What’s Special about Human Imitation? A Comparison with Enculturated Apes
Abstract
:1. Introduction
1.1. The Enculturated Mind
1.2. Assessing Domain-Specific Imitation Performance
2. Candidates for Shared Social and Imitation Learning Mechanisms
2.1. Imitation of Familiar Transitive Actions on Objects
2.2. Imitation of Familiar Transitive Actions on the Body
2.3. Imitation of Familiar Object-Object Actions and Tool-Use
2.4. “Rational” Imitation or “Goal Emulation”
3. Candidates for Uniquely Derived Social and Imitation Learning Mechanisms
3.1. Imitation of Intransitive Actions or Gestures
3.2. Imitation of Novel Transitive Actions on Objects
3.3. Imitation of Novel Object-Object Actions or Tool-Use
4. Discussion
4.1. Associative and “General Process” Accounts of Continuities and Discontinuities
4.2. Motivational Accounts for Discontinuities
4.3. Developmental Accounts for Discontinuities
4.4. Humans Possess Imitation-Specific Specializations
5. Future Directions
5.1. Characterizing Difficulties Copying Opaque Transitive Actions
5.2. Characterizing Difficulties Copying Intransitive Actions
5.3. Characterizing Difficulties Copying Novel Actions with Objects
5.4. Data Sharing and Openness
6. Conclusions
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Herrmann, E.; Call, J.; Hernandez-Lloreda, M.V.; Hare, B.; Tomasello, M. Humans have evolved specialized skills of social cognition: The cultural intelligence hypothesis. Science 2007, 317, 1360–1366. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Dean, L.G.; Kendal, R.L.; Schapiro, S.J.; Thierry, B.; Laland, K.N. Identification of the social and cognitive processes underlying human cumulative culture. Science 2012, 335, 1114–1118. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Whiten, A. The scope of culture in chimpanzees, humans and ancestral apes. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 2011, 366, 997–1007. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Tennie, C.; Call, J.; Tomasello, M. Ratcheting up the ratchet: On the evolution of cumulative culture. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 2009, 364, 2405–2415. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Jones, S.S. Imitation in infancy: The development of mimicry. Psychol. Sci. 2007, 18, 593–599. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Piaget, J. Play, Dreams & Imitation; Norton: New York, NY, USA, 1962. [Google Scholar]
- Young, G.S.; Rogers, S.J.; Hutman, T.; Rozga, A.; Sigman, M.; Ozonoff, S. Imitation from 12 to 24 months in autism and typical development: A longitudinal rasch analysis. Dev. Psychol. 2011, 47, 1565–1578. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Tennie, C.; Call, J.; Tomasello, M. Push or pull: Imitation versus emulation in human children and great apes. Ethology 2006, 112, 1159–1169. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tennie, C.; Call, J.; Tomasello, M. Evidence for emulation in chimpanzees in social settings using the floating peanut task. PLoS ONE 2010, 5, e10544. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Call, J.; Carpenter, M.; Tomasello, M. Copying results and copying actions in the process of social learning: Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and human children (Homo sapiens). Anim. Cogn. 2005, 8, 151–163. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Nagell, K.; Olguin, R.S.; Tomasello, M. Processes of social learning in the tool use of chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and human children (Homo sapiens). J. Comp. Psychol. 1993, 107, 174–186. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Galef, B.G. Imitation in animals: History, definition, and interpretation of data from the psychological laboratory. In Social Learning: Psychological and Biological Perspectives; Galef, Z., Ed.; Lawrence Erlbaum: Mahwah, NJ, USA, 1988; pp. 3–28. [Google Scholar]
- Tomasello, M.; Call, J. Primate Cognition; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 1997; pp. 284–303. [Google Scholar]
- Subiaul, F. The imitation faculty in monkeys: Evaluating its features, distribution and evolution. J. Anthropol. Sci. 2007, 85, 35–62. [Google Scholar]
- Wright, N.T.; Cannon, B.R.; Wilder, P.T.; Morgan, M.T.; Varney, K.M.; Zimmer, D.B.; Weber, D.J. Solution structure of s100a1 bound to the capz peptide (trtk12). J. Mol. Biol. 2009, 386, 1265–1277. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Whiten, A.; Ham, R. On the nature and evolution of imitation in the animal kingdom: Reappraisal of a century of research. Adv. Stud. Behav. 1992, 21, 239–283. [Google Scholar]
- Byrne, R.W. Imitation of novel complex actions: What does the evidence from animals mean? Adv. Stud. Behav. Res. Methods 2002, 31, 77–105. [Google Scholar]
- Custance, D.M.; Whiten, A.; Bard, K.A. Can young chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) imitate arbitrary actions? Hayes and hayes (1952) revisited. Behaviour 1995, 132, 837–859. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bering, J.M. A critical review of the “enculturation hypothesis”: The effects of human rearing on great ape social cognition. Anim. Cogn. 2004, 7, 201–212. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Terrace, H.S. Nim, 1st ed.; Random House: New York, NY, USA, 1979. [Google Scholar]
- Kellogg, W.N.; Kellogg, L.A. The Ape and the Child; A Study of Environmental Influence upon Early Behavior; Whittlesey House McGraw-Hill Book Company, Inc.: New York, NY, USA; London, UK, 1933. [Google Scholar]
- Hayes, K.J.; Hayes, C. Imitation in a home-raised chimpanzee. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 1952, 45, 450–459. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Savage-Rumbaugh, E.S.; Murphy, J.; Sevcik, R.A.; Brakke, K.E.; Williams, S.L.; Rumbaugh, D.M. Language comprehension in ape and child. Monogr. Soc. Res. Child Dev. 1993, 58, 1–222. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Tomasello, M.; Call, J. The role of humans in the cognitive development of apes revisited. Anim. Cogn. 2004, 7, 213–215. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Lyn, H.; Russell, J.L.; Hopkins, W.D. The impact of environment on the comprehension of declarative communication in apes. Psychol. Sci. 2010, 21, 360–365. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Vonk, J.; Povinelli, D.J. Social and physical reasoning in human-reared chimpanzees. In Perception, Causation & Objectivity; Roessler, J., Lerman, H., Eilan, N., Eds.; Oxford Univrsity Press: Oxford, UK, 2012; pp. 342–367. [Google Scholar]
- Bjorklund, D.F.; Pellegrini, A.D. The Origins of Human Nature: Evolutionary Developmental Psychology; American Psychological Association: Washington, DC, USA, 2002. [Google Scholar]
- Deacon, T.W. The Symbolic Species: The Co-Evolution of Language and the Brain; Norton Paperback: New York, NY, USA, 1997. [Google Scholar]
- Tomasello, M. The Cultural Origins of Human Cognition; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 1999. [Google Scholar]
- Tomasello, M. A Natural History of Human Thinking; Harvard University Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2014. [Google Scholar]
- Boesch, C. What makes us human (Homo sapiens)? The challenge of cognitive cross-species comparison. J. Comp. Psychol. 2007, 121, 227–240. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bard, K.A.; Leavens, D.A. The importance of development for comparative primatology. Annu. Rev. Anthropol. 2014, 43, 183–200. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Povinelli, D.J. Folk Physics for Apes; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2000. [Google Scholar]
- Tomasello, M.; Call, J. Assessing the validity of ape-human comparisons: A reply to boesch (2007). J. Comp. Psychol. 2008, 122, 449–452. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Tomasello, M.; Savage-Rumbaugh, S.; Kruger, A.C. Imitative learning of actions on objects by children, chimpanzees, and enculturated chimpanzees. Child Dev. 1993, 64, 1688–1705. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Heyes, C. Grist and mills: On the cultural origins of cultural learning. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 2012, 367, 2181–2191. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Heyes, C. When does social learning become cultural learning? Dev. Sci. 2015. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Heyes, C. What’s social about social learning? J. Comp. Psychol. 2012, 126, 193–202. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Tessari, A.; Rumiati, R.I. The strategic control of multiple routes in imitation of actions. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 2004, 30, 1107–1116. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Subiaul, F. Dissecting the imitation faculty: The multiple imitation mechanisms (mim) hypothesis. Behav. Process. 2010, 83, 222–234. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Jones, S.S. The development of imitation in infancy. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 2009, 364, 2325–2335. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Thorpe, W.H. Learning and Instinct in Animals; Methuen: London, UK, 1956. [Google Scholar]
- Galef, B.G. Social learning and imitation. In Social Learning in Animals: The Roots of Culture; Heyes, G., Ed.; Academic Press, Inc: San Diego, CA, USA, 1996. [Google Scholar]
- Tomasello, M.; Davis-Dasilva, M.; Camak, L.; Bard, K. Observational learning of tool-use by young chimpanzees. J. Hum. Evol. 1987, 2, 175–186. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Custance, D.; Whiten, A.; Fredman, T. Social learning of an artificial fruit task in capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella). J. Comp. Psychol. 1999, 113, 13–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Subiaul, F.; Patterson, E.M.; Barr, R. The cognitive structure of goal emulation during the preschool years. Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 2015. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Whiten, A.; Custance, D.M.; Gomez, J.; Teixidor, P. Imitative learning of artificial fruit processing in children (Homo sapiens) and chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). J. Comp. Psychol. 1996, 110, 3–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Byrne, R.W.; Russon, A.E. Learning by imitation: A hierarchical approach. Behav. Brain Sci. 1998, 21, 667–721. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Whiten, A.; McGuigan, N.; Marshall-Pescini, S.; Hopper, L.M. Emulation, imitation, over-imitation and the scope of culture for child and chimpanzee. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 2009, 364, 2417–2428. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Zentall, T.R. Perspectives on observational learning in animals. J. Comp. Psychol. 2012, 126, 114–128. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Carpenter, M.; Call, J. The chemistry of social learning. Dev. Sci. 2002, 5, 22–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Heyes, C. Imitation by association. In Perspectives on Imitation; Hurley, S., Chater, N., Eds.; The MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2004; Volume 1, pp. 157–176. [Google Scholar]
- Heyes, C.; Bird, G.; Johnson, H.; Haggard, P. Experience modulates automatic imitation. Brain Res. Cogn. Brain Res. 2005, 22, 233–240. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Subiaul, F.; Anderson, S.; Brandt, J.; Elkins, J. Multiple imitation mechanisms in children. Dev. Psychol. 2012, 48, 1165–1179. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Subiaul, F.; Patterson, E.M.; Schilder, B.; Renner, E.; Barr, R. Becoming a high-fidelity-super-imitator: What are the contributions of social and individual learning? Dev. Sci. 2014. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Zentall, T.R. An analysis of imitative learning in animals. In Social Learning in Animals: The Roots of Culture; Heyes, C., Galef, B., Eds.; Academic Press: San Diego, CA, USA, 1996; pp. 221–238. [Google Scholar]
- Subiaul, F. Mosaic cognitive evolution: The case of imitation learning. In The Human Brain Evolving; Broadfield, D., Yuan, M., Schick, K., Toth, N., Eds.; Stone Age Institute Press: Gosport, UK, 2010. [Google Scholar]
- Bates, L.A.; Byrne, R.W. Imitation: What animal imitation tells us about animal cognition. Wiley Interdiscip. Rev. Cogn. Sci. 2010, 1, 685–695. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Jeannerod, M. Neural simulation of action: A unifying mechanism for motor cognition. Neuroimage 2001, 14, S103–S109. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Nehaniv, C.L.; Dautenhahn, K. The correspondence problem. In Imitation in Animals and Artifacts; Nehaniv, C.L., Dautenhahn, K., Eds.; MIT University Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2002; pp. 41–61. [Google Scholar]
- Tessari, A.; Canessa, N.; Ukmar, M.; Rumiati, R.I. Neuropsychological evidence for a strategic control of multiple routes in imitation. Brain 2007, 130, 1111–1126. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bonivento, C.; Rothstein, P.; Humphreys, G.; Chechlacz, M. Neural correlates of transitive and intransitive action imitation: An investigation using voxel-based morphometry. Neuroimage Clin. 2014, 6, 488–497. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Byrne, R.W.; Tanner, J.E. Gestural imitation by a gorilla: Evidence and nature of the capacity. Int. J. Psychol. Psychol.Ther. 2006, 6, 215–231. [Google Scholar]
- Catmur, C.; Walsh, V.; Heyes, C. Associative sequence learning: The role of experience in the development of imitation and the mirror system. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 2009, 364, 2369–2380. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Cook, R.; Press, C.; Dickinson, A.; Heyes, C. Acquisition of automatic imitation is sensitive to sensorimotor contingency. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 2010, 36, 840–852. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Cooper, R.P.; Cook, R.; Dickinson, A.; Heyes, C.M. Associative (not hebbian) learning and the mirror neuron system. Neurosci. Lett. 2013, 540, 28–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Leighton, J.; Heyes, C. Hand to mouth: Automatic imitation across effector systems. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 2010, 36, 1174–1183. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Rumiati, R.I.; Tessari, A. Imitation of novel and well-known actions: The role of short-term memory. Exp. Brain Res. 2002, 142, 425–433. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Rumiati, R.I.; Weiss, P.H.; Tessari, A.; Assmus, A.; Zilles, K.; Herzog, H.; Fink, G.R. Common and differential neural mechanisms supporting imitation of meaningful and meaningless actions. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2005, 17, 1420–1431. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Engel, A.; Burke, M.; Fiehler, K.; Bien, S.; Rosler, F. Motor learning affects visual movement perception. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2008, 27, 2294–2302. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Grossberg, S.; Paine, R.W. A neural model of cortico-cerebellar interactions during attentive imitation and predictive learning of sequential handwriting movements. Neural Netw. 2000, 13, 999–1046. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lopes, M.; Santos-Victor, J. Visual learning by imitation with motor representations. IEEE Trans. Syst. Man Cybern. B Cybern. 2005, 35, 438–449. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Reithler, J.; van Mier, H.I.; Peters, J.C.; Goebel, R. Nonvisual motor learning influences abstract action observation. Curr. Biol. 2007, 17, 1201–1207. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Heyes, C. Homo imitans? Seven reasons why imitation couldn’t possibly be associative. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 2016, 371, 20150069. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Paquette, D. Discovering and learning tool-use for fishing honey by captive chimpanzees. Hum. Evol. 1992, 7, 17–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Takeshita, H.; Van Hooff, J.A.R.M. Tool use by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) of the arnhem zoo community. Jpn. Psychol. Res. 1996, 38, 163–173. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bauer, P.J. Holding it all together: How enabling relations facilitate young children’s event recall. Cogn. Dev. 1992, 7, 1–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bauer, P.J.; Hertsgaard, L.A.; Dropik, P.; Daly, B.P. When even arbitrary order becomes important: Developments in reliable temporal sequencing of arbitratrily ordered events. Memory 1998, 6, 165–198. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bauer, P.J.; Mandler, J.M. One thing follows another: Effects of temporal structure on 1- to 2-year olds’ recall of events. Dev. Psychol. 1989, 25, 197–206. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Subiaul, F.; Cantlon, J.F.; Holloway, R.L.; Terrace, H.S. Cognitive imitation in rhesus macaques. Science 2004, 305, 407–410. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Renner, E.; Price, E.E.; Subiaul, F. Sequential recall of meaningful and arbitrary sequences by orangutans and human children: Does content matter? Anim. Cogn. 2016, 19, 39–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Renner, E.; Subiaul, F. Individual, Observational, and Social Learning in Orangutans; American Association of Physical Anthropology: St. Louis, MO, USA, 2015. [Google Scholar]
- Subiaul, F.; Schilder, B. Working memory constraints on imitation and emulation. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2014, 128, 190–200. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Subiaul, F.; Zimmermann, L.; Renner, E.; Schilder, B.; Barr, R. Elemental imitation mechanisms in preschool age children. J. Cogn. Dev. 2016, 17, 221–243. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Terrace, H.S. The simultaneous chain: A new approach to serial learning. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2005, 9, 202–210. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Henrich, J.; McElreath, R. The evolution of cultural evolution. Evo. Anthropol. 2003, 12, 123–135. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rakoczy, H.; Tomasello, M.; Striano, T. On tools and toys: How children learn to act on and pretend with ‘virgin objects’. Dev. Sci. 2005, 8, 57–73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Gardner, R.A.; Gardner, B.T. Teaching sign language to a chimpanzee. Science 1969, 165, 664–672. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Sanders, R.J. Teaching apes to ape language: Explaining the imitative and nonimitative signing of a chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes). J. Comp. Psychol. 1985, 99, 197–210. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Call, J.; Tomasello, M. Use of social information in the problem solving of orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus) and human children (Homo sapiens). J. Comp. Psychol. 1995, 109, 308–320. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Myowa-Yamakoshi, M.; Tomonaga, M.; Tanaka, M.; Matsuzawa, T. Imitation in neonatal chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Dev. Sci. 2004, 7, 437–442. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Call, J. Body imitation in an enculturated orangutan (Pongo pygmaeus). Cybern. Syst. 2001, 32, 97–119. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tomasello, M.; Carpenter, M.; Call, J.; Behne, T.; Moll, H. Understanding and sharing intentions: The origins of cultural cognition. Behav. Brain Sci. 2005, 28, 675–735. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Carrasco, L.; Posada, S.; Colell, M. New evidence on imitation in an enculturated chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes). J. Comp. Psychol. 2009, 123, 385–390. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Myowa-Yamakoshi, M.; Matsuzawa, T. Factors influencing imitation of manipulatory actions in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). J. Comp. Psychol. 1999, 113, 128–136. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Miles, H.L.; Mitchell, R.W.; Harper, S.E. Simon says: The development of imitation in an enculturated orangutan. In Reaching into Thought: The Minds of the Great Apes; Russon, A.E., Bard, K., Parker, S.T., Eds.; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 1996; pp. 278–300. [Google Scholar]
- Tomasello, M.; Kruger, A.; Ratner, H. Cultural learning. Behav. Brain Sci. 1993, 16, 496–511. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Buttelmann, D.; Carpenter, M.; Call, J.; Tomasello, M. Enculturated chimpanzees imitate rationally. Dev. Sci. 2007, 10, F31–F38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Tomasello, M.; Carpenter, M. The emergence of social cognition in three young chimpanzees. Monogr. Soc. Res. Child Dev. 2005, 70, vii–132. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
- Hopper, L.M.; Spiteri, A.; Lambeth, S.P.; Schapiro, S.J.; Horner, V.; Whiten, A. Experimental studies of traditions and underlying transmission processes in chimpanzees. Anim. Behav. 2007, 73, 1021–1032. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Horner, V.; Whiten, A. Causal knowledge and imitation/emulation switching in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and children (Homo sapiens). Anim. Cogn. 2005, 8, 164–181. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Whiten, A.; Horner, V.; de Waal, F.B. Conformity to cultural norms of tool use in chimpanzees. Nature 2005, 437, 737–740. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Whiten, A. Imitation of the sequential structure of actions by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). J. Comp. Psychol. 1998, 112, 270–281. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Horner, V.; Whiten, A.; Flynn, E.; de Waal, F.B. Faithful replication of foraging techniques along cultural transmission chains by chimpanzees and children. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2006, 103, 13878–13883. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Gergely, G.; Bekkering, H.; Kiraly, I. Rational imitation in preverbal infants. Nature 2002, 415, 755. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Buttelmann, D.; Carpenter, M.; Call, J.; Tomasello, M. Rational tool use and tool choice in human infants and great apes. Child Dev. 2008, 79, 609–626. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Gleissner, B.; Meltzoff, A.N.; Bekkering, H. Children’s coding of human action: Cognitive factors influencing imitation in 3-year-olds. Dev. Sci. 2000, 3, 405–414. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Meltzoff, A.N.; Moore, M.K. Explaining facial imitation: A theoretical model. Early Dev. Parent. 1997, 6, 179–192. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ray, E.; Heyes, C. Imitation in infancy: The wealth of the stimulus. Dev. Sci. 2011, 14, 92–105. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Terrace, H.S.; Petitto, L.A.; Sanders, R.J.; Bever, T.G. Can an ape create a sentence? Science 1979, 206, 891–902. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Fouts, R.S. Use of guidance in teaching sign language to a chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes). J. Comp. Psychol. 1972, 80, 515–522. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Shapiro, G.L. Factors Influencing the Variance in Sign Language Performance by Four Juvenile Orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus); University of Oklahoma: Norman, OK, USA, 1985. [Google Scholar]
- Patterson, F. Linguistic capabilities of a lowland gorilla. In Sign Language and Language Acquisition in Man and Ape; Peng, F.C.C., Ed.; Westview Press: Boulder, CO, USA, 1978; pp. 161–201. [Google Scholar]
- Bard, K.A. Neonatal imitation in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) tested with two paradigms. Anim. Cogn. 2007, 10, 233–242. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ferrari, P.F.; Paukner, A.; Ruggiero, A.; Darcey, L.; Unbehagen, S.; Suomi, S.J. Interindividual differences in neonatal imitation and the development of action chains in rhesus macaques. Child Dev. 2009, 80, 1057–1068. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ferrari, P.F.; Visalberghi, E.; Paukner, A.; Fogassi, L.; Ruggiero, A.; Suomi, S.J. Neonatal imitation in rhesus macaques. PLoS Biol. 2006, 4, e302. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Meltzoff, A.N.; Moore, K.M. Newborn infants imitate adult facial gestures. Child Dev. 1983, 54, 702–709. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Meltzoff, A.N.; Moore, M.K. Imitation of facial and manual gestures by human neonates. Science 1977, 198, 75–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Simpson, E.A.; Murray, L.; Paukner, A.; Ferrari, P.F. The mirror neuron system as revealed through neonatal imitation: Presence from birth, predictive power and evidence of plasticity. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 2014, 369, 20130289. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Oostenbroek, J.; Suddendorf, T.; Nielsen, M.; Redshaw, J.; Kennedy-Costantini, S.; Davis, J.; Clark, S.; Slaughter, V. Comprehensive longitudinal study challenges the existence of neonatal imitation in humans. Curr. Biol. 2016, 26, 1334–1338. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Diamond, A. Developmental time course in human infants and infant monkeys, and the neural bases of, inhibitory control in reaching. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1990, 608, 637–669. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Watanabe, H.; Forssman, L.; Green, D.; Bohlin, G.; von Hofsten, C. Attention demands influence 10- and 12-month-old infants’ perseverative behavior. Dev. Psychol. 2012, 48, 46–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Rogers, S.J.; Hepburn, S.L.; Stackhouse, T.; Wehner, E. Imitation performance in toddlers with autism and those with other developmental disorders. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 2003, 44, 763–781. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Heimann, M.; Nelson, K.E.; Schaller, J. Neonatal imitation of tongue protrusion and mouth opening: Methodological aspects and evidence of early individual differences. Scand. J. Psychol. 1989, 30, 90–101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Want, S.C.; Harris, P.L. Learning from other people’s mistakes: Causal understanding in learning to use a tool. Child Dev. 2001, 72, 431–443. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Thorndike, E.L. Animal intelligence: An experimental study of the associative processes in animals. Psychol. Rev. Monogr. Suppl. 1898, 2. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Thorndike, E.L. Animal Intelligence; Macmillan: New York, NY, USA, 1911. [Google Scholar]
- Povinelli, D.J.; Bering, J.M.; Giambrone, S. Toward a science of other minds: Escaping the argument by analogy. Cogn. Sci. 2000, 24, 509–541. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Myowa-Yamakoshi, M.; Matsuzawa, T. Imitation of intentional manipulatory actions in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). J. Comp. Psychol. 2000, 114, 381–391. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Gillespie-Lynch, K.; Greenfield, P.M.; Lyn, H.; Savage-Rumbaugh, S. Gestural and symbolic development among apes and humans: Support for a multimodal theory of language evolution. Front. Psychol. 2014, 5, 1228. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Gardner, R.A.; Gardner, B.T. Early signs of language in child and chimpanzee. Science 1975, 187, 752–753. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Vanvuchelen, M.; Roeyers, H.; Weerdt, W.D. Development and initial validation of the preschool imitation and praxis scale (pips). Res. Autism Spectr. Disord. 2011, 5, 463–473. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Whiten, A.; Goodall, J.; McGrew, W.C.; Nishida, T.; Reynolds, V.; Sugiyama, Y.; Tutin, C.E.; Wrangham, R.W.; Boesch, C. Cultures in chimpanzees. Nature 1999, 399, 682–685. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- van Schaik, C.P.; Ancrenaz, M.; Borgen, G.; Galdikas, B.; Knott, C.D.; Singleton, I.; Suzuki, A.; Utami, S.S.; Merrill, M. Orangutan cultures and the evolution of material culture. Science 2003, 299, 102–105. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Baddeley, A. Working memory: Theories, models, and controversies. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2012, 63, 1–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Tessari, A.; Bosanac, D.; Rumiati, R.I. Effect of learning on imitation of new actions: Implications for a memory model. Exp. Brain Res. 2006, 173, 507–513. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Harnick, F.S. The relationship between ability level and task difficulty in producing imitation in infants. Child Dev. 1978, 49, 209–212. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Silberberg, A.; Kearns, D. Memory for the order of briefly presented numerals in humans as a function of practice. Anim. Cogn. 2009, 12, 405–407. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Carpenter, M.; Tomasello, M.; Savage-Rumbaugh, S. Joint attention and imitative learning in children, chimpanzees, and enculturated chimpanzees. Soc. Dev. 1995, 4, 217–237. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Poti, P.; Parenti, F. Primates’ constructional abilities. J. Anthropol. Sci. 2013, 91, 111–133. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
- Nevid, J.S. Essentials of Psychology: Concepts and Applications, 4th ed.; Cengage Learning: Belmont, CA, USA, 2015. [Google Scholar]
- Devine, R.T.; Hughes, C. Relations between false belief understanding and executive function in early childhood: A meta-analysis. Child Dev. 2014, 85, 1777–1794. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Barrett, H.C. The Shape of Thought: How Mental Adaptations Evolve; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 2015. [Google Scholar]
- De Waal, F.B.; Boesch, C.; Horner, V.; Whiten, A. Comparing social skills of children and apes. Science 2008, 319, 569. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hattori, Y.; Kano, F.; Tomonaga, M. Differential sensitivity to conspecific and allospecific cues in chimpanzees and humans: A comparative eye-tracking study. Biol. Lett. 2010, 6, 610–613. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kano, F.; Tomonaga, M. How chimpanzees look at pictures: A comparative eye-tracking study. Proc. Biol. Sci. 2009, 276, 1949–1955. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Rosati, A.G.; Wobber, V.; Hughes, K.; Santos, L.R. Comparative Dev. Psychol.y: How is human cognitive development unique? Evol. Psychol. 2014, 12, 448–473. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Subiaul, F.; Okamoto-Barth, S.; Bath, J.; Povinelli, D.J. Human cognitive specializations. In Evolution of Nervous Systems: Volume V; Preuss, T., Kaas, J., Eds.; Elsevier: New York, NY, USA, 2006. [Google Scholar]
- Moll, H.; Tomasello, M. 12- and 18-month-old infants follow gaze to spaces behind barriers. Dev. Sci. 2004, 7, F1–F9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Okamoto, S.; Tomonaga, M.; Ishii, K.; Kawai, N.; Tanaka, M.; Matsuzawa, T. An infant chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) follows human gaze. Anim. Cogn. 2002, 5, 107–114. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Okamoto-Barth, S.; Tomonaga, M.; Tanaka, M.; Matsuzawa, T. Development of using experimenter-given cues in infant chimpanzees: Longitudinal changes in behavior and cognitive development. Dev. Sci. 2008, 11, 98–108. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Woodward, A.L. Infants selectively encode the goal object of an actor’s reach. Cognition 1998, 69, 1–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Csibra, G.; Gergely, G.; Biro, S.; Koos, O.; Brockbank, M. Goal attribution without agency cues: The perception of ‘pure reason’ in infancy. Cognition 1999, 72, 237–267. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gergely, G.; Csibra, G. Teleological reasoning in infancy: The infant’s naive theory of rational action. A reply to premack and premack. Cognition 1997, 63, 227–233. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wobber, V.; Herrmann, E.; Hare, B.; Wrangham, R.; Tomasello, M. Differences in the early cognitive development of children and great apes. Dev. Psychobiol. 2014, 56, 547–573. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Matsuzawa, T. Comparative cognitive development. Dev. Sci. 2007, 10, 97–103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Tomonaga, M. Development of chimpanzee social cognition in the first 2 years of life. In Cognitive Development in Chimpanzees; Matsuzawa, M., Tomonaga, M., Tanaka, M., Eds.; Springer-Verlag: Tokyo, Japan, 2006; pp. 182–197. [Google Scholar]
- Carpenter, M.; Nagell, K.; Tomasello, M. Social cognition, joint attention, and communicative competence from 9 to 15 months of age. Monogr. Soc. Res. Child Dev. 1998, 63, 1–143. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Slaughter, V.; McConnell, D. Emergence of joint attention: Relationships between gaze following, social referencing, imitation, and naming in infancy. J. Genet. Psychol. 2003, 164, 54–71. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kummer, H. In Quest of the Sacred Baboon: A Scientist’s Journey; Princeton University Press: Princeton, NJ, USA, 1995. [Google Scholar]
- Lyons, D.E. The rational continuum of human imitation. In Mirror Neuron Systems: The Role of Mirroring Processes in Social Cognition; Pineda, J.A., Ed.; Human Press: New York, NY, USA, 2009; pp. 77–106. [Google Scholar]
- Lyons, D.E.; Young, A.G.; Keil, F.C. The hidden structure of overimitation. Proc. Nata.l Acad. Sci. USA 2007, 104, 19751–19756. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Csibra, G.; Gergely, G. Natural pedagogy as evolutionary adaptation. Philos. Tran. R. Soc. Lond. Ser. B Biol. Sci. 2011, 366, 1149–1157. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Csibra, G.; Gergely, G. Natural pedagogy. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2009, 13, 148–153. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Csibra, G. Teleological and referential understanding of action in infancy. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 2003, 358, 447–458. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Huang, C.T.; Heyes, C.; Charman, T. Infants’ behavioral reenactment of “failed attempts”: Exploring the roles of emulation learning, stimulus enhancement, and understanding of intentions. Dev. Psychol. 2002, 38, 840–855. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hayes, C. The Ape in Our House, 1st ed.; Harper: New York, NY, USA, 1951; p. 247. [Google Scholar]
- Patterson, F.G. The gestures of a gorilla: Language acquisition in another pongid. Brain Lang. 1978, 5, 72–97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Goldin-Meadow, S. Widening the lens: What the manual modality reveals about language, learning and cognition. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 2014, 369, 20130295. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bates, E. Language and Context: The Acquisition of Pragmatics; Academic Press: New York, NY, USA, 1976. [Google Scholar]
- Lyn, H.; Greenfield, P.M.; Savage-Rumbaugh, S.; Gillespie-Lynch, K.; Hopkins, W.D. Nonhuman primates do declare! A comparison of declarative symbol and gesture use in two children, two bonobos, and a chimpanzee. Lang. Commun. 2011, 31, 63–74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Tomasello, M. Origins of Human Communication; MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2008. [Google Scholar]
- Tomasello, M.; Carpenter, M. Shared intentionality. Dev. Sci. 2007, 10, 121–125. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Rivas, E. Recent use of signs by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) in interactions with humans. J. Comp. Psychol. 2005, 119, 404–417. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- De Veer, M.W.; Gallup, G.G., Jr.; Theall, L.A.; van den Bos, R.; Povinelli, D.J. An 8-year longitudinal study of mirror self-recognition in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Neuropsychologia 2003, 41, 229–234. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mahovetz, L.M.; Young, L.J.; Hopkins, W.D. The influence of avpr1a genotype on individual differences in behaviors during a mirror self-recognition task in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Genes Brain Behav. 2016, 15, 445–452. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Pope, S.M.; Russell, J.L.; Hopkins, W.D. The association between imitation recognition and socio-communicative competencies in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Front. Psychol. 2015, 6, 188. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Bird, G.; Brindley, R.; Leighton, J.; Heyes, C. General processes, rather than “goals,” explain imitation errors. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 2007, 33, 1158–1169. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bates, E.; Dick, F. Language, gesture, and the developing brain. Dev. Psychobiol. 2002, 40, 293–310. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Holmer, E.; Heimann, M.; Rudner, M. Imitation, sign language skill and the developmental ease of language understanding (d-elu) model. Front. Psychol. 2016, 7, 107. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Limongelli, L.; Boysen, S.T.; Visalberghi, E. Comprehension of cause-effect relations in a tool-using task by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). J. Comp. Psychol. 1995, 109, 18–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Eriksen, B.A.; Eriksen, C.W. Effects of noise letters upon identification of a target letter in a non-search task. Percept. Psychophys. 1974, 16, 143–149. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Stroop, J.D. Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. J. Exp. Psychol. 1935, 18, 643–662. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Humstone, H.J. Memory span tests. Psychol. Clin. 1919, 12, 196–200. [Google Scholar]
- Wimmer, H.; Perner, J. Beliefs about beliefs: Representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children’s understanding of deception. Cognition 1983, 13, 103–128. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Thorndike, E.L.; Woodworth, R.S. The influence of improvement in one mental function upon the efficiency of other functions. Psychol. Rev. 1901, 8, 247–261. [Google Scholar]
- Benton, A.L.; Fogel, M.L. Three-dimensional constructional praxis. A clinical test. Arch. Neurol. 1962, 7, 347–354. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Gordon, A.S.; Millman, D.S.; Steiger, L.; Adolph, K.E.; Gilmore, R.O. Researcher-library collaborations: Data repositories as a service for researchers. J. Libr. Sch. Commun. 2015, 3, 2–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kiraly, I.; Csibra, G.; Gergely, G. Beyond rational imitation: Learning arbitrary means actions from communicative demonstrations. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2013, 116, 471–486. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Jeannerod, M. The mechanism of self-recognition in humans. Behav. Brain Res. 2003, 142, 1–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Transparent-Visible Actions | Opaque-Non-Visible Actions | Transparent/Opaque | ||
---|---|---|---|---|
Novel/Familiar | Novel | |||
Study [Subject] | Species | Intransitive | Intransitive | Bi-Manual |
Viki [22] | Chimpanzee | ^ | ^ | ^ |
Washoe [88] | Chimpanzee | – | – | |
Nim [89] | Chimpanzee | – | – | |
Kanzi [53] | Bonobo | – | ||
Panbanisha [53] | Bonobo | – | ||
Panzee [53] | Chimpanzee | – | ||
KA [18] | Chimpanzee | ^ | ^ | ^ |
SC [18] | Chimpanzee | ^ | – | ^ |
Chantek [90] | Orangutan | |||
PRI [91]* | Chimpanzee | |||
Chantek [92] | Orangutan | ^ | ^ | ^ |
PRI Chimpanzees Alex [93] | Chimpanzee | |||
Annet [93] | Chimpanzee | |||
Alexandra [93] | Chimpanzee | |||
Zura [63] | Gorilla | ^ | ^ | |
Lili [94] | Chimpanzee | + | + |
Transparent-Visible Actions | Opaque-Non-Visible Actions | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Novel | Novel | Novel | Novel | ||
Study [Subject] | Species | Object-Object | Transitive | Object-Object | Transitive |
Viki [22] | Chimpanzee | – | ^ | ||
Washoe [88] | Chimpanzee | ||||
Nim [89] | Chimpanzee | ||||
Kanzi [53] | Bonobo | ^ | ^ | ^ | |
Panbanisha [53] | Bonobo | ^ | ^ | + | |
Panzee [53] | Chimpanzee | ^ | ^ | ^ | |
KA [18] | Chimpanzee | ^ | ^ | ||
SC [18] | Chimpanzee | ^ | ^ | ||
Chantek [90] | Orangutan | – | |||
PRI [91]* | Chimpanzee | ^ | – | – | |
Chantek [92] | Orangutan | ||||
PRI Chimpanzees Alex [93] | Chimpanzee | – | – | ||
Annet [93] | Chimpanzee | – | + | ||
Alexandra [93] | Chimpanzee | + | ^ | ||
Zura [63] | Gorilla | ||||
Lili [94] | Chimpanzee | – | ^ | – | ^ |
© 2016 by the author; licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC-BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Subiaul, F. What’s Special about Human Imitation? A Comparison with Enculturated Apes. Behav. Sci. 2016, 6, 13. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs6030013
Subiaul F. What’s Special about Human Imitation? A Comparison with Enculturated Apes. Behavioral Sciences. 2016; 6(3):13. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs6030013
Chicago/Turabian StyleSubiaul, Francys. 2016. "What’s Special about Human Imitation? A Comparison with Enculturated Apes" Behavioral Sciences 6, no. 3: 13. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs6030013
APA StyleSubiaul, F. (2016). What’s Special about Human Imitation? A Comparison with Enculturated Apes. Behavioral Sciences, 6(3), 13. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs6030013