Time Matters: Time Perspectives Predict Intertemporal Prosocial Preferences
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Participants and Measurement
2.2. Intertemporal Prosocial Discounting Task
2.3. Procedure
2.4. Discounting Data Analysis
3. Results
3.1. Time Delay Reduces Generosity
3.2. Different Time Perspectives and Generosity
3.3. Time Perspective Predicts Intertemporal Prosocial Preferences
4. Discussion
5. Conclusions and Policy Implications
Supplementary Materials
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Conflicts of Interest
Abbreviations
PITP | Present Impulsive Time Perspective |
FTP | Future Time Perspective |
ZTPI-C | the Chinese version of the Zimbardo Time Perspective Inventory |
PTBM | the Perceived-Time–Based Model |
References
- Amasino, D.R.; Sullivan, N.J.; Kranton, R.E.; Huettel, S.A. Amount and time exert independent influences on intertemporal choice. Nat. Hum. Behav. 2019, 3, 383–392. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- O’Donoghue, T.; Rabin, M. Present Bias: Lessons Learned and to Be Learned. Am. Econ. Rev. 2015, 105, 273–279. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Bisin, A.; Hyndman, K. Present-bias, procrastination and deadlines in a field experiment. Games Econ. Behav. 2020, 119, 339–357. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wang, Y.; Sloan, F.A. Present bias and health. J. Risk Uncertain. 2018, 57, 177–198. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Xiao, J.J.; Porto, N. Present bias and financial behavior. Financ. Plan. Rev. 2019, 2, e1048. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Andreoni, J.; Serra-Garcia, M. Time inconsistent charitable giving. J. Public Econ. 2021, 198, 104391. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chopra, F.; Eisenhauer, P.; Falk, A.; Graeber, T. Intertemporal Altruism. SSRN 3771742. 2021. Available online: http://dx.doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.3771742 (accessed on 18 June 2023).
- Keough, K.A.; Zimbardo, P.G.; Boyd, J.N. Who’s Smoking, Drinking, and Using Drugs? Time Perspective as a Predictor of Substance Use. Basic Appl. Soc. Psychol. 1999, 21, 149–164. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zimbardo, P.; Boyd, J. The Time Paradox: The New Psychology of Time That Will Change Your Life; Free Press: New York, NY, USA, 2008; p. xix, 378. [Google Scholar]
- Zimbardo, P.G.; Boyd, J.N. Putting time in perspective: A valid, reliable individual-differences metric. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1999, 77, 1271–1288. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zimbardo, P.G.; Keough, K.A.; Boyd, J.N. Present time perspective as a predictor of risky driving. Personal. Individ. Differ. 1997, 23, 1007–1023. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Roberts, W.A. Are animals stuck in time? Psychol. Bull. 2002, 128, 473–489. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Suddendorf, T.; Corballis, M.C. The evolution of foresight: What is mental time travel, and is it unique to humans? Behav. Brain Sci. 2007, 30, 299–313. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Baumeister, R.F.; Vohs, K.D.; Oettingen, G. Pragmatic Prospection: How and Why People Think about the Future. Rev. Gen. Psychol. 2016, 20, 3–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Baumeister, R.F.; Maranges, H.M.; Sjåstad, H. Consciousness of the future as a matrix of maybe: Pragmatic prospection and the simulation of alternative possibilities. Psychol. Conscious. Theory Res. Pract. 2018, 5, 223–238. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Sjåstad, H. Short-sighted greed? Focusing on the future promotes reputation-based generosity. Judgm. Decis. Mak. 2019, 14, 199–213. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Takahashi, T. Loss of Self-Control in Intertemporal Choice May Be Attributable to Logarithmic Time-Perception. In Behavioral Economics of Preferences, Choices, and Happiness; Ikeda, S., Kato, H.K., Ohtake, F., Tsutsui, Y., Eds.; Springer: Tokyo, Japan, 2016; pp. 117–122. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Zauberman, G.; Kim, B.K.; Malkoc, S.A.; Bettman, J.R. Discounting Time and Time Discounting: Subjective Time Perception and Intertemporal Preferences. J. Mark. Res. 2009, 46, 543–556. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kim, B.K.; Zauberman, G. Psychological time and intertemporal preference. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 2019, 26, 90–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bradford, W.D.; Dolan, P.; Galizzi, M.M. Looking ahead: Subjective time perception and individual discounting. J. Risk Uncertain. 2019, 58, 43–69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Apaydın, N.; Üstün, S.; Kale, E.H.; Çelikağ, İ.; Özgüven, H.D.; Baskak, B.; Çiçek, M. Neural Mechanisms Underlying Time Perception and Reward Anticipation. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2018, 12, 115. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Han, R.; Takahashi, T. Psychophysics of time perception and valuation in temporal discounting of gain and loss. Phys. A Stat. Mech. Its Appl. 2012, 391, 6568–6576. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Stolarski, M.; Fieulaine, N.; Van Beek, W. (Eds.) Time Perspective Theory; Review, Research and Application: Essays in Honor of Philip G. Zimbardo; Springer International Publishing/Springer Nature: Cham, Switzerland, 2015; p. xxvi, 551. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lu, T.; Liang, D.; Hong, M.; Sun, J. Regret Now, Compensate It Later: The Benefits of Experienced Regret on Future Altruism. Front. Psychol. 2022, 13, 840809. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wang, Y.; Wang, X.; Yang, X.; Yuan, F.; Li, Y. The influence of temporal focus on individual intertemporal decision-making in life history strategy framework. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2023, 211, 112250. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kim, B.K.; Zauberman, G. Perception of anticipatory time in temporal discounting. J. Neurosci. Psychol. Econ. 2009, 2, 91–101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Li, X.; Wang, C.; Lyu, H.; Worrell, F.C.; Mello, Z.R. Psychometric properties of the Chinese version of the Zimbardo Time Perspective Inventory. Curr. Psychol. 2022, 42, 13547–13559. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bowling, A.; Hankins, M.; Windle, G.; Bilotta, C.; Grant, R. A short measure of quality of life in older age: The performance of the brief Older People’s Quality of Life questionnaire (OPQOL-brief). Arch. Gerontol. Geriatr. 2013, 56, 181–187. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Sijtsma, K.; Molenaar, I.W. Introduction to Nonparametric Item Response Theory; Sage: Thousand Oaks, CA, USA, 2002; Volume 5. [Google Scholar]
- van der Ark, L.A. Mokken Scale Analysis in R. J. Stat. Softw. 2007, 20, 1–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Meijer, R.R.; Baneke, J.J. Analyzing psychopathology items: A case for nonparametric item response theory modeling. Psychol. Methods 2004, 9, 354–368. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Visser-Bochane, M.I.; Reijneveld, S.A.; Krijnen, W.P.; van der Schans, C.P.; Luinge, M.R. Identifying Milestones in Language Development for Young Children Ages 1 to 6 Years. Acad. Pediatr. 2020, 20, 421–429. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Stochl, J.; Jones, P.B.; Croudace, T.J. Mokken scale analysis of mental health and well-being questionnaire item responses: A non-parametric IRT method in empirical research for applied health researchers. BMC Med. Res. Methodol. 2012, 12, 74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Jin, L.; Xiao, Y.; Yi-Beng, Z. The Influence of Time Perspective Trait on Food Choice Preference: The Moderating Role of Construal Level on Implicit Preference. J. Psychol. Sci. 2017, 40, 1161–1167. [Google Scholar]
- Faul, F.; Erdfelder, E.; Buchner, A.; Lang, A.G. Statistical power analyses using G*Power 3.1: Tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behav. Res. Methods 2009, 41, 1149–1160. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Vekaria, K.M.; Brethel-Haurwitz, K.M.; Cardinale, E.M.; Stoycos, S.A.; Marsh, A.A. Social discounting and distance perceptions in costly altruism. Nat. Hum. Behav. 2017, 1, 0100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ou, J.; Wu, Y.; Hu, Y.; Gao, X.; Li, H.; Tobler, P.N. Testosterone reduces generosity through cortical and subcortical mechanisms. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2021, 118, e2021745118. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- de Leeuw, J.R. jsPsych: A JavaScript library for creating behavioral experiments in a Web browser. Behav. Res. Methods 2015, 47, 1–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Schram, A. Artificiality: The tension between internal and external validity in economic experiments. J. Econ. Methodol. 2005, 12, 225–237. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Trope, Y.; Liberman, N. Temporal construal. Psychol. Rev. 2003, 110, 403–421. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Myerson, J.; Green, L.; Warusawitharana, M. Area under the Curve as a Measure of Discounting. J. Exp. Anal. Behav. 2001, 76, 235–243. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Bayat, S.; Naglie, G.; Rapoport, M.J.; Stasiulis, E.; Widener, M.J.; Mihailidis, A. A GPS-Based Framework for Understanding Outdoor Mobility Patterns of Older Adults with Dementia: An Exploratory Study. Gerontology 2021, 68, 106–120. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gorissen, B.; de Bruin, A.; Miranda-Bedate, A.; Korthagen, N.; Wolschrijn, C.; de Vries, T.J.; van Weeren, R.; Tryfonidou, M.A. Hypoxia negatively affects senescence in osteoclasts and delays osteoclastogenesis. J. Cell. Physiol. 2019, 234, 414–426. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Ogawa, K.; Ida, T. Investigating Donating Behavior Using Hypothetical Dictator Game Experiments. Rev. Soc. Econ. 2015, 73, 176–195. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sobol-Kwapinska, M.; Jankowski, T. Positive Time: Balanced Time Perspective and Positive Orientation. J. Happiness Stud. 2016, 17, 1511–1528. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Zaleski, Z.; Przepiórka, A. Goals Need Time Perspective to Be Achieved. In Time Perspective Theory; Review, Research and Application: Essays in Honor of Philip G. Zimbardo; Stolarski, M., Fieulaine, N., van Beek, W., Eds.; Springer International Publishing: Cham, Switzerland, 2015; pp. 323–335. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Taylor, J.; Wilson, J.C. Failing time after time: Time perspective, procrastination, and cognitive reappraisal in goal failure. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 2016, 46, 557–564. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Liu, H.Z.; Lyu, X.K.; Wei, Z.H.; Mo, W.L.; Luo, J.R.; Su, X.Y. Exploiting the dynamics of eye gaze to bias intertemporal choice. J. Behav. Decis. Mak. 2021, 34, 419–431. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- BI Cuihua, Q.H. The role of time perception in intertemporal decision-making: New exploration of time decision-making model. Adv. Psychol. Sci. 2022, 30, 1106–1118. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rapert, M.I.; Thyroff, A.; Grace, S.C. The generous consumer: Interpersonal generosity and pro-social dispositions as antecedents to cause-related purchase intentions. J. Bus. Res. 2021, 132, 838–847. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pornpattananangkul, N.; Chowdhury, A.; Feng, L.; Yu, R. Social Discounting in the Elderly: Senior Citizens are Good Samaritans to Strangers. J. Gerontol. Ser. B 2017, 74, 52–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Bleidorn, W.; Hill, P.L.; Back, M.D.; Denissen, J.J.A.; Hennecke, M.; Hopwood, C.J.; Jokela, M.; Kandler, C.; Lucas, R.E.; Luhmann, M.; et al. The policy relevance of personality traits. Am. Psychol. 2019, 74, 1056–1067. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ma, D.S.; Correll, J.; Wittenbrink, B. The chicago face database: A free stimulus set of faces and norming data. Behav. Res. Methods 2015, 47, 1122–1135. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Christov-Moore, L.; Iacoboni, M. Self-other resonance, its control and prosocial inclinations: Brain-behavior relationships. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2016, 37, 1544–1558. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
Group 1 (N = 38) | Group 2 (N = 39) | |
---|---|---|
Present Impulsive | 4.17 | 2.87 |
Future | 2.58 | 4.43 |
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2023 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Lu, T.; Liang, D.; Hong, M. Time Matters: Time Perspectives Predict Intertemporal Prosocial Preferences. Behav. Sci. 2023, 13, 590. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs13070590
Lu T, Liang D, Hong M. Time Matters: Time Perspectives Predict Intertemporal Prosocial Preferences. Behavioral Sciences. 2023; 13(7):590. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs13070590
Chicago/Turabian StyleLu, Teng, Dapeng Liang, and Mei Hong. 2023. "Time Matters: Time Perspectives Predict Intertemporal Prosocial Preferences" Behavioral Sciences 13, no. 7: 590. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs13070590
APA StyleLu, T., Liang, D., & Hong, M. (2023). Time Matters: Time Perspectives Predict Intertemporal Prosocial Preferences. Behavioral Sciences, 13(7), 590. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs13070590