The Aesthetic Developmental Characteristics of Contour Features in Children and Adolescents with High- and Low- Level Visual Aesthetic Sensitivity across Grade Levels
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Methods
2.1. Participants
2.2. Experimental Design
2.3. Materials
2.4. Experimental Apparatus
2.5. Experimental Procedures
3. Results
3.1. Preference of Contour Features
3.2. The Beauty of the Contour Features
4. Discussion
4.1. Visual Aesthetic Preferences for Contour Features
4.2. Aesthetic Differences between Grades on Contour Features
4.3. The Effect of Level of Visual Aesthetic Sensitivity on Aesthetic Preference for Contour Features
4.4. Limitations and Future Directions
5. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Chuquichambi, E.G.; Vartanian, O.; Skov, M.; Corradi, G.B.; Nadal, M.; Silvia, P.J.; Munar, E. How Universal Is Preference for Visual Curvature? A Systematic Review and Meta-analysis. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 2022, 1518, 151–165. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Corradi, G.; Munar, E. The Curvature Effect. In The Oxford Handbook of Empirical Aesthetics; Nadal, M., Vartanian, O., Eds.; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2020; pp. 510–532. ISBN 978-0-19-882435-0. [Google Scholar]
- Silvia, P.J.; Barona, C.M. Do People Prefer Curved Objects? Angularity, Expertise, and Aesthetic Preference. Empir. Stud. Arts 2009, 27, 25–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hevner, K. Experimental Studies of the Affective Value of Colors and Lines. J. Appl. Psychol. 1935, 19, 385–398. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lundholm, H. The Affective Tone of Lines: Experimental Researches. Psychol. Rev. 1921, 28, 43–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Poffenberger, A.T. The Feeling Value of Lines. J. Appl. Psychol. 1924, 8, 187–205. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bertamini, M.; Palumbo, L.; Gheorghes, T.N.; Galatsidas, M. Do Observers like Curvature or Do They Dislike Angularity? Br. J. Psychol. 2016, 107, 154–178. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Palmer, S.E.; Schloss, K.B.; Sammartino, J. Visual Aesthetics and Human Preference. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2013, 64, 77–107. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Munar, E.; Gómez-Puerto, G.; Call, J.; Nadal, M. Common Visual Preference for Curved Contours in Humans and Great Apes. PLoS ONE 2015, 10, e0141106. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bar, M.; Neta, M. Humans Prefer Curved Visual Objects. Psychol. Sci. 2006, 17, 645–648. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Chuquichambi, E.G.; Corradi, G.B.; Munar, E.; Rosselló-Mir, J. When Symmetric and Curved Visual Contour Meet Intentional Instructions: Hedonic Value and Preference. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 2021, 74, 1525–1541. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Cotter, K.N.; Silvia, P.J.; Bertamini, M.; Palumbo, L.; Vartanian, O. Curve Appeal: Exploring Individual Differences in Preference for Curved versus Angular Objects. i-Perception 2017, 8, 204166951769302. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Gómez-Puerto, G.; Munar, E.; Nadal, M. Preference for Curvature: A Historical and Conceptual Framework. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2016, 9, 712. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Leder, H.; Tinio, P.P.L.; Bar, M. Emotional Valence Modulates the Preference for Curved Objects. Perception 2011, 40, 649–655. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Corradi, G.; Chuquichambi, E.G.; Barrada, J.R.; Clemente, A.; Nadal, M. A New Conception of Visual Aesthetic Sensitivity. Br. J. Psychol. 2019, 111, 630–658. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Corradi, G.; Rosselló-Mir, J.; Vañó, J.; Chuquichambi, E.; Bertamini, M.; Munar, E. The Effects of Presentation Time on Preference for Curvature of Real Objects and Meaningless Novel Patterns. Br. J. Psychol. 2019, 110, 670–685. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Palumbo, L.; Rampone, G.; Bertamini, M. The Role of Gender and Academic Degree on Preference for Smooth Curvature of Abstract Shapes. PeerJ 2021, 9, e10877. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Palumbo, L.; Rampone, G.; Bertamini, M.; Sinico, M.; Clarke, E.; Vartanian, O. Visual Preference for Abstract Curvature and for Interior Spaces: Beyond Undergraduate Student Samples. Psychol. Aesthet. Creat. Arts 2022, 16, 577–593. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chuquichambi, E.G.; Palumbo, L.; Rey, C.; Munar, E. Shape Familiarity Modulates Preference for Curvature in Drawings of Common-Use Objects. PeerJ 2021, 9, e11772. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Clemente, A.; Pearce, M.T.; Skov, M.; Nadal, M. Evaluative Judgment across Domains: Liking Balance, Contour, Symmetry and Complexity in Melodies and Visual Designs. Brain Cogn. 2021, 151, 105729. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Gómez-Puerto, G.; Rosselló, J.; Corradi, G.; Acedo-Carmona, C.; Munar, E.; Nadal, M. Preference for Curved Contours across Cultures. Psychol. Aesthet. Creat. Arts 2018, 12, 432–439. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kastl, A.J.; Child, I.L. Emotional Meaning of Four Typographical Variables. J. Appl. Psychol. 1968, 52, 440–446. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Velasco, C.; Woods, A.T.; Hyndman, S.; Spence, C. The Taste of Typeface. i-Perception 2015, 6, 2041669515593040. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ruta, N.; Vañó, J.; Pepperell, R.; Corradi, G.B.; Chuquichambi, E.G.; Rey, C.; Munar, E. Preference for Paintings Is Also Affected by Curvature. Psychol. Aesthet. Creat. Arts 2023, 17, 307–321. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Leder, H.; Carbon, C.-C. Dimensions in Appreciation of Car Interior Design. Appl. Cogn. Psychol. 2005, 19, 603–618. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Westerman, S.J.; Gardner, P.H.; Sutherland, E.J.; White, T.; Jordan, K.; Watts, D.; Wells, S. Product Design: Preference for Rounded versus Angular Design Elements: Rounded versus Angular Design. Psychol. Mark. 2012, 29, 595–605. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dazkir, S.S.; Read, M.A. Furniture Forms and Their Influence on Our Emotional Responses Toward Interior Environments. Environ. Behav. 2012, 44, 722–732. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tawil, N.; Ascone, L.; Kühn, S. The Contour Effect: Differences in the Aesthetic Preference and Stress Response to Photo-Realistic Living Environments. Front. Psychol. 2022, 13, 933344. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Vartanian, O.; Navarrete, G.; Chatterjee, A.; Fich, L.B.; Leder, H.; Modroño, C.; Nadal, M.; Rostrup, N.; Skov, M. Impact of Contour on Aesthetic Judgments and Approach-Avoidance Decisions in Architecture. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2013, 110, 10446–10453. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vartanian, O.; Navarrete, G.; Chatterjee, A.; Fich, L.B.; Leder, H.; Modroño, C.; Rostrup, N.; Skov, M.; Corradi, G.; Nadal, M. Preference for Curvilinear Contour in Interior Architectural Spaces: Evidence from Experts and Nonexperts. Psychol. Aesthet. Creat. Arts 2019, 13, 110–116. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gómez-Puerto, G.; Munar, E.; Acedo, C.; Gomila, A. Is the Human Initial Preference for Rounded Shapes Universal? Preliminary Results of an Ongoing Cross-Cultural Research. In Perception; Pion: London, UK, 25 August 2013; Volume 42, p. 102. [Google Scholar]
- Ruta, N.; Mastandrea, S.; Penacchio, O.; Lamaddalena, S.; Bove, G. A Comparison between Preference Judgments of Curvature and Sharpness in Architectural Façades. Archit. Sci. Rev. 2019, 62, 171–181. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Carbon, C.-C. The Cycle of Preference: Long-Term Dynamics of Aesthetic Appreciation. Acta Psychol. 2010, 134, 233–244. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fantz, R.L.; Miranda, S.B. Newborn Infant Attention to Form of Contour. Child Dev. 1975, 46, 224–228. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ruff, H.A.; Birch, H.G. Infant Visual Fixation: The Effect of Concentricity, Curvilinearity, and Number of Directions. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 1974, 17, 460–473. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Quinn, P.C.; Brown, C.R.; Streppa, M.L. Perceptual Organization of Complex Visual Configurations by Young Infants. Infant Behav. Dev. 1997, 20, 35–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hopkins, J.R.; Kagan, J.; Brachfeld, S.; Hans, S.; Linn, S. Infant Responsivity to Curvature. Child Dev. 1976, 47, 1166–1171. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jadva, V.; Hines, M.; Golombok, S. Infants’ Preferences for Toys, Colors, and Shapes: Sex Differences and Similarities. Arch. Sex. Behav. 2010, 39, 1261–1273. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Child, I.L. Observations on the Meaning of Some Measures of Esthetic Sensitivity. J. Psychol. 1964, 57, 49–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Eysenck, H.J. The General Factor in Aesthetic Judgements. Br. J. Psychol. Gen. Sect. 1940, 31, 94–102. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Eysenck, H.J. A New Measure of “good Taste” in Visual Art. Leonardo 1983, 16, 229–231. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Götz, K.O.; Lynn, R.; Borisy, A.R.; Eysenck, H.J. A New Visual Aesthetic Sensitivity Test: I. Construction and Psychometric Properties. Percept. Mot. Ski. 1979, 49, 795–802. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Marschallek, B.E.; Weiler, S.M.; Jörg, M.; Jacobsen, T. Make It Special! Negative Correlations between the Need for Uniqueness and Visual Aesthetic Sensitivity. Empir. Stud. Arts 2021, 39, 101–117. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Myszkowski, N. The First Glance Is the Weakest: “Tasteful” Individuals Are Slower to Judge Visual Art. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2019, 141, 188–195. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Duffy, R. An Analysis of Aesthetic Sensitivity and Creativity with Other Variables in Grades Four, Six, Eight, and Ten. J. Educ. Res. 1979, 73, 26–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Myszkowski, N. Aesthetic Sensitivity. In The Oxford Handbook of Empirical Aesthetics; Nadal, M., Vartanian, O., Eds.; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2020; pp. 1–21. ISBN 978-0-19-882435-0. [Google Scholar]
- Bairisal, S.; Kujur, A. Response Time Differences in the Aesthetic Judgment of Individuals on Beautiful and Ugly Images. Int. J. Arts Humanit. Soc. Sci. Stud. 2020, 5, 19–29. [Google Scholar]
- Myszkowski, N.; Çelik, P.; Storme, M. A Meta-Analysis of the Relationship between Intelligence and Visual “Taste” Measures. Psychol. Aesthet. Creat. Arts 2018, 12, 24–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Götz, K.O. VAST: Visual Aesthetic Sensitivity Test, 4th ed.; Concept Verlag: Dusseldorf, Germany, 1985. [Google Scholar]
- Chan, J.; Eysenck, H.J.; Götz, K.O. A New Visual Aesthetic Sensitivity Test III. Cross-Cultural Comparison between Hong Kong Children and Adults, and English and Japanese Samples. Percept. Mot. Ski. 1980, 50, 1325–1326. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Frois, J.P.; Eysenck, H. The Visual Aesthetic Sensitivity Test Applied to Portuguese Children and Fine Arts Students. Creat. Res. J. 1995, 8, 277–284. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mitrovic, A.; Hegelmaier, L.M.; Leder, H.; Pelowski, M. Does Beauty Capture the Eye, Even If It’s Not (Overtly) Adaptive? A Comparative Eye-Tracking Study of Spontaneous Attention and Visual Preference with VAST Abstract Art. Acta Psychol. 2020, 209, 103133. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Myszkowski, N.; Storme, M.; Zenasni, F.; Lubart, T. Is Visual Aesthetic Sensitivity Independent from Intelligence, Personality and Creativity? Personal. Individ. Differ. 2014, 59, 16–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Graves, M. Design Judgment Test; Psychological Corporation: New York, NY, USA, 1948. [Google Scholar]
- Meier, N.C. The Meier Art Tests. I. Art Judgment; Bureau of Educational Research and Service, University of Iowa: Iowa City, IA, USA, 1940. [Google Scholar]
- Stich, C. Development of Scales for Aesthetic Research. Ph.D. Thesis, Free University of Berlin, Berlin, Germany, 2005. [Google Scholar]
- Myszkowski, N.; Storme, M. Measuring “Good Taste” with the Visual Aesthetic Sensitivity Test-Revised (VAST-R). Personal. Individ. Differ. 2017, 117, 91–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Child, I.L. Aesthetic Judgment in Children. Society 1970, 7, 47–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bairisal, S.; Kumar, J. Design and Development of a New Instrument for Measuring Aesthetic Sensitivity. In Research into Design for a Connected World; Chakrabarti, A., Ed.; Smart Innovation, Systems and Technologies; Springer: Singapore, 2019; Volume 135, pp. 281–290. ISBN 9789811359767. [Google Scholar]
- Kelley, T.L. The Selection of Upper and Lower Groups for the Validation of Test Items. J. Educ. Psychol. 1939, 30, 17–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- R Core Team. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing; R Foundation for Statistical Computing: Vienna, Austria, 2018; Available online: http://www.r-project.org (accessed on 10 December 2023).
- Bates, D.; Mächler, M.; Bolker, B.; Walker, S. Fitting Linear Mixed-Effects Models Using Lme4. J. Stat. Softw. 2015, 67, 1–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kuznetsova, A.; Brockhoff, P.B.; Christensen, R.H. lmerTest Package: Tests in Linear Mixed Effects Models. J. Stat. Softw. 2017, 82, 1–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Aronoff, J.; Woike, B.A.; Hyman, L.M. Which Are the Stimuli in Facial Displays of Anger and Happiness? Configurational Bases of Emotion Recognition. Emot. Recognit. 1992, 62, 1050–1066. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Larson, C.L.; Aronoff, J.; Steuer, E.L. Simple Geometric Shapes Are Implicitly Associated with Affective Value. Motiv. Emot. 2012, 36, 404–413. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bar, M.; Neta, M. Visual Elements of Subjective Preference Modulate Amygdala Activation. Neuropsychologia 2007, 45, 2191–2200. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rocha, T.A.; Peixoto, F.; Jesus, S.N. Aesthetic Development in Children, Adolescents and Young Adults. Análise Psicológica 2020, 38, 1–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Parsons, M.; Johnston, M.; Durham, R. Developmental Stages in Children’s Aesthetic Responses. J. Aesthetic Educ. 1978, 12, 83–104. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wohlwill, J.F. The Gardner-Winner View of Children’s Visual-Artistic Development: Overview, Assessment, and Critique. Vis. Arts Res. 1985, 11, 1–22. [Google Scholar]
- Hardiman, G.W.; Zernich, T. Some Considerations of Piaget’s Cognitive-Structuralist Theory and Children’s Artistic Development. Stud. Art Educ. 1980, 21, 12–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Piaget, J. Piaget’s Theory. In Handbook of Child Psychology: Vol. 1. History, Theory, and Methods; Kessen, W., Ed.; Wiley: New York, NY, USA, 1983. [Google Scholar]
- Kawabata, H.; Zeki, S. Neural Correlates of Beauty. J. Neurophysiol. 2004, 91, 1699–1705. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ishizu, T.; Zeki, S. Toward A Brain-Based Theory of Beauty. PLoS ONE 2011, 6, e21852. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Grade | High Level of Visual Aesthetic Sensitivity Students | Low Level of Visual Aesthetic Sensitivity Students | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
n | Age | Score | n | Age | Score | |
4 | 46 | 10.2 (0.5) | 37.72 (1.97) | 46 | 10.2 (0.4) | 28.00 (1.56) |
6 | 50 | 12.1 (0.4) | 38.34 (2.07) | 50 | 12.2 (0.5) | 28.04 (2.99) |
8 | 44 | 14.0 (0.5) | 38.73 (1.74) | 44 | 14.2 (0.4) | 27.82 (2.75) |
Curved Contour | Sharp-Angled Contour | |||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
High Level of Visual Aesthetic Sensitivity | Low Level of Visual Aesthetic Sensitivity | High Level of Visual Aesthetic Sensitivity | Low Level of Visual Aesthetic Sensitivity | |||||||||
Grade 4 | Grade 6 | Grade 8 | Grade 4 | Grade 6 | Grade 8 | Grade 4 | Grade 6 | Grade 8 | Grade 4 | Grade 6 | Grade 8 | |
preference | 3.90 (1.31) | 3.93 (1.50) | 3.98 (1.49) | 3.94 (1.43) | 3.67 (1.30) | 3.94 (1.37) | 3.18 (1.37) | 2.59 (1.07) | 2.78 (1.35) | 3.50 (1.42) | 3.09 (1.40) | 3.53 (1.39) |
beauty | 3.77 (1.30) | 3.99 (1.45) | 3.97 (1.58) | 3.89 (1.39) | 3.70 (1.29) | 3.94 (1.35) | 3.10 (1.43) | 2.60 (1.08) | 2.81 (1.44) | 3.44 (1.39) | 3.03 (1.35) | 3.54 (1.36) |
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2024 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Zhang, J.; Lu, Z.; Wang, Y.; Bai, X. The Aesthetic Developmental Characteristics of Contour Features in Children and Adolescents with High- and Low- Level Visual Aesthetic Sensitivity across Grade Levels. Behav. Sci. 2024, 14, 416. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs14050416
Zhang J, Lu Z, Wang Y, Bai X. The Aesthetic Developmental Characteristics of Contour Features in Children and Adolescents with High- and Low- Level Visual Aesthetic Sensitivity across Grade Levels. Behavioral Sciences. 2024; 14(5):416. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs14050416
Chicago/Turabian StyleZhang, Ju, Zijia Lu, Yongsheng Wang, and Xuejun Bai. 2024. "The Aesthetic Developmental Characteristics of Contour Features in Children and Adolescents with High- and Low- Level Visual Aesthetic Sensitivity across Grade Levels" Behavioral Sciences 14, no. 5: 416. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs14050416
APA StyleZhang, J., Lu, Z., Wang, Y., & Bai, X. (2024). The Aesthetic Developmental Characteristics of Contour Features in Children and Adolescents with High- and Low- Level Visual Aesthetic Sensitivity across Grade Levels. Behavioral Sciences, 14(5), 416. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs14050416