropriate ranges of size and fitted the participants comfortably.
Valid trials were considered those in which participants had been effective in maintaining their balance throughout the trial’s duration (i.e., 30 s). During the analysis, the recorded center of pressure (CoP) data from the force plate (Wii, A/D converter, 24-bit resolution, 1000 Hz, Biovision, Wehrheim, Germany) were filtered using a 2nd bi-directional-order digital low-pass Butterworth filter with a 15 Hz cut-off frequency and analyzed with MATLAB custom-made scripts (R2012a, 64 Bit; Mathworks, Natick, MA, USA) from the 2nd to the 27th second (Δt = 25 s) of each 30 s trial time. The assessment of postural balance performance is typically based on the CoP displacement, whose derived values represent the geometrical location of the reaction force vector on the platform during quiet standing [46]. Postural balance performance was determined by the following parameters: (a) CoP path length, defined as the sum of Euclidean distances between adjacent measurement points, and (b) CoP sway range, defined as the range (i.e., from minimum to maximum) of the CoP values in the anteroposterior and mediolateral directions. To assess performance in the two-legged quiet stance trials, the average values of the three trials were used.

2.2.2. Concurrent Cognitive Tasks

Two cognitive tasks were used in the dual-task condition, hereafter defined as Cognitive Task-A (COGN_A) and Cognitive Task-B (COGN_B), to assess interference effects on postural balance performance (Figure 3). COGN_A referred to the assessment of short-term memory capacity elicited by a visual stimulus and processed by a phonological response [44]. A series of strings based on 10-digit task stimuli using Microsoft PowerPoint© were presented on a 15.6 inch FHD (1920 × 1080 pixels) HP Pavillion laptop, positioned at eye level in front of participants and at a 2 m distance. By using the “rand” function in Microsoft Excel©, random-number strings were generated, spanning from 0 to 9 and allowing repeated digits within a string. Due to participants’ age and educational level (i.e., all participants had a university degree) and after some pilot trials, we decided to use a 10-digit span for all participants, set as the maximum number of digits in a string that a participant could correctly recall. After a familiarization trial, during which the participant was given instructions about the test procedure and a digit string to try, each participant, while quietly standing in front of the HP Pavillion laptop, was presented with a new series of 10 slides at an automated velocity of slide transition lasting 1 s. As already described, each slide displayed a different digit using a 239-point Calibri font. During the slide transition (Figure 3, left), participants were instructed to mentally rehearse the 10-digit string that was eventually formed until the last slide disappeared and a black screen appeared. They were then asked to verbally recall the exact order of appearance of the digit string. The cognitive task performance was assessed based on the recorded number of errors, for example, a score of 6 meant that the participant had correctly recalled the order of appearance of the first 4 digits in that trial [44].
The second cognitive task (COGN_B) was the sentence completion task, which is a visual and verbal formation task of a 4-word sentence [35]. This language-processing task is a valid task for the assessment of the dual-task interference effect because it is considered that one’s attentional and/or focusing resources would be more easily interfered by a motor task due to the visual processing and verbal coding required in the sentence completion task as compared to an attentional task of auditory modality [47]. Four-word sentences with four blanks were generated, two of which had a letter preceding the blank and two others had an asterisk. When a letter preceded the blank, the word must start with that letter, whereas any word could be used in the blanks with asterisks (Figure 3, right). The order of letters and asterisks was random in the sentence and no names or locations were allowed to be used. After being provided with the necessary explanations and instructions as well as with 1–2 familiarization trials, participants were presented with a series of slides displaying 4-word sentences for a duration of 30 s, during which they were instructed to verbally complete as many sentences as possible. COGN_B’s conditions were similar as in COGN_A (i.e., participant’s position with regard to laptop, Microsoft PowerPoint© specifications (e.g., type and font size)) except for the fact that one researcher (A.D.) manually changed the slides at the instant the participant had completed forming the 4-word sentence displayed. Cognitive performance on each trial of the sentence completion task was based on the criteria of correctly using cued starting letters and creating a grammatically correct sentence. Therefore, for any sentence that was completed, the range of assigned points was between 0 and 6, with 1 point being assigned for each word meeting the criteria and 2 points being assigned for the sentence being grammatically correct [35]. For example, the sentence “teacher asked students exam” would receive 4 points due to grammar errors of 2 points (Figure 3, right). All sentences from every participant were recorded by a mobile phone (iPhone11, Apple, Cupertino, CA, USA) to be assessed afterwards, and performance was determined by the total score of the completed sentences divided by the sum of those sentences. For example, in the case of a trial during which 10 sentences of 6 points each were completed, the final score was 10 × 6 = 60/10 = 6. As a reference, the cognitive performance on both dual tasks was measured as the average of two trials while seated.

2.3. Statistical Analysis

For the statistical analysis, we first checked for the normal distribution of the CoP outcome parameters (CoP path length, range of anteroposterior and mediolateral CoP sway) using the Kolmogorov–Smirnov test with Lilliefors correction. Testing of normality failed for the mediolateral CoP sway range in the control (p = 0.017) and dual-task_A (p < 0.001) and _B condition (p = 0.024) for the medium-heel height shoe. Similarly, the normality check failed for that same parameter in the dual-task_A (p = 0.019) and dual-task_B condition (p < 0.001) for the high-heeled shoe. However, following visual inspection with quantile–quantile (Q-Q) plots, the mediolateral CoP data in the above situations were normal with slight deviations. One-way ANOVA was used to test the effect of heel height (low, medium, high) on postural balance performance. In the event of a significant heel height effect, post hoc comparisons were performed. A 3 × 2 ANOVA with cognitive load (control, dual-task_A, dual-task_B) as a within-subjects factor and heel height (medium, high) as a between-subjects factor was performed on the two-legged CoP parameters. To control for the violation of the sphericity assumption, the Geisser–Greenhouse-adjusted F-test was used [48]. In case a significant main or interaction effect (cognitive load by heel height) was found, a Bonferroni-corrected multiple comparisons analysis was conducted. Further, a one-way ANOVA was used to check for possible interferences of heel height (medium, high) on cognitive performance between the reference and the dual tasks. To estimate the strength of the potential main or interaction effects on the investigated outcome parameters, Cohen’s d with values of d < 0.2, 0.2 ≤ d < 0.8 and d ≥ 0.8 indicating a small, medium or large effect size was calculated [49]. All statistical analyses were performed using SPSS IBM v.21 and the significance level was set at a = 0.05. For the graphical representation of the outcomes, we used boxplots depicting the median and the 5th to 95th percentile as whiskers.

3. Results

The anthropometric characteristics of the participants are presented in Table 1. Based on the results from the IPAQ, participants were overall classified as having a medium physical activity (PA) level amounting to a weekly average of 2680 ± 1936 metabolic equivalents (METs) per minute. The detailed distribution of the participants’ age and PA levels can be found in Supplementary Table S1. Briefly, with the exception of one participant being assessed with a low PA level, the majority of them (n = 14) were assessed with a medium PA level, while six participants were assessed with a high PA level, respectively (Supplementary Table S1).
In the control condition, a significant main effect of heel height was found for the CoP parameters (Figure 4). Specifically, path length was significantly increased (F2,60 = 32.7, p < 0.001, d = 1.019) following the increase in heel height and post hoc comparisons showed significantly higher values between low- and medium-heel, low- and high-heel and medium- and high-heel height, respectively (p < 0.001 for all comparisons, Figure 4A). Similarly, the increase in heel height significantly affected the anteroposterior CoP sway range (F2,60 = 19.1, p < 0.001, d = 0.779) with the respective CoP values being significantly higher between the low- and medium-heel (p = 0.001) as well as the low- and high-heel heights (p < 0.001) (Figure 4B). Post hoc comparisons did not yield a statistically significant (p = 0.068) difference between the medium- and high-heel heights for the AP sway range. In the case of the CoP mediolateral sway range, there was not a significant effect of heel height (F2,60 = 3.0, p > 0.05, d = 0.311) (Figure 4C).
The two-way ANOVA revealed no statistically significant main effect of cognitive load (F2,75 = 1.642, p = 0.202, d = 0.197) and a non-significant interaction effect of cognitive load by heel height (F2,80 = 0.994, p = 0.375, d = 0.153, Figure 5A). There was a significant main effect of heel height (F1,40 = 12.2, p = 0.001, d = 0.539) and post hoc comparisons showed that path length values were significantly greater with the high compared to the medium heels in all conditions (control: p = 0.002, dual-task_A: p = 0.022, dual-task_B: p = 0.032) (Figure 5A). In the anteroposterior range of CoP sway, there was no significant main effect of cognitive load (F2,78 = 0.056, p = 0.943, d = 0.031) or heel height (F1,40 = 0.218, p = 0.643, d = 0.069) and no significant interaction of cognitive load by heel height (F2,80 = 1.288, p = 0.281, d = 0.175, Figure 5B). In the mediolateral direction, a significant main effect of cognitive load was found (F2,77 = 4.464, p = 0.016, d = 0.325), but post hoc comparisons showed a significantly greater range of CoP sway (p < 0.040) only for the medium-heeled shoe in the dual-task_B condition compared to the control condition (Figure 5C). No statistically significant effect of heel height (F1,40 = 0.082, p = 0.776, d = 0.044) and cognitive load by heel height interaction (F2,80 = 1.237, p = 0.296, d = 0.172) was found in the mediolateral sway range (Figure 5C).
There was no difference in cognitive performance between the medium-heeled and high-heeled conditions compared to the reference sitting condition for both dual tasks (COGN_A: F2,35 = 0.676, p = 0.494, d = 0.128; COGN_B: F2,38 = 1.509, p = 0.234, d = 0.226, Figure 6).

4. Discussion

The present study investigated the effect of shoe heel height and concurrent cognitive demands on postural balance performance in women. First, it was hypothesized that postural balance performance would be impaired due to heel height. The results on the CoP sway magnitude showed that balance performance decreased as heel height increased, thus confirming our first hypothesis. It was further hypothesized that the interference of concurrent cognitive demands on postural balance performance would be more pronounced during quiet standing in shoes with an increased heel height. The results did not show a significant systematic effect of cognitive load on balance performance, nor an interaction between cognitive load and balance performance at different heel heights, rejecting our second hypothesis.
It has been shown that an increase in heel height increases postural instability and induces a number of kinematic changes that place significant neuromuscular demands on the system. The main kinematic changes in the lower limbs when standing in high heels (i.e., typically over 4 cm) include lumbar flattening, posterior pelvic tilt and a significant decrease in the range of motion of the pelvis, knee and ankle joints [1,2,3]. Heel elevation induces an increase in electromyographic activity of the lower limb muscles, particularly the calf muscles, which may increase lower limb stiffness to cope with the possible threat to postural stability [11,12], indicating a greater neuromuscular effort to balance control [13]. Furthermore, the magnitude and velocity of CoP oscillation either in anteroposterior or mediolateral direction increased with increasing heel height, indicating a significant compromise in postural balance [6,8,9]. We found a significantly greater CoP path length, up to 1.05 times greater in the medium- and high-heeled shoes compared to the low-heeled shoes. The increased CoP path length was associated with greater anteroposterior sway magnitude, up to 75%, while mediolateral CoP sway was unaffected by heel height. In agreement with our results, Gerber et al. and Mika et al. [6,7] also reported that the increase in heel height mainly affected the CoP range in the anteroposterior direction, with no changes in the mediolateral direction. Taking these results together, we can argue that the greater neuromuscular demand of balance control with increased heel height is regulated with anteroposterior adjustments of the CoP. From a biomechanical point of view, the increased heel in shoes mainly reduces the mediolateral base of support and thus limits the possibility of mediolateral adjustments.
Everyday life is abundant with situations where cognitive and motor tasks occur simultaneously, and because cognitive resources are required for postural and motor control [15,50], there is an interaction between cognitive and motor tasks. This interaction between the two tasks referred to as dual-tasking interference has been shown to result in the diversion of attentional resources to the secondary cognitive tasks [15,33], thus rendering locomotor performance vulnerable. It has been reported that balance safety is prioritized at the expense of cognitive task performance [35,36], referred to as the “posture-first strategy” [35]. Both cognitive tasks were considered as perceptually challenging. The participants performed the first task by recalling the exact order of the 10-digit string with an average of 5 ± 2 errors, while in the sentence completion task, their performance was ~92% successful (mean score 5.5 out of the maximum score of 6 per sentence). These scores show that both cognitive tasks demanded close to maximum performance from the participants, and we can argue that important cognitive resources were used. In cases where a concurrent cognitive task demands cognitive resources to perform the balance task, people prioritize postural safety [35,36,37], so in our experiment, a decrease in cognitive performance due to increased heel height would indicate a prioritization of postural balance. However, we did not find any differences in cognitive performance with increasing heel height in either of the cognitive tasks used, indicating that the cognitive load did not limit the cognitive resources needed for postural balance during the quiet stance.
Contrary to our hypothesis, the results did not show any systematic effect of dual-tasking interference on postural performance, thereby suggesting that the cognitive resources required for the execution of the cognitive tasks did not compete with the attentional resources involved with preserving balance while wearing shoes of elevated heel height. Previous studies investigating the dual-task posture–cognition interference have provided inconsistent results with the postural task challenge emerging as a critical factor for the reported discordance [25,29]. In agreement with our findings, Bustillo-Casero et al. [51], Linder et al. [40] and Siu and Woollacott [41] did not find any change in postural sway during the barefoot quiet stance in young adult participants while simultaneously executing complex cognitive tasks. Similarly, and closer to the age range of our middle-aged subjects, Bourlon et al. [42] reported no dual-task interference on postural sway during the concurrent execution of cognitive reaction time tasks. Standing is a very common activity in daily life. It has been reported that approximately 41% of the time spent in activities or postures in daily life is spent standing [52]. Experience-based anticipatory tonic muscle activation contributes to postural balance [53] and may minimize the involvement of higher control centers. Nevertheless, we found a significant deterioration in postural balance with increasing heel height. The deterioration of postural balance with increasing heel height during standing, but without interference of cognitive load, may have important implications for daily life. Despite the availability of cognitive resources, the increase in balance challenge during standing due to increased heel height cannot be fully compensated for, and balance safety is more affected by mechanical demands. Considering these results together, the non-found effect of dual-tasking interference on postural performance suggests that our participants adopted the “posture-first” strategy to respond to their perceived level of postural threat and ensure balance safety is not compromised [35,36,39]. Prior experience in a specific task is known to improve balance through anticipatory adjustments [54]. In our study, we investigated balance responses in humans who were not habitual wearers of high-heeled shoes and who may have some deficits in anticipatory balance control compared to habitual wearers. Theoretically, the effect of cognitive load would be lower in habitual wearers of high-heeled shoes due to their experience. Therefore, our main finding that cognitive load does not additionally worsen balance performance with increasing heel height will be valid.
A limitation of the current study is that the interaction between cognition and postural balance control was examined here in quiet standing, whereas a complementary inclusion of dynamic conditions, such as walking with high-heeled shoes, might have provided additional insight into the allocation of resources during dual tasking. Given that approximately 41% of all daily activities involve standing and approximately 11% involve walking [52], we are confident that our results in standing have important ecological validity. Although the age range of our participants was quite wide (i.e., 21–54 years) and the results of the study can be generalized to a wide age range, we did not study older people and our results may not be representative of this age group. Moreover, the majority of included participants were assessed with a medium physical activity level which might have facilitated them in reaching the close to maximum performance in the cognitive tasks as compared to the same situation executed by healthy albeit not so physically active participants. Although we found a decrease in balance with increasing shoe heel height in our healthy participants, impaired participants may show interference in balance performance due to concurrent cognitive load during stance [55]. Moreover, a major issue in the context of the posture–cognition dual-tasking methodology relates to the perceptual demands of the cognitive tasks [31,56]. In this study, the cognitive load was not individualized; therefore, the possibility that the two cognitive tasks used here might have not been demanding enough to require additional processing resources that could interfere with postural balance control cannot be ruled out. Likewise, another potential limitation might stem from the type of the cognitive tasks with regard to recruiting internal or external focus of attention. We used two continuous cognitive tasks that required participants to respond to information displayed on a laptop screen in front of them rather than have them mentally perform a cognitive task (e.g., mentally counting the occurrence of a preselected digit in an auditory sequence of three-digit numbers) [57]. It has been suggested that the use of a continuous cognitive task along with an external attentional focus permits attention to be steered away from the postural balance task, thus allowing automatic processes to operate for postural control [58].

5. Conclusions

In conclusion, the increased demand for balance control with increased heel height is mainly regulated by anteroposterior adjustments of the CoP, most likely because the increased heel in shoes reduces the mediolateral base of support, limiting mediolateral adjustments. Despite the cognitive demands of the cognitive tasks used, both cognitive and balance performance did not deteriorate in the dual-task conditions, suggesting that cognitive load did not limit postural balance during quiet stance. The same deterioration in balance performance found with increasing shoe heel height in the single and dual-task conditions suggests that balance in quiet stance is more affected by the increased mechanical demand. Finally, our results indicate that neuromuscular adaptations to mechanically induced instability are more important, as cognitive load does not cause additional balance deterioration beyond that caused by shoe heel height in physically active women.

Supplementary Materials

The following supporting information can be downloaded at: https://www.mdpi.com/article/10.3390/app15084485/s1, Table S1: Age and physical activity levels distribution of participants based on their scores in the short self-answered International Physical Activity Questionnaire.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, M.-E.N. and A.A.; methodology, M.-E.N. and A.D.; software, A.S.; validation, M.-E.N. and A.S.; formal analysis, A.D. and M.-E.N.; investigation, M.-E.N. and A.D.; resources, A.S. and A.A.; data curation, M.-E.N. and A.D.; writing—original draft preparation, M.-E.N.; writing—review and editing, M.-E.N. and A.A.; visualization, M.-E.N. and A.A.; supervision, M.-E.N.; project administration, M.-E.N. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

This study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki, and approved by the Ethics Committee of the Faculty of Physical Education and Sport Science, School of Physical Education and Sport Science, National and Kapodistrian University of Athens (approval number: 1542, date of approval: 9 October 2023).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors on request.

Acknowledgments

The authors are grateful to all the women who participated in the study.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

Abbreviations

The following abbreviations are used in this manuscript:
COGN_AFirst cognitive short-term memory task
COGN_BSecond cognitive verbal fluency task
CoPCenter of pressure

References

  1. Opila, K.A.; Wagner, S.S.; Schiowitz, S.; Chen, J. Postural alignment in barefoot and high-heeled stance. Spine 1988, 13, 542–547. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Snow, R.E.; Williams, K.R. High heeled shoes: Their effect on center of mass position, posture, three-dimensional kinematics, rearfoot motion, and ground reaction forces. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 1994, 75, 568–576. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Chen, Y.; Li, J.X.; Wang, L. Influences of heel height on human postural stability and functional mobility between inexperienced and experienced high heel shoe wearers. PeerJ 2020, 8, e10239. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Weitkunat, T.; Buck, F.M.; Jentzsch, T.; Simmen, H.P.; Werner, C.M.; Osterhoff, G. Influence of high-heeled shoes on the sagittal balance of the spine and the whole body. Eur. Spine J. 2016, 25, 3658–3665. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Xu, D.; Zhou, H.; Quan, W.; Ma, X.; Chon, T.E.; Fernandez, J.; Gusztav, F.; Kovács, A.; Baker, J.S.; Gu, Y. New insights optimize landing strategies to reduce lower limb injury risk. Cyborg. Bionic. Syst. 2024, 5, 0126. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Gerber, S.B.; Costa, R.V.; Grecco, L.A.; Pasini, H.; Marconi, N.F.; Oliveira, C.S. Interference of high-heeled shoes in static balance among young women. Hum. Mov. Sci. 2012, 31, 1247–1252. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Mika, A.; Oleksy, Ł.; Kielnar, R.; Świerczek, M. The influence of high- and low-heeled shoes on balance in young women. Acta Bioeng. Biomech. 2016, 18, 97–103. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  8. Wan, F.K.W.; Yick, K.L.; Yu, W.W.M. Effects of heel height and high-heel experience on foot stability during quiet standing. Gait Posture 2019, 68, 252–257. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  9. Yamada-Yanagawa, A.; Sasagawa, S.; Nakazawa, K.; Ishii, N. Effects of Occasional and Habitual Wearing of High-Heeled Shoes on Static Balance in Young Women. Front. Sports Act. Living 2022, 4, 760991. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  10. Truszczyńska, A.; Trzaskoma, Z.; Stypinska, Z.; Drzal-Grabiec, J.; Tarnowski, A. Is static balance affected by using shoes of different height? Biomed. Hum. Kinet. 2016, 8, 137–144. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Albertsen, I.M.; Ghédira, M.; Gracies, J.M.; Hutin, É. Postural stability in young healthy subjects—Impact of reduced base of support, visual deprivation, dual tasking. J. Electromyogr. Kinesiol. 2017, 33, 27–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Dault, M.C.; Geurts, A.C.; Mulder, T.W.; Duysens, J. Postural control and cognitive task performance in healthy participants while balancing on different support-surface configurations. Gait Posture 2001, 14, 248–255. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Hapsari, V.D.; Xiong, S. Effects of high heeled shoes wearing experience and heel height on human standing balance and functional mobility. Ergonomics 2016, 59, 249–264. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Gefen, A.; Megido-Ravid, M.; Itzchak, Y.; Arcan, M. Analysis of muscular fatigue and foot stability during high-heeled gait. Gait Posture 2002, 15, 56–63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Fraizer, E.V.; Mitra, S. Methodological and interpretive issues in posture-cognition dual-tasking in upright stance. Gait Posture 2008, 27, 271–279. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Jacobs, J.V.; Horak, F.B. Cortical control of postural responses. J. Neural Transm. 2007, 114, 1339–1348. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  17. Riccio, G.E.; Stoffregen, T.A. Affordances as constraints on the control of stance. Hum. Mov. Sci. 1988, 7, 265–300. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Wickens, C.D. Processing resources and attention. In Multiple-Task Performance, 1st ed.; Damos, D., Ed.; Taylor-Francis: London, UK, 1991; pp. 3–34. [Google Scholar]
  19. Schaefer, S. The ecological approach to cognitive-motor dual-tasking: Findings on the effects of expertise and age. Front. Psychol. 2014, 5, 1167. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  20. Ouchi, Y.; Okada, H.; Yoshikawa, E.; Nobezawa, S.; Futatsubashi, M. Brain activation during maintenance of standing postures in humans. Brain 1999, 122, 329–338. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  21. Ouchi, Y.; Okada, H.; Yoshikawa, E.; Futatsubashi, M.; Nobezawa, S. Absolute changes in regional cerebral blood flow in association with upright posture in humans: An orthostatic PET study. J. Nucl. Med. 2001, 42, 707–712. [Google Scholar]
  22. Horak, F.B. Postural orientation and equilibrium: What do we need to know about neural control of balance to prevent falls? Age Ageing 2001, 35, ii7–ii11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Peterka, R.J.; Loughlin, P.J. Dynamic regulation of sensorimotor integration in human postural control. J. Neurophysiol. 2004, 91, 410–423. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. Roberts, T.D.M. Understanding Balance: The Mechanics of Posture and Locomotion; Chapman & Hall: London, UK, 1995. [Google Scholar]
  25. Lacour, M.; Bernard-Demanze, L.; Dumitrescu, M. Posture control, aging, and attention resources: Models and posture-analysis methods. Neurophysiol. Clin. 2008, 38, 411–421. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Vuillerme, N.; Nafati, G. How attentional focus on body sway affects postural control during quiet standing. Psychol. Res. 2007, 71, 192–200. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  27. Redfern, M.S.; Jennings, J.R.; Martin, C.; Furman, J.M. Attention influences sensory integration for postural control in older adults. Gait Posture 2001, 14, 211–216. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Shumway-Cook, A.; Woollacott, M. Attentional demands and postural control: The effect of sensory context. J. Gerontol. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 2000, 55, M10–M16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  29. Salihu, A.T.; Hill, K.D.; Jaberzadeh, S. Effect of cognitive task complexity on dual task postural stability: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Exp. Brain Res. 2022, 240, 703–731. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Ghai, S.; Ghai, I.; Effenberg, A.O. Effects of dual tasks and dual-task training on postural stability: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Clin. Interv. Aging 2017, 12, 557–577. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Barra, J.; Bray, A.; Sahni, V.; Golding, J.F.; Gresty, M.A. Increasing cognitive load with increasing balance challenge: Recipe for catastrophe. Exp. Brain Res. 2006, 174, 734–745. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Dault, M.C.; Yardley, L.; Frank, J.S. Does articulation contribute to modifications of postural control during dual-task paradigms? Brain Res. Cogn. Brain Res. 2003, 16, 434–440. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Kerr, B.; Condon, S.M.; McDonald, L.A. Cognitive spatial processing and the regulation of posture. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 1985, 11, 617–622. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  34. Mujdeci, B.; Turkyilmaz, D.; Yagcioglu, S.; Aksoy, S. The effects of concurrent cognitive tasks on postural sway in healthy subjects. Braz. J. Otorhinolaryngol. 2016, 82, 3–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Shumway-Cook, A.; Woollacott, M.; Kerns, K.A.; Baldwin, M. The effects of two types of cognitive tasks on postural stability in older adults with and without a history of falls. J. Gerontol. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 1997, 52A, M232–M240. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Mersmann, F.; Bohm, S.; Bierbaum, S.; Dietrich, R.; Arampatzis, A. Young and old adults prioritize dynamic stability control following gait perturbations when performing a concurrent cognitive task. Gait Posture 2013, 37, 373–377. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Small, G.H.; Neptune, R.R. Task-prioritization and balance recovery strategies used by young healthy adults during dual-task walking. Gait Posture 2022, 95, 115–120. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  38. Ruffieux, J.; Keller, M.; Lauber, B.; Taube, W. Changes in Standing and Walking Performance Under Dual-Task Conditions Across the Lifespan. Sports Med. 2015, 45, 1739–1758. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  39. Yogev-Seligmann, G.; Hausdorff, J.M.; Giladi, N. Do we always prioritize balance when walking? Towards an integrated model of task prioritization. Mov. Disord. 2012, 27, 765–770. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Linder, S.M.; Koop, M.M.; Ozinga, S.; Goldfarb, Z.; Alberts, J.L. A Mobile Device Dual-Task Paradigm for the Assessment of mTBI. Mil. Med. 2019, 184, 174–180. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  41. Siu, K.C.; Woollacott, M.H. Attentional demands of postural control: The ability to selectively allocate information-processing resources. Gait Posture 2007, 25, 121–126. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Bourlon, C.; Lehenaff, L.; Batifoulier, C.; Bordier, A.; Chatenet, A.; Desailly, E.; Fouchard, C.; Marsal, M.; Martinez, M.; Rastelli, F.; et al. Dual-tasking postural control in patients with right brain damage. Gait Posture 2014, 39, 188–193. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Al-Yahya, E.; Dawes, H.; Smith, L.; Dennis, A.; Howells, K.; Cockburn, J. Cognitive motor interference while walking: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2011, 35, 715–728. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Riley, M.A.; Baker, A.A.; Schmit, J.M.; Weaver, E. Effects of visual and auditory short-term memory tasks on the spatiotemporal dynamics and variability of postural sway. J. Mot. Behav. 2005, 37, 311–324. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  45. Papathanasiou, G.; Georgoudis, G.; Papandreou, M.; Spyropoulos, P.; Georgakopoulos, D.; Kalfakakou, V.; Evangelou, A. Reliability measures of the short International Physical Activity Questionnaire (IPAQ) in Greek young adults. Hell. J. Cardiol. 2009, 50, 283–294. [Google Scholar]
  46. Prieto, T.E.; Myklebust, J.B.; Hoffmann, R.G.; Lovett, E.G.; Myklebust, B.M. Measures of postural steadiness: Differences between healthy young and elderly adults. IEEE Trans. Biomed. Eng. 1996, 43, 956–966. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  47. Wickens, C.D. Multiple resources and performance prediction. Theor. Issues Ergonom. Sci. 2002, 3, 159–177. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Blanca, M.J.; Arnau, J.; García-Castro, F.J.; Alarcón, R.; Bono, R. Repeated measures ANOVA and adjusted F-tests when sphericity is violated: Which procedure is best? Front. Psychol. 2023, 14, 1192453. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  49. Cohen, J. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences, 2nd ed.; Routledge: Oxfordshire, UK, 1988. [Google Scholar]
  50. Boisgontier, M.P.; Beets, I.A.; Duysens, J.; Nieuwboer, A.; Krampe, R.T.; Swinnen, S.P. Age-related differences in attentional cost associated with postural dual tasks: Increased recruitment of generic cognitive resources in older adults. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2013, 37, 1824–1837. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  51. Bustillo-Casero, P.; Villarrasa-Sapiña, I.; García-Massó, X. Effects of dual task difficulty in motor and cognitive performance: Differences between adults and adolescents. Hum. Mov. Sci. 2017, 55, 8–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  52. Tokur, D.; Grimmer, M.; Seyfarth, A. Review of balance recovery in response to external perturbations during daily activities. Hum. Mov. Sci. 2020, 69, 102546. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Jakubowski, K.L.; Martino, G.; Beck, O.N.; Sawicki, G.S.; Ting, L.H. Center of mass states render multijoint torques throughout standing balance recovery. J. Neurophysiol. 2025, 133, 206–221. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Bohm, S.; Mademli, L.; Mersmann, F.; Arampatzis, A. Predictive and reactive locomotor adaptability in healthy elderly: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Sports Med. 2015, 45, 1759–1777. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  55. Wunderlich, A.; Wollesen, B.; Asamoah, J.; Delbaere, K.; Li, K. The impact of cognitive-motor interference on balance and gait in hearing-impaired older adults: A systematic review. Eur. Rev. Aging. Phys. Act. 2024, 21, 17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  56. Gursoy, Z.G.; Yilmaz, U.; Celik, H.; Arpinar-Avsar, P.; Kirazci, S. Effect of individualized cognitive and postural task difficulty levels on postural control during dual task condition. Gait Posture 2022, 96, 1–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  57. Potvin-Desrochers, A.; Richer, N.; Lajoie, Y. Cognitive tasks promote automatization of postural control in young and older adults. Gait Posture 2017, 57, 40–45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Polskaia, N.; Richer, N.; Dionne, E.; Lajoie, Y. Continuous cognitive task promotes greater postural stability than an internal or external focus of attention. Gait Posture 2015, 41, 454–458. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. (Left): Schematic representation of the procedures conducted in control condition (no cognitive load involved) for the assessment of (i) postural balance at 3 shoe wearing conditions with a low-, medium- and high-heel height and (ii) cognitive load by the use of two cognitive tasks (COGN_A and COGN_B; details are given in the text). (Right): Schematic representation of the dual-task conditions A and B, which were conducted after a 45 min wash-out period to limit any possible learning effect. The gray horizontal bar in all upright figures represents the force plate that was used for the postural balance assessment.
Figure 1. (Left): Schematic representation of the procedures conducted in control condition (no cognitive load involved) for the assessment of (i) postural balance at 3 shoe wearing conditions with a low-, medium- and high-heel height and (ii) cognitive load by the use of two cognitive tasks (COGN_A and COGN_B; details are given in the text). (Right): Schematic representation of the dual-task conditions A and B, which were conducted after a 45 min wash-out period to limit any possible learning effect. The gray horizontal bar in all upright figures represents the force plate that was used for the postural balance assessment.
Applsci 15 04485 g001
Figure 2. Illustration of the different heel heights used in the study: (A) example of a moccasin style shoe with the low-heel height (1–2 cm), (B) photo of the dress style shoe with the medium-heel height (7 cm), and (C) photo of the dress style shoe with the high-heel height (10.1 cm).
Figure 2. Illustration of the different heel heights used in the study: (A) example of a moccasin style shoe with the low-heel height (1–2 cm), (B) photo of the dress style shoe with the medium-heel height (7 cm), and (C) photo of the dress style shoe with the high-heel height (10.1 cm).
Applsci 15 04485 g002
Figure 3. Flowchart illustrating the experimental procedure for the dual-task conditions to assess possible interference effects on postural balance performance. Dual-task_A (left) required postural balance assessment while concurrently executing a short-memory cognitive task (COGN-A), while in dual-task_B (right), a verbal fluency cognitive task was executed concurrently with the postural balance assessment. The reader is referred to the main text for a detailed description of procedures.
Figure 3. Flowchart illustrating the experimental procedure for the dual-task conditions to assess possible interference effects on postural balance performance. Dual-task_A (left) required postural balance assessment while concurrently executing a short-memory cognitive task (COGN-A), while in dual-task_B (right), a verbal fluency cognitive task was executed concurrently with the postural balance assessment. The reader is referred to the main text for a detailed description of procedures.
Applsci 15 04485 g003
Figure 4. Path length (A), CoP sway range in anteroposterior (B) and CoP sway range in mediolateral direction (C) for the low-, medium- and high-heel heights in the two-legged quiet stance postural balance assessment in the control condition (i.e., without any further cognitive load). Data depict the median and the 5th–95th percentile as whiskers. * Statistically significant (p < 0.05) post hoc comparisons between respective shoe heel heights.
Figure 4. Path length (A), CoP sway range in anteroposterior (B) and CoP sway range in mediolateral direction (C) for the low-, medium- and high-heel heights in the two-legged quiet stance postural balance assessment in the control condition (i.e., without any further cognitive load). Data depict the median and the 5th–95th percentile as whiskers. * Statistically significant (p < 0.05) post hoc comparisons between respective shoe heel heights.
Applsci 15 04485 g004
Figure 5. Path length (A), CoP sway range in anteroposterior (B) and CoP sway range in mediolateral direction (C) in the two-legged quiet stance balance assessment while wearing medium- (gray fill) and high-heel (white fill) height shoes in the control and dual-task conditions. Data depict the median and the 5th–95th percentile as whiskers. * Statistically significant main effect of heel height (p < 0.01). # Statistically significant main effect of cognitive load (p < 0.05).
Figure 5. Path length (A), CoP sway range in anteroposterior (B) and CoP sway range in mediolateral direction (C) in the two-legged quiet stance balance assessment while wearing medium- (gray fill) and high-heel (white fill) height shoes in the control and dual-task conditions. Data depict the median and the 5th–95th percentile as whiskers. * Statistically significant main effect of heel height (p < 0.01). # Statistically significant main effect of cognitive load (p < 0.05).
Applsci 15 04485 g005
Figure 6. Cognitive performance in the short-term memory task (COGN_A) and language-processing task (COGN_B) in sitting (reference) and while wearing medium- and high-heeled shoes. Data refer to the median and the 5th–95th percentile as whiskers.
Figure 6. Cognitive performance in the short-term memory task (COGN_A) and language-processing task (COGN_B) in sitting (reference) and while wearing medium- and high-heeled shoes. Data refer to the median and the 5th–95th percentile as whiskers.
Applsci 15 04485 g006
Table 1. Anthropometric data of participants (n = 21, mean ± SD).
Table 1. Anthropometric data of participants (n = 21, mean ± SD).
Variable
Age (years)37.8 ± 10.7
Body Mass (kg)66.5 ± 10.2
Height (cm)169.0 ± 6.3
Body Mass Index (kg/m2)23.2 ± 2.8
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Nikolaidou, M.-E.; Dervenioti, A.; Schroll, A.; Arampatzis, A. The Mechanically Destabilizing Effect of Increased Heel Height in Women Is Not Enhanced by Dual-Task Interference. Appl. Sci. 2025, 15, 4485. https://doi.org/10.3390/app15084485

AMA Style

Nikolaidou M-E, Dervenioti A, Schroll A, Arampatzis A. The Mechanically Destabilizing Effect of Increased Heel Height in Women Is Not Enhanced by Dual-Task Interference. Applied Sciences. 2025; 15(8):4485. https://doi.org/10.3390/app15084485

Chicago/Turabian Style

Nikolaidou, Maria-Elissavet, Aikaterini Dervenioti, Arno Schroll, and Adamantios Arampatzis. 2025. "The Mechanically Destabilizing Effect of Increased Heel Height in Women Is Not Enhanced by Dual-Task Interference" Applied Sciences 15, no. 8: 4485. https://doi.org/10.3390/app15084485

APA Style

Nikolaidou, M.-E., Dervenioti, A., Schroll, A., & Arampatzis, A. (2025). The Mechanically Destabilizing Effect of Increased Heel Height in Women Is Not Enhanced by Dual-Task Interference. Applied Sciences, 15(8), 4485. https://doi.org/10.3390/app15084485

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop