Next Article in Journal
Mental Health of University Students When Returning to Face-to-Face Classes: A Cross-Sectional Study
Previous Article in Journal
The Importance of Measuring Mental Wellbeing in the Context of Psychological Distress: Using a Theoretical Framework to Test the Dual-Continua Model of Mental Health
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Review

Gambling and Aging: An Overview of a Risky Behavior

1
Cognition Lanque Langage Ergonomie, Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique, Université Tou-louse-II-Jean-Jaurès, CEDEX 09, 31058 Toulouse, France
2
Laboratoire de Psychopathologie et Processus de Santé, Université Paris Cité, 92100 Boulogne-Billancourt, France
3
Centre Québécois d’Excellence Pour le Traitement du Jeu, Université Laval, Quebec, QC G1V 0A6, Canada
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Behav. Sci. 2023, 13(6), 437; https://doi.org/10.3390/bs13060437
Submission received: 24 April 2023 / Revised: 16 May 2023 / Accepted: 18 May 2023 / Published: 23 May 2023

Abstract

:
Gambling is a field of study that has grown since the 2000s. Much research has focused on adolescents and youth as a vulnerable population. The rate of aging gamblers is increasing; however, evidence-based knowledge of this population is still too sparse. After introducing the issue (1), this article provides a narrative review of older adults’ gambling through three sections: (2) older adult gamblers (age, characteristics, and motivations), (3) gambling as a risky decision-making situation, and (4) gambling disorder related to older adults. By drawing on the existing literature from a problematization perspective, this type of review can highlight complex and original research topics and provoke thought and controversy to generate avenues for future research. This narrative review provides an overview of the existing literature on gambling among older adults and offers perspectives on how aging can affect decision-making and thus gambling for this population. Older adults are a specific population, not only in terms of the consequences of gambling disorders but also in terms of the motivations and cognitions underlying gambling behaviors. Studies on behavioral science focusing on decision-making in older adults could help in the development of public policy in terms of targeted prevention.

1. Introduction

Gambling refers to any operation offered to the public, under any name whatsoever, in order to raise the hope of a gain which is due, even partially, to chance, and for which a financial sacrifice is required from the participants [1]. Gambling is therefore an activity that relies on chance and in which someone risks money in order to win. This risky activity is increasingly part of the daily lives of people from many cultures [2,3,4] and is of concern to the World Health Organization itself [5]. From North America to Australia, Europe, Africa, and Asia, more than one in two individuals on average report having gambled in the past year [6,7,8,9,10]. Among older adults from different countries, those with a non-Caucasian cultural identity range from 26.6 to 56.2 percent to gamble [11], with participation across all cultures ranging up to 85.6 percent [12]. In France, 49.3% of 55–64 year olds and 37.6% of 65–75 year olds gambled in 2019 [7]. Older adults have positive attitudes towards gambling, which is seen as an ordinary and safe form of entertainment [11,13]. While 79.5% of people over 60 say they gamble for entertainment, 62.7% also say they gamble to win money [14]. Additionally, it is when gambling is perceived as a reward in itself that gambling is risky [15].
How can the gambling attractiveness among older adults be explained? The occupational, emotional, financial, and health-related changes experienced by this population mean that they have a lot of free time available and a different relationship with money, leisure, and the future, making older adults a prime clientele for gambling promoters [15]. In a Silver Economy logic [16], older adults are the target of many marketing campaigns [17,18], which present gambling offers as the perfect answer to seniors’ needs (organized service road to casinos, first bet offered, and promise of socialization), as well as a means to fight isolation [19]. Isolation is the common theme in the three trajectories that can lead older adults from recreational to gambling disorder: a habit pathway associated with gambling habituation, a dormant pathway marked by pre-existing excessive behaviors or impulsivity, and a grief pathway associated with unresolved losses [20]. Increased availability and exposure to gambling products and establishments can send older adults down the habit pathway. Since increased exposure leads to gambling disorder, rates of gambling disorder among older adults are likely to increase [21]. Numerous studies have shown an increase in participation rates and rates of gambling-related disorders among older adults (for a review, [22]). These increases are expected to become even more pronounced in the coming years simply because of the population aging [23].
Although the prevalence rates of gambling disorder are lower among older adults than among their younger counterparts, it is a problem of concern among this population [11,24], particularly because of the severity of the consequences, such as financial loss, debt accumulation, and illegal behaviors (forging, fraud, theft, or embezzlement to finance gambling) directly related to gambling [23]. Fixed incomes, in particular, make older adults an extremely vulnerable group [24]. However, the issue of aging in gambling is still too rarely investigated. Longitudinal studies and collaborative research combining epidemiology, sociology, psychology, and behavioral analysis are lacking, and the current work is mostly descriptive and exploratory [11]. The literature also lacks empirical data [12,25]. While there are 13,423 publications archived on Psychinfo from 1973 to 2022 with the keyword “gambling” alone, the keywords “gambling and aging” or “gambling and elderly” only return 258 and 86 publications, respectively. If we add the keyword “experiment”, the number of publications drops to eight and zero, respectively. Cross-linking the literature on older adults’ gambling with that on decision-making in risky situations offers, however, some avenues for behavioral research.

2. Older Adult Gamblers: Age, Characteristics, and Motivations to Gamble

2.1. Older Adult Gamblers’ Age

Although the World Health Organization set the age of entry into old age at between 60 and 65, this institution recognizes that old age must be defined by new roles and various factors other than biological age [26]. Even though the term “senior”, which appeared in the early 1990s, refers to those over 55 years of age or even over 45 years of age in the workplace [27], some people have proposed the probability of surviving 10 years as an indicator of entry into old age, thus advancing the age of old age in line with the increase in demographic aging [28]. The age of old age is therefore a matter of debate. There is also no consensus in the gambling literature, with the lower bound being, depending on the study, 50 years (e.g., [29,30]), 55 years (e.g., [2,12,17,31,32,33,34,35,36,37]), 56 years (e.g., [38]), 60 years (e.g., [14,39,40]), 65 years (e.g., [41,42,43]), or sometimes 70 years (e.g., [44]).
The chosen limit is only rarely justified. For Asian studies, the reason is that retirement is at age 55. Granero et al. [30] justified the lower limit of 50 years because of the large variations in the literature. Pilver et al. [35] defined the limit of 55 for comparability with previous work. In Connecticut, Petry [45] found that older female gamblers began regular gambling at age 55, whereas regular gambling among older male gamblers began before age 30. The large prevalence survey on substance use and gambling among seniors [46] in New Brunswick, Canada, set the cut-off at 55. Lastly, age 55 was identified as a reasonable threshold for the onset of age-related impairment in decision-making [47].

2.2. Older Adult Gamblers’ Characteristics

The question of the profile of the older gambler does not lead to any definitive typical conclusion. There are variations by gender and socioeconomic level in the general population, but there is no clear pattern among older adults [24]. Some studies suggest that the prevalence of gambling disorder among older adults is higher among men than women [48,49], but others indicate that it is similar or even higher among women [50,51,52,53]. At the socio-economic level, while education [49] and income [54] have been identified by some studies as risk factors, others have found no link [48]. Regarding types of gambling, there are games of pure chance, non-strategic, such as lotto or casino games, and games involving a degree of skill such as poker, sports betting, or horseracing [55]. Older adults prefer non-strategic games over strategic games [22,56], and the prevalence of gambling disorder is greater in casino games such as slot machines than in lotteries [53]. The theoretical model developed by Tira et al. [20] proposes three trajectories to gambling disorder. The habit pathway is characterized by repeated exposure to a gambling practice and conditions (“beginner’s luck” and big wins) that foster the entrenchment of erroneous beliefs (gambler’s fallacy, superstitions, and near-miss), which lead to an escalation of gambling activity. From a reason to start gambling, breaking the boredom becomes an excuse to gamble more. The dormant pathway relates to predispositions to compulsive behaviors. This pre-existing vulnerability, along with strong co-morbidities (tobacco and alcohol dependence) and low levels of self-control, leads to significant risk-taking for the sheer pleasure it provides. In contrast to this presumed biological or genetic predisposition of the dormant pathway, the occurrence of certain negative life events (essentially losses of various kinds and intensities) characterize the grief pathway. The emotional vulnerability induced by losses (of role, of loved ones, of income, or of physical abilities) gives rise to a need to gamble and to respond to the incentives linked to gambling, in order to escape negative emotions such as loneliness, despair, depression, or anger [20]. For example, older adults are likely to increase their gambling activity in the hopes of offsetting the loss of income [53]. Motivation to win money, combined with excitement seeking, predicts gambling disorder, especially among slot machine gamblers over 60 [53].

2.3. Older Adults’ Motivations for Gambling: Risk Factors or Not?

Of the studies that have examined gambling in aging people, the largest number have focused on motivations, with no universal consensus. While New Zealand older adults gamble to win money more frequently than younger gamblers [52], this is clearly not a pronounced motivating factor among British older adults [21]. For the latter population, gambling is an escape from the psychological (loneliness or grief) and physical (pain and decline) stress and discomfort associated with aging. The high accessibility and availability of gambling environments compared to other leisure activities make it a warm and feel-good activity [21]. The most common reasons reported by casino gamblers over 65 are to relax (68.1% of respondents), relieve boredom (30%), pass the time (42.7%), and go away for the day (43.7%) [57]. Of the five dimensions of motivation to gamble for older Americans (i.e., thrill of winning, escape, socialization, fun, and curiosity), the primary motivation is fun and pleasure [58]. From the perspective of self-determination theory [59,60], the main motivations of casino gamblers over the age of 60 refer to intrinsic motivations (i.e., the activity is a reward in itself), such as entertainment and pleasure. Two-thirds of respondents, however, expressed extrinsic motivations (i.e., the activity has an instrumental value or a utilitarian purpose), such as financial gain. More self-determined reasons for gambling (intrinsic motivations) are associated with greater participation in gambling, and less self-determined reasons (extrinsic motivations) are associated with less participation in gambling [61]. Beyond simple participation, for both older adults (Mage = 74.59) and younger adults (Mage = 22.61), the severity of gambling disorder is more associated with a tension-relief motivation than with money-making and thrill-seeking [52]. Motivational factors related to psychological and physical stress reduction [21] are similar, in the general population, to the coping motivations, via negative reinforcement, of the “behaviorally conditioned gambler” [62]. This subgroup of problem gamblers has no premorbid characteristics but has comorbidities that are consequences, not causes, of repeated problem gambling behaviors. These gamblers fluctuate between regular and problem gambling due to the effects of conditioning, increased vulnerability to erroneous cognitions, errors in judgment, and poor decision-making. With the least severe difficulties of all the problem gamblers, this subgroup is motivated to enter treatment and can successfully return to controlled levels of gambling after treatment [62].
Another motivation for older adults’ gambling is a desire to exercise one’s mind [12]. Gambling can, indeed, satisfy unmet psychological needs for not only emotional but also cognitive stimulation [21]. One of the reasons for the popularity of gambling among older adults is that it provides an opportunity for cognitive stimulation [63,64,65], which is thought to improve cognitive functioning and thus counteract the well-documented decline in cognitive performance associated with aging [47,66,67,68,69]. Recreational gambling among older adults is associated with positive outcomes in terms of cognitive stimulation, socialization, health, and well-being [42,70,71]. In a leisure state, gambling increases activity, develops socialization processes, and stimulates cognitive functions, thus favoring better overall health [72]. Recreational gamblers over 65 report higher levels of “good to excellent” general health than non-gamblers [42]. Gamblers over the age of 60 develop cognitive and behavioral strategies to prevent them from falling into gambling disorder [71] and to keep their gambling under control. On the cognitive level, this involves keeping in mind the real dangers of gambling, being aware that the odds are stacked against one, and never considering oneself lucky; on the behavioral level, it involves regulating one’s practice through concrete actions such as only taking a predefined amount of money, not gambling alone, stopping and not reinvesting one’s winnings, and optimizing one’s gambling time by betting small sums. Thus, recreational gambling has a number of beneficial effects. On the other hand, problem-gambling behaviors have been associated with high levels of anxiety and loneliness [73,74]. Older adults may perceive gambling as a way to reduce negative mood states [43]. The need for protection and assertiveness contributes to sustained gambling [75]. When the primary motivation for gambling is to escape anxiety and loneliness, the likelihood of experiencing gambling-related harms is greater [23,37,43].

3. Gambling: A Risky Decision-Making Situation in Which Older Adults Are Likely to Be Vulnerable Due to Their Specificities

3.1. Decision-Making, Risk-Taking, and Gambling

Gambling is an activity whose outcome is based mainly or entirely on chance and involves an irreversible prior stake of money or something of value [76]. As defined by Rogers [77], gambling is a situation in which the gambler decides to take the risk of losing, with the hope of winning according to some probabilities. The cognitive approach distinguishes three types of decision-making situations [78]: under certainty (decisions for which we know and are absolutely certain of the outcome), under uncertainty (decisions for which we do not know the probability of the outcome), or at risk (decisions for which the outcomes are known as well as the probabilities of these outcomes). A risky decision is, for example, a situation in which there is a 30% chance of doubling one’s stake [79]. In such a situation, we know the possible outcome of the stake (doubling it) and the probability of this outcome (about one chance in three). However, although we know the probability of this consequence, we cannot be certain of the occurrence of the desired outcome (doubling the bet), so there is still some uncertainty. The neuroscientific approach distinguishes between decision making under ambiguity (the probability of obtaining a specific outcome is unknown or is a matter of chance, the possible choice—safe versus risky—does not differ in terms of the value of the reward and there is no greater risk in making either choice) and decision making under risk (the probability of each outcome is known and the participant has to decide between a safe choice—associated with a high probability of obtaining a relatively small reward in value—and a risky choice—associated with a low probability of obtaining a significantly larger reward in value). From an ecological perspective, the gambling situation involves making a decision under both uncertainty (Kahneman and Tversky [80,81] have shown that humans have great difficulty processing probabilities) and risk (known or potentially known outcomes and probabilities).
Although widely investigated in the decision-making literature developed from economic theories [82], risk-taking has been studied in only a few gambling studies. Decision-making is a complex cognitive process aiming at the selection of a type of action among different alternatives in order to lead to supposedly rational decisions and to the formation of judgments [83]. Indeed, the decision-making process involves “specifying possible courses of action, collecting information about the alternatives, identifying likely future events and other circumstances relevant to the decision, and considering the possible outcomes contingent on the chosen action and the prevailing circumstances” [84] (p. 297). Identified as early as the 1950s, the discrepancies between the normative prescriptions of economic theory (assuming rationality in decision-making) and real human behavior are at the origin of the heuristics and biases trend and Kahneman and Tversky’s studies [83]. These authors’ theory postulates that the reason why the treatment of information about probabilities and consequences escapes normative economic theory is because it is governed by perceptual and attentional mechanisms. It provides a richer and more ecological behavioral model to better account for the complexity of human decision-making in real life than traditional economic theory [82].
More precisely, risk decision-making occurs when individuals make choices whose consequences depend on the probability of occurrence of certain events [85]. The prospect theory [86] describes the mental process of decision in two phases; it assumes that risk-taking behaviors vary according to the definition of the risky situation. The first phase, called editing, where the individual subjectively represents the perspectives offered in a decision problem, depends on the type of information processing operated. Then, comes a phase of evaluation of the previously edited perspectives, which allows a choice to be made based on the highest subjective utility. The calculation of this subjective utility integrates two functions [85]: a function of the subjective value relative to a reference point and a function of probability weights relative to the coding of perspectives in a domain and the type of probability (see Table 1).
Figure 1 illustrates how loss-zone functions reflect both risk-taking behavior and loss aversion. Because the annoyance of losing a certain amount of money is greater than the pleasure of winning the same amount [86], the subjective value function in the loss domain (on the left of the vertical axis) is not symmetrical to that in the gain domain (on the right), relative to a reference point (represented by the horizontal axis). In fact, the gains curve rises less quickly than the losses curve, which means that the gains become less valuable than the losses as one moves away from the reference point.
With reference to prospect theory, the inclusion of the gambler’s state of wealth at the time of betting as a reference point for the definition of the gambling situation makes it possible to calculate the risk-taking of participants [87]. This is especially interesting because unlike the research on gambling, the research on decision-making has massively examined the effects of age (for a multi-disciplinary review about aging, see [88]). Decision-making changes with aging [82]. Older adults tend to make decisions that are less advantageous compared to younger adults [47,68].

3.2. Older Adults’ Risk-Taking and Decision-Making

Mather et al. [89] studied the risk-taking of adults aged 55–89 with respect to the certainty effect (see Table 2) defined by Kahneman and Tversky [86] whereby, in addition to the desirability of the outcome (gains being desirable and losses undesirable), individuals perceive certain outcomes (such as a strictly 0% or 100% chance of the outcome occurring), as different from and more influential in decision-making than uncertain outcomes (all other probabilities between 0 and 100%).
The certainty effect thus predicts risk aversion when it comes to gains and risk seeking when it comes to losses. Older adults are more sensitive than younger ones to this certainty effect [89]. Older participants take more risks than younger ones when it comes to potential losses but also fewer risks than younger ones when it comes to potential gains [90,91]. It thus appears that attitudes towards risk change with aging. Moreover, the decisions of participants aged 65 to 90 years lead to the lowest monetary results: 39% less earnings than young people aged 21 to 25 years and 37% less earnings than adults aged 30 to 50 years [91]. Finally, despite a high IQ (above 110) guaranteeing appropriate calculation and reasoning faculties and a very satisfactory Mini Mental State Examination (MMSE) score (M = 29.03/30, SD = 0.9), attesting to the good mental capacities of the participants, the majority of the elderly showed striking and costly inconsistencies in their choice behaviors between the different monetary proposals [91]. The oldest adults thus show an impairment of decision-making despite their intellectual faculties. The normal process of cognitive decline leads, in certain situations, to deficits in decision-making after age 60 [66]. Some authors consider these deficits to suggest the possibility that some apparently normal older people show disproportionate aging of the ventromedial prefrontal cortices [68], but there is no clear, consensus explanation for the impairment of decision-making with age. The fact is that adults between the ages of 56 and 85 show a poorer performance on the Iowa Gambling Task (IGT, decision-making task under ambiguity where the participant must draw cards from four piles and maximize his or her winnings) than younger adults [47]. These impairments in decision-making also form the basis of gambling disorder.

3.3. From Decision-Making to Gambling Disorder

Gambling disorder is considered a mental disorder that impairs decision-making in risky situations [92]. Looking at risk-taking from the gambling disorder point of view, it reveals that loss aversion is unevenly distributed among gamblers with the disorder. The 57 subjects (mean age 34.1 years) in Takeuchi et al. [92] were asked to choose between two options, each of which offered a 50% chance of a certain alternative occurring versus a 50% chance of the other (see Table 3).
The distributions of loss aversion were significantly different: the control group (n = 26) showed a relatively homogeneous level of loss aversion (12 subjects with low aversion, 7 with medium aversion, and 7 with high aversion), whereas the gamblers with a gambling disorder (n = 31) showed heterogeneous loss aversion (19 subjects with low aversion, 1 with medium aversion, and 11 with high aversion). This suggest that gambling disorder is thus not be a uniform disorder but a heterogeneous one, with a marked and polarized difference in attitudes to risk. Combined with their increased participation in JHAs [93], the greater loss aversion among older adults [89] may be an additional factor in the growth in gambling disorder prevalence expected in this population as a result of demographic aging [30]. Although some adaptive processes, such as the use of an intuitive thinking system, ensure that older adults make satisfactory decisions in some contexts [94], the specific context of gambling may work against their efficiency. While intuitive decision making saves cognitive effort, the associated heuristics may favor systematic biases [95]. In a gambling situation, the use of heuristics is a harmful bias, leading to irrational decision-making and risk-taking where emotion plays a definite role [96]. The age effect variations depending on heuristic types also appear to rely on factors beyond cognitive aging, such as motivation and emotion [88]. When making a decision, older adults focus more than younger ones on emotional content and sometimes even on specifically positive content [82].

4. Gambling Disorder: How Is It Related to Older Adults through Control?

Additionally known as excessive gambling, problem gambling, gambling addiction, or pathological gambling, gambling disorder is a DSM-5 term referring to a chronic and progressive illness characterized by a preoccupation with gambling and a need to increase the value and frequency of bets to achieve a desired level of excitement [97]. Gambling disorder include but are not limited to compulsive gambling and refer to all gambling behaviors that may disrupt or interfere with family, personal, or professional activities [98]. The concept of use disorder bases the diagnosis on the use mode of an object (substance or not). The use of the object, which is a source of pleasure, is renewed despite its harmful repercussions in various areas of daily life. This puts a strong hypothesis of the cause of disorders linked to use, which is located at the level of a disorder of the control system [99]. Impaired activity in the prefrontal cortex has been identified in the decreased cognitive control over gambling urges [100]. The role of the prefrontal cortex in the reward system is thought to be regulated by dopaminergic transmission, which is dysfunctional in individuals with gambling disorder [101]. The hypothesis of an alteration in dopaminergic pathways has also been put forward for behavioral addictions (gambling, compulsive purchases, sexuality, and eating) linked to Parkinson’s disease treatments [102]. According to Guillou Landreat and colleagues [53], this should be put in perspective with the altered regulatory relationship between dopamine and prefrontal reward-related activity detected in healthy older adults [103]. These alterations may reduce older adults’ ability to control their gambling activity. Beyond brain mechanisms, this type of control is also a matter of an individual’s belief about whether or not they can resist an opportunity to gamble in a given situation [104], with the object of control being the behavior. This belief refers to the self-efficacy notion, defined as a system of beliefs about one’s own self-efficacy and one’s ability to organize and execute the actions required to produce given results, which influences performance through its motivational function [105]. Since aging is accompanied by losses that reduce control over the environment, situations of deprivation of a sense of control are assumed to increase with age. They are thus likely to develop in older people a belief in learned helplessness, defined as a generalized abandonment behavior resulting from the experience of uncontrollable aversive events [106]. Maintaining a sense of self-efficacy despite diminished abilities is beneficial adaptive functioning for older adults [107]. Individual performance is better predicted by beliefs about abilities than by actual abilities [108]. The illusion of control may thus make it possible to limit the feeling of acquired powerlessness, caused by repeated confrontations with situations of deprivation of the sense of control [109] and may thus generate benefits for older people [110]. While control (even illusory) over gambling behavior (see, e.g., Casey et al. [104] for work on self-efficacy), guarantees risk-free practice, the illusion of control over the game itself, as a tendency to overestimate the weight of one’s actions in situations determined, in reality, by chance (a factor beyond one’s control), is, on the contrary, a well-known risk factor [111,112]. We are therefore faced with a paradox where, in order to “gamble responsibly”, the gambler is encouraged to show control, but only over his or her behavior, while remaining aware that he or she has no control over the game [113]. Since reducing gambling-related cognitive distortions (such as the illusion of control) is one of the best predictors of recovery from gambling disorder [114], cognitive distortions in older adults merit investigation for targeted prevention [53]. Their identification in the perception of gambling by Asians over 60 reveals that three levels of cognitive distortions play a role in the maintenance and escalation of their gambling practice [115]: these erroneous cognitions relate to probabilistic control (chasing wins or hot hands, trying to recover from cold hands, and gambler’s fallacy), interpretive control (the belief that there are more wins than losses), and illusion of control (perception of competence, near wins, luck, and superstitions). However, older French people (55+) show less illusion of control than very young people (18–25), even though they take more risks in simulated gambling [116]. It should be noted that, in addition to the cultural difference, the majority of the French participants were non-gamblers or occasional gamblers, whereas the Asian participants were regular gamblers.
The consequences of gambling disorder are of concern, with older gamblers (65+) having greater difficulty recovering from health complications, psychological and social problems, and financial difficulties resulting from problem gambling [117]. Indeed, older adults with gambling disorder, who are less likely to be identified and less likely to seek help, appear to be more vulnerable (financially fragile and at risk of suicide) than other age groups [118]. It should be noted that some gamblers, on the contrary, combine old age with wisdom by moving from gambling disorder to recreational gambling as they age [15]. Although rates of participation and gambling disorder tend to decline with age, they increased for both younger and older people between 1997 and 2000 [93]. More research is needed on the gambling behaviors of older gamblers and their determinants in order to identify the tipping point from leisure to addiction.

5. Discussion

Generally perceived by older adults as a safe leisure activity, gambling is, for them, a way to break from isolation and boredom [11,12]. While the motivations of aging gamblers may vary across cultures, entertainment and pleasure are universal motivations [14,58]. Recreational gambling among older adults is even associated with positive outcomes in terms of socialization, health, well-being, and cognitive stimulation [42,71]. In contrast, when the main motivation is to escape anxiety and loneliness [73,74], the consequences are harmful [30,37,43] and more deleterious in older adults than in younger ones [117,118]. In addition, age-related changes in decision-making processes are a vulnerability factor for gambling disorder. They lead older adults to take greater risks in uncertain contexts, especially those involving losses [88,89,90,91].
The older adults’ isolation is not only a risk factor for problem gambling in this population, but it can also interfere with the effectiveness of therapies [119]. Family or social support is essential to therapeutic effectiveness [120]. Although studies lack standardized methods to draw solid conclusions, cognitive-behavioral therapy has shown some effectiveness on gambling disorders. The effects are not immediate, but improvements in gamblers’ quality of life and decreases in stress, anxiety, depression, gambling frequency, and wagering amounts are seen at three, six, and twelve months [121]. Interventions that use a manual and those that combine cognitive-behavioral therapy with mindfulness or Gamblers Anonymous support group sessions are the most effective. Group cognitive-behavioral therapy appears to be an effective treatment approach for both restoring self-control over gambling behavior and reducing the emotional distress associated with this disorder [122]. Research on the effectiveness of cognitive-behavioral therapy in the elderly has highlighted its major value in older adults, who benefit from it to a degree roughly equivalent to that of younger adults. Although the cognitive-behavioral theoretical foundations remain the same, specific adaptations of therapeutic strategies and processes can optimize results in the elderly [123]. Care must be taken in group therapy to ensure that all group members have similar cognitive abilities; otherwise, the progress of all participants may be compromised. Realistic and achievable objectives, adapted to the patient, must be considered [120]. The use of a specific format that focuses more on behavioral interventions is sometimes advocated [124].

6. Conclusions

Before concluding, we note the limitations of this narrative review. The methodological criteria are lower than in a systematic review, which may include a snowballing literature search [125]. However, this type of review allows us to have a general view of a specific subject, to raise issues that are usually neglected, and to encourage further research on this subject [126,127,128].
Older adults are a specific population for gambling issues, not only in terms of the consequences of gambling disorder [98,117,118] but also in terms of the cognitions underlying gambling behaviors [116]. To understand the mechanisms that cause older adults to shift from leisure to addiction, we need to understand the specific behaviors of these aging gamblers. While the value of behavioral approaches to understanding and preventing substance use disorders is clear [129], there is also much work to be conducted on non-substance use disorders such as gambling, and particularly with populations whose specificities need to be examined, such as older gamblers.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, V.L.F., C.L. and M.F.; investigation, M.F.; writing—original draft preparation, M.F.; writing—review and editing, M.F., C.L., C.B., I.G., J.P., I.V. and V.L.F. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

The work has been funded by the Agence Nationale de la Recherche, France (234372 Jeuvieilli project–APPG 2020–CE36).

Institutional Review Board Statement

Not applicable.

Informed Consent Statement

Not applicable.

Data Availability Statement

No new data were created or analyzed in this study. Data sharing is not applicable to this article.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest. The funders had no role in the writing of the manuscript; no constraints on publishing have been imposed.

References

  1. Observatoire Français des Drogues et des Tendances Addictives. Available online: https://www.ofdt.fr/produits-et-addictions/de-z/jeux-de-hasard-et-d-argent/ (accessed on 20 April 2023).
  2. Abbott, M.; Binde, P.; Clark, L.; Hodgins, D.; Johnson, M.; Manitowabi, D.; Quilty, L.; Spangberg, J.; Volberg, R.; Walker, D.; et al. Conceptual Framework of Harmful Gambling: An International Collaboration, 3rd ed.; Gambling Research Exchange Ontario (GREO): Guelph, ON, Canada, 2018. [Google Scholar]
  3. Binde, P. Why People Gamble: A Model with Five Motivational Dimensions. Int. Gambl. Stud. 2013, 13, 81–97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Jacques, C.; Ladouceur, R.; Ferland, F. Impact of Availability on Gambling: A Longitudinal Study. Can. J. Psychiatry 2000, 45, 810–815. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Abbott, M.W. Gambling and Gambling-Related Harm: Recent World Health Organization Initiatives. Public Health 2020, 184, 56–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Calado, F.; Griffiths, M.D. Problem Gambling Worldwide: An Update and Systematic Review of Empirical Research (2000–2015). J. Behav. Addict. 2016, 5, 592–613. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Costes, J.-M.; Richard, J.-B.; Eroukmanoff, V.; Le Nézet, O.; Philippon, A. Les Français et Les Jeux d’argent et de Hasard. Résultats Du Baromètre de Santé Publique France 2019. Tend. OFDT 2020, 138, 1–6. [Google Scholar]
  8. Dowling, N.A.; Youssef, G.J.; Jackson, A.C.; Pennay, D.W.; Francis, K.L.; Pennay, A.; Lubman, D.I. National Estimates of Australian Gambling Prevalence: F Indings from a Dual-Frame Omnibus Survey. Addiction 2016, 111, 420–435. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Hilbrecht, M.; Baxter, D.; Abbott, M.; Binde, P.; Clark, L.; Hodgins, D.C.; Manitowabi, D.; Quilty, L.; SpÅngberg, J.; Volberg, R.; et al. The Conceptual Framework of Harmful Gambling: A Revised Framework for Understanding Gambling Harm. J. Behav. Addict. 2020, 9, 190–205. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Ssewanyana, D.; Bitanihirwe, B. Problem Gambling among Young People in Sub-Saharan Africa. Front. Public Health 2018, 6, 23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Luo, H.; Ferguson, M. Gambling among Culturally Diverse Older Adults: A Systematic Review of Qualitative and Quantitative Data. Int. Gambl. Stud. 2017, 17, 290–316. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Tse, S.; Hong, S.-I.; Wang, C.-W.; Cunningham-Williams, R.M. Gambling Behavior and Problems Among Older Adults: A Systematic Review of Empirical Studies. J. Gerontol. 2012, 67, 639. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Medeiros, G.C.; Leppink, E.; Yaemi, A.; Mariani, M.; Tavares, H.; Grant, J. Gambling Disorder in Older Adults: A Cross-Cultural Perspective. Compr. Psychiatry 2015, 58, 116–121. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Martin, F.; Lichtenberg, P.A.; Templin, T.N. A Longitudinal Study: Casino Gambling Attitudes, Motivations, and Gambling Patterns among Urban Elders. J. Gambl. Stud. 2011, 27, 287–297. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Gaimard, M.; Gateau, M. Seniors, Jeux de Hasard et D’argent. Loisir Ou Addiction? Sociétés; Éditions Universitaires de Dijon: Dijon, France, 2017; ISBN 978-2-36441-208-8. [Google Scholar]
  16. Blanchet, M. Gérontocroissance et territoires: Quel potentiel pour la silver économie ? Popul. Avenir 2018, 739, 4–7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Bjelde, K.; Chromy, B.; Pankow, D. Casino Gambling among Older Adults in North Dakota: A Policy Analysis. J. Gambl. Stud. 2008, 24, 423–440. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Chhabra, D. Are Late Life Gamblers a Lucrative Market in Gambling Tourism? A Case Study of Iowa, United States. J. Hosp. Tour. Res. 2009, 33, 245–254. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Luquiens, A. Il n’y a pas d’âge pour jouer trop! NPG Neurol. Psychiatr. Gériatrie 2016, 16, 243–245. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Tira, C.; Jackson, A.C.; Tomnay, J.E. Pathways to Late-Life Problematic Gambling in Seniors: A Grounded Theory Approach. Gerontologist 2014, 54, 1035–1048. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Pattinson, J.; Parke, A. Gambling Behaviour and Motivation in British Older Adult Populations: A Grounded Theoretical Framework. J. Gambl. Issues 2017, 34, 55–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Lister, J.J.; Nower, L. Gambling and Older Adults. In The Wiley-Blackwell Handbook of Disordered Gambling; John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 2013; pp. 347–360. ISBN 978-1-118-31607-8. [Google Scholar]
  23. Granero, R.; Jiménez-Murcia, S.; Fernández-Aranda, F.; Del Pino-Gutiérrez, A.; Mena-Moreno, T.; Mestre-Bach, G.; Gómez-Peña, M.; Moragas, L.; Aymamí, N.; Giroux, I.; et al. Contribution of Stressful Life Events to Gambling Activity in Older Age. Ageing Soc. 2020, 42, 1513–1537. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Subramaniam, M.; Wang, P.; Soh, P.; Vaingankar, J.A.; Chong, S.A.; Browning, C.J.; Thomas, S.A. Prevalence and Determinants of Gambling Disorder among Older Adults: A Systematic Review. Addict. Behav. 2015, 41, 199–209. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Skinner, W.; Turner, N.E. Gambling on An Aging Population. J. Gambl. Issues 2018, 39, 1–5. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. World Health Organization. Available online: https://www.who.int/news-room/fact-sheets/detail/ageing-and-health (accessed on 20 April 2023).
  27. Ladouceur, B. Vincent Caradec, Sociologie de la vieillesse et du vieillissement. Lectures 2008, 128, 127. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Bourdelais, P. Âge de La Vieillesse (L’); Odile Jacob: Paris, France, 1993; ISBN 2-7381-0200-X. [Google Scholar]
  29. Christensen, M.H.; Patsdaughter, C.A. Gambling Behaviors in Black Older Adults: Perceived Effects on Health. J. Gerontol. Nurs. 2004, 30, 34–39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  30. Granero, R.; Jiménez-Murcia, S.; del Pino-Gutiérrez, A.; Mena-Moreno, T.; Mestre-Bach, G.; Gómez-Peña, M.; Moragas, L.; Aymamí, N.; Giroux, I.; Grall-Bronnec, M.; et al. Gambling Phenotypes in Older Adults. J. Gambl. Stud. 2020, 36, 809–828. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Giroux, I.; Ferland, F.; Savard, C.; Jacques, C.; Brochu, P.; Nadeau, D.; Landreville, P.; Sévigny, S. Les Joueurs sans Problème de Jeu de 55 Ans et plus: Événements, Conséquences et Caractéristiques Structurelles et Environnementales Influençant Les Habitudes de Jeu. J. Gambl. Issues 2016, 32, 89–110. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Guillou Landreat, M.; Le Reste, J.Y.; Théréné-Mouden, C.; Caillon, J.; Cholet, J.; Grall Bronnec, M. Les Jeux de Hasard et d’argent Chez Les Sujets Âgés. Soins Gérontologie 2016, 5237, 1–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Lai, D.W. Gambling and the Older Chinese in Canada. J. Gambl. Stud. 2006, 22, 121–141. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. McKay, C. Double Jeopardy: Older Women and Problem Gambling. Int. J. Ment. Health Addict. 2005, 3, 35–53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Pilver, C.E.; Libby, D.J.; Hoff, R.A.; Potenza, M.N. Problem Gambling Severity and the Incidence of Axis I Psychopathology among Older Adults in the General Population. J. Psychiatr. Res. 2013, 47, 534–541. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Tira, C.; Jackson, A.C. Exploring the Gray Areas: Senior Gamblers’ Perceptions of What Is and What Isn’t Gambling. J. Gambl. Issues 2015, 24–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. van der Maas, M.; Mann, R.E.; McCready, J.; Matheson, F.I.; Turner, N.E.; Hamilton, H.A.; Schrans, T.; Ialomiteanu, A. Problem Gambling in a Sample of Older Adult Casino Gamblers: Associations with Gambling Participation and Motivations. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry Neurol. 2016, 30, 3–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Nower, L.; Blaszczynski, A. Characteristics of Problem Gamblers 56 Years of Age or Older: A Statewide Study of Casino Self-Excluders. Psychol. Aging 2008, 23, 577–584. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Erickson, L.; Molina, C.A.; Ladd, G.T.; Pietrzak, R.H.; Petry, N.M. Problem and Pathological Gambling Are Associated with Poorer Mental and Physical Health in Older Adults. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 2005, 20, 754–759. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Kausch, O. Pathological Gambling among Elderly Veterans. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry Neurol. 2004, 17, 13–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Boisvert, Y.; Papineau, E.; Lesemann, F. L’offre Organisée de Jeux de Hasard et d’argent Aux Aînés: Responsabilité Sociale, Gouvernance et Prévention; Conseil des aînés, MSSS, le Secrétariat aux aînés, l’AQRP et le FQRSC: Montréal, QC, Canada, 2012. [Google Scholar]
  42. Desai, R.A.; Maciejewski, P.K.; Dausey, D.J.; Caldarone, B.J.; Potenza, M.N. Health Correlates of Recreational Gambling in Older Adults. Am. J. Psychiatry 2004, 161, 1672–1679. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Parke, A.; Griffiths, M.; Pattinson, J.; Keatley, D. Age-Related Physical and Psychological Vulnerability as Pathways to Problem Gambling in Older Adults. J. Behav. Addict. 2018, 7, 137–145. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Vander Bilt, J.; Dodge, H.H.; Pandav, R.; Shaffer, H.J.; Ganguli, M. Gambling Participation and Social Support among Older Adults: A Longitudinal Community Study. J. Gambl. Stud. 2004, 20, 373–389. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Petry, N.M. A Comparison of Young, Middle-Aged, and Older Adult Treatment-Seeking Pathological Gamblers. Gerontologist 2002, 42, 92–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Schellinck, T.; Schrans, T.; Walsh, G.; Grace, J. 2002 Seniors Survey—Prevalence of Substance Use and Gambling among New Brunswick Adults Aged 55+; Department of Health and Wellness New Brunswick: Moncton, NB, Canada, 2002. [Google Scholar]
  47. Fein, G.; McGillivray, S.; Finn, P. Older Adults Make Less Advantageous Decisions than Younger Adults: Cognitive and Psychological Correlates. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. JINS 2007, 13, 480–489. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Ladd, G.T.; Molina, C.A.; Kerins, G.J.; Petry, N.M. Gambling Participation and Problems among Older Adults. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry Neurol. 2003, 16, 172–177. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Pietrzak, R.H.; Morasco, B.J.; Blanco, C.; Grant, B.F.; Petry, N.M. Gambling Level and Psychiatric and Medical Disorders in Older Adults: Results from the National Epidemiologic Survey on Alcohol and Related Conditions. Am. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 2007, 15, 301–313. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  50. Afifi, T.O.; Cox, B.J.; Martens, P.J.; Sareen, J.; Enns, M.W. Demographic and Social Variables Associated with Problem Gambling among Men and Women in Canada. Psychiatry Res. 2010, 178, 395–400. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  51. Blanco, C.; Hasin, D.S.; Petry, N.; Stinson, F.S.; Grant, B.F. Sex Differences in Subclinical and DSM-IV Pathological Gambling: Results from the National Epidemiologic Survey on Alcohol and Related Conditions. Psychol. Med. 2006, 36, 943. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  52. Clarke, D. Older Adults’ Gambling Motivation and Problem Gambling: A Comparative Study. J. Gambl. Stud. 2008, 24, 175–192. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  53. Guillou Landreat, M.; Cholet, J.; Grall Bronnec, M.; Lalande, S.; Le Reste, J.Y. Determinants of Gambling Disorders in Elderly People—A Systematic Review. Front. Psychiatry 2019, 10, 837. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Zaranek, R.R.; Lichtenberg, P.A. Urban Elders and Casino Gambling: Are They at Risk of a Gambling Problem? J. Aging Stud. 2008, 22, 13–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Bjerg, O. Problem Gambling in Poker: Money, Rationality and Control in a Skill-Based Social Game. Int. Gambl. Stud. 2010, 10, 239–254. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Del Pino-Gutiérrez, A.; Granero, R.; Fernández-Aranda, F.; Mena-Moreno, T.; Mestre-Bach, G.; Gómez-Peña, M.; Moragas, L.; Aymamí, N.; Giroux, I.; Grall-Bronnec, M.; et al. Gambling Activity in the Old-Age General Population. Ageing Soc. 2022, 42, 2757–2783. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. McNeilly, D.P.; Burke, W.J. Late Life Gambling: The Attitudes and Behaviors of Older Adults. J. Gambl. Stud. 2000, 16, 393–415. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Phillips, W.J. Senior Casino Motivation and Gaming Intention: An Extended Theory of Planned Behavior Model; Thèse sous la direction de Canter, D.D., et Jang, S., Eds.; Kansas State University: Manhattan, KS, USA, 2009. [Google Scholar]
  59. Deci, E.L.; Ryan, R.M. Intrinsic Motivation and Self-Determination in Human Behavior; Plenum Press: New York, NY, USA; London, UK, 1985; ISBN 1489922733. [Google Scholar]
  60. Deci, E.L.; Ryan, R.M. The “What” and “Why” of Goal Pursuits: Human Needs and the Self-Determination of Behavior. Psychol. Inq. 2000, 11, 227–268. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Chantal, Y.; Vallerand, R.J. Skill versus Luck: A Motivational Analysis of Gambling Involvement. J. Gambl. Stud. 1996, 12, 407–418. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Blaszczynski, A.; Nower, L. A Pathways Model of Problem and Pathological Gambling. Addiction 2002, 97, 487–499. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Campbell, F. The Future of Gambling. Futurist 1976, 10, 84–90. [Google Scholar]
  64. Laditka, S.B.; Corwin, S.J.; Laditka, J.N.; Liu, R.; Tseng, W.; Wu, B.; Beard, R.L.; Sharkey, J.R.; Ivey, S.L. Attitudes about Aging Well among a Diverse Group of Older Americans: Implications for Promoting Cognitive Health. Gerontologist 2009, 49 (Suppl. 1), S30–S39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. McNeilly, D.P.; Burke, W.J. Disposable Time and Disposable Income: Problem Casino Gambling Behavior in Older Adults. J. Clin. Geropsychology 2002, 8, 75–85. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Beitz, K.M.; Salthouse, T.A.; Davis, H.P. Performance on the Iowa Gambling Task: From 5 to 89 Years of Age. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 2014, 143, 1677–1689. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Boggio, P.S.; Campanhã, C.; Valasek, C.A.; Fecteau, S.; Pascual-Leone, A.; Fregni, F. Modulation of Decision-Making in a Gambling Task in Older Adults with Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2010, 31, 593–597. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Denburg, N.L.; Tranel, D.; Bechara, A. The Ability to Decide Advantageously Declines Prematurely in Some Normal Older Persons. Neuropsychologia 2005, 43, 1099–1106. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Denburg, N.L.; Weller, J.A.; Yamada, T.H.; Shivapour, D.M.; Kaup, A.R.; LaLoggia, A.; Cole, C.A.; Tranel, D.; Bechara, A. Poor Decision Making among Older Adults Is Related to Elevated Levels of Neuroticism. Ann. Behav. Med. Publ. Soc. Behav. Med. 2009, 37, 164–172. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. O’Brien Cousins, S.; Witcher, C.; Moodie, J. High Quality Aging or Gambling with Health? The Lifestyles of Elders Who Play Bingo: Final Summary Report; Alberta Gaming Research Institute: Calgary, AB, Canada, 2002. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Hagen, B.; Nixon, G.; Solowoniuk, J. Stacking the Odds: A Phenomenological Study of Non-Problem Gambling in Later Life*. Can. J. Aging Rev. Can. Vieil. 2005, 24, 433–442. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Wainstein, L.; Grall-Bronnec, M.; Venisse, J.-L. Caractéristiques Des Conduites de Jeux Chez Le Sujet Âgé. NPG Neurol. Psychiatr. Geriatr. 2008, 8, 17–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  73. Grant, J.E.; Kim, S.W.; Odlaug, B.L.; Buchanan, S.N.; Potenza, M.N. Late-Onset Pathological Gambling: Clinical Correlates and Gender Differences. J. Psychiatr. Res. 2009, 43, 380–387. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  74. Kerber, C.S.; Black, D.W.; Buckwalter, K. Comorbid Psychiatric Disorders among Older Adult Recovering Pathological Gamblers. Issues Ment. Health Nurs. 2008, 29, 1018–1028. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  75. Cowley, E.; Briley, D.A.; Farrell, C. How Do Gamblers Maintain an Illusion of Control? J. Bus. Res. 2015, 68, 2181–2188. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  76. Ladouceur, R.; Walker, M. A Cognitive Perspective on Gambling. In Trends in Cognitive and Behavioural Therapies; Salkovskis, P.M., Ed.; Wiley: New York, NY, USA, 1996; pp. 89–120. [Google Scholar]
  77. Rogers, P. The Cognitive Psychology of Lottery Gambling: A Theoretical Review. J. Gambl. Stud. 1998, 14, 111–134. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  78. Allain, P. Decision-making: Theoretical aspects, neuroanatomy and assessment. Rev. Neuropsychol. 2013, 5, 69–81. [Google Scholar]
  79. Lemaire, P. Psychologie Cognitive; De Boeck Université: Bruxelles, Belgium, 1999. [Google Scholar]
  80. Kahneman, D.; Tversky, A. Subjective Probability: A Judgment of Representativeness. Cognit. Psychol. 1972, 3, 430–454. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  81. Kahneman, D.; Tversky, A. On the Psychology of Prediction. Psychol. Rev. 1973, 80, 237–251. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  82. Peters, E.; Hess, T.M.; Västfjäll, D.; Auman, C. Adult Age Differences in Dual Information Processes: Implications for the Role of Affective and Deliberative Processes in Older Adults’ Decision Making. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 2007, 2, 1–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  83. Egidi, M. Le Processus Dual Du Raisonnement: Origines, Problèmes et Perspectives. In Économie et Cognition; Walliser, B., Ed.; Cogniprisme; Éditions de la Maison des sciences de l’homme: Paris, France, 2017; pp. 11–54. ISBN 978-2-7351-1877-9. [Google Scholar]
  84. Ajzen, I. The Social Psychology of Decision Making. In Social Psychology: Handbook of Basic Principles; Higgins, E.T., Kruglanski, A.W., Eds.; Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 1996; pp. 297–325. [Google Scholar]
  85. Martinez, F. L’individu face au risque: L’apport de Kahneman et Tversky. Idées Économiques Soc. 2010, 161, 15–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  86. Kahneman, D.; Tversky, A. Prospect Theory: An Analysis of Decision under Risk. Econometrica 1979, 47, 263–291. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  87. Martinez, F. Lien Entre l’annonce Du Résultat d’autrui, l’illusion de Contrôle et La Prise de Risque Dans Un Jeu de Hasard et d’argent; Le mirail: Toulouse, France, 2004. [Google Scholar]
  88. Löckenhoff, C.E. Aging and Decision-Making: A Conceptual Framework for Future Research-A Mini-Review. Gerontology 2018, 64, 140–148. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  89. Mather, M.; Mazar, N.; Gorlick, M.A.; Lighthall, N.R.; Burgeno, J.; Schoeke, A.; Ariely, D. Risk Preferences and Aging: The “Certainty Effect” in Older Adults’ Decision Making. Psychol. Aging 2012, 27, 801. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  90. Pachur, T.; Mata, R.; Hertwig, R. Who Dares, Who Errs? Disentangling Cognitive and Motivational Roots of Age Differences in Decisions Under Risk. Psychol. Sci. 2017, 28, 504–518. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  91. Tymula, A.; Belmaker, L.A.R.; Ruderman, L.; Glimcher, P.W.; Levy, I. Like Cognitive Function, Decision Making across the Life Span Shows Profound Age-Related Changes. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2013, 110, 17143–17148. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  92. Takeuchi, H.; Kawada, R.; Tsurumi, K.; Yokoyama, N. Heterogeneity of Loss Aversion in Pathological Gambling. J. Gambl. Stud. 2016, 32, 1143–1154. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  93. Welte, J.W.; Barnes, G.M.; Tidwell, M.-C.O.; Hoffman, J.H. Gambling and Problem Gambling Across the Lifespan. J. Gambl. Stud. 2011, 27, 49–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  94. Queen, T.L.; Berg, C.A.; Lowrance, W. Chapter 18-A Framework for Decision Making in Couples across Adulthood. In Aging and Decision Making; Hess, T.M., Strough, J., Löckenhoff, C.E., Eds.; Academic Press: San Diego, CA, USA, 2015; pp. 371–392. ISBN 978-0-12-417148-0. [Google Scholar]
  95. Strough, J.; Karns, T.E.; Schlosnagle, L. Decision-Making Heuristics and Biases across the Life Span. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 2011, 1235, 57–74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  96. Moutier, S. Le rôle des émotions dans les jeux de hasard et d’argent. Développement typique et atypique des capacités de prise de décision. Psychotropes 2015, 21, 9–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  97. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th ed.; American Psychiatric Association Publishing: Washington DC, USA, 2013. [Google Scholar]
  98. Grant Stitt, B.; Giacopassi, D.; Nichols, M. Gambling Among Older Adults: A Comparative Analysis. Exp. Aging Res. 2003, 29, 189–203. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  99. Gazel, C.; Fatséas, M.; Auriacombe, M. Quels Changements Pour Les Addictions Dans Le DSM-5. Lett. Psychiatre 2014, 10, 50–53. [Google Scholar]
  100. Moccia, L.; Pettorruso, M.; De Crescenzo, F.; De Risio, L.; di Nuzzo, L.; Martinotti, G.; Bifone, A.; Janiri, L.; Di Nicola, M. Neural Correlates of Cognitive Control in Gambling Disorder: A Systematic Review of FMRI Studies. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2017, 78, 104–116. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  101. Pettorruso, M.; Martinotti, G.; Cocciolillo, F.; De Risio, L.; Cinquino, A.; Di Nicola, M.; Camardese, G.; Migliara, G.; Moccia, L.; Conte, E.; et al. Striatal Presynaptic Dopaminergic Dysfunction in Gambling Disorder: A 123 I-FP-CIT SPECT Study. Addict. Biol. 2019, 24, 1077–1086. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  102. Pettorruso, M.; Martinotti, G.; Fasano, A.; Loria, G.; Di Nicola, M.; De Risio, L.; Ricciardi, L.; Conte, G.; Janiri, L.; Bentivoglio, A.R. Anhedonia in Parkinson’s Disease Patients with and without Pathological Gambling: A Case-Control Study. Psychiatry Res. 2014, 215, 448–452. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  103. Dreher, J.-C.; Meyer-Lindenberg, A.; Kohn, P.; Berman, K.F. Age-Related Changes in Midbrain Dopaminergic Regulation of the Human Reward System. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2008, 105, 15106–15111. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  104. Casey, L.M.; Oei, T.P.S.; Melville, K.M.; Bourke, E.; Newcombe, P.A. Measuring Self-Efficacy in Gambling: The Gambling Refusal Self-Efficacy Questionnaire. J. Gambl. Stud. 2008, 24, 229–246. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  105. Bandura, A. Self-Efficacy: The Exercise of Control; W H Freeman/Times Books/Henry Holt & Co.: New York, NY, USA, 1997; ISBN 978-0-7167-2626-5. [Google Scholar]
  106. Hiroto, D.S.; Seligman, M.E. Generality of Learned Helplessness in Man. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1975, 31, 311–327. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  107. Kern, D. La formation en gérontologie du point de vue des Sciences de l’Education. Gerontol. Soc. 2006, 29118, 117–130. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  108. Maly, M.R.; Costigan, P.A.; Olney, S.J. Self-Efficacy Mediates Walking Performance in Older Adults with Knee Osteoarthritis. J. Gerontol. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 2007, 62, 1142–1146. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  109. Dubois, N. La Psychologie Du Contrôle: Les Croyances Internes et Externes; Vies sociales; Presses universitaires de Grenoble: Grenoble, France, 1990; ISBN 978-2-7061-0292-9. [Google Scholar]
  110. Piper, A.; Langer, E. Aging and Mindful Control. In The Psychology of Control and Aging; Baltes, M., Baltes, P., Eds.; Lawrence Erlbaum associates: London, UK, 1986; pp. 71–89. ISBN 978-1-84872-459-4. [Google Scholar]
  111. Barrault, S.; Varescon, I. Distorsions cognitives et pratique de jeu de hasard et d’argent: État de la question. Psychol. Française 2012, 57, 17–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  112. Keren, G.B.; Wagenaar, W.A. On the Psychology of Playing Blackjack: Normative and Descriptive Considerations with Implications for Decision Theory. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 1985, 114, 133. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  113. Mouneyrac, A. Messages de Prévention Promouvant Le Jeu Responsable: Une Injonction Paradoxale Dans Les Jeux de Hasard et d’argent. Ph.D. Thesis, Université Toulouse le Mirail-Toulouse II, Toulouse, France, 2019. [Google Scholar]
  114. Rossini-Dib, D.; Fuentes, D.; Tavares, H. A Naturalistic Study of Recovering Gamblers: What Gets Better and When They Get Better. Psychiatry Res. 2015, 227, 17–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  115. Subramaniam, M.; Chong, S.A.; Browning, C.; Thomas, S. Cognitive Distortions among Older Adult Gamblers in an Asian Context. PLoS ONE 2017, 12, e0178036. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  116. Fontaine, M.; Floch, V.L.; Lemercier, C. Gambling and Ageing: Less Illusion but More Risk. Ageing Soc. 2023, 43, 556–575. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  117. Ariyabuddhiphongs, V. Older Adults and Gambling: A Review. Int. J. Ment. Health Addict. 2012, 10, 297–308. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  118. Pittet, Y.-K.; Simon, O.; Besson, J. Jeu excessif et personnes âgées. La Gazette Médicale. 2014, 1, 10–13. [Google Scholar]
  119. Vostrý, M.; Fischer, S.; Lanková, B. The Effect of Combined Therapy on the Support and Development of Social Skills of People with Multiple Sclerosis in Senior Age. Neuro Endocrinol. Lett. 2020, 41, 270–274. [Google Scholar]
  120. Barnes, M.P. Principles of Neurological Rehabilitation. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 2003, 74, iv3–iv7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  121. Higueruela-Ahijado, M.; López-Espuela, F.; Caro-Alonso, P.Á.; Novo, A.; Rodríguez-Martín, B. Efficacy of Cognitive-Behavioral Therapy in Improving the Quality of Life of People with Compulsive Gambling, a Systematic Review. Arch. Psychiatr. Nurs. 2023, 43, 127–142. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  122. Jiménez-Murcia, S.; Álvarez-Moya, E.M.; Granero, R.; Neus Aymami, M.; Gómez-Peña, M.; Jaurrieta, N.; Sans, B.; Rodriguez-Marti, J.; Vallejo, J. Cognitive-Behavioral Group Treatment for Pathological Gambling: Analysis of Effectiveness and Predictors of Therapy Outcome. Psychother. Res. 2007, 17, 544–552. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  123. Palazzolo, J. Les thérapies comportementales et cognitives du sujet âgé: Aspects pratiques et spécifiques. NPG Neurol. Psychiatr. Gériatrie 2020, 20, 56–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  124. Simon, S.S.; Cordás, T.A.; Bottino, C.M.C. Cognitive Behavioral Therapies in Older Adults with Depression and Cognitive Deficits: A Systematic Review. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 2015, 30, 223–233. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  125. Framarin, A.; Déry, V. Les Revues Narratives: Fondements Scientifiques Pour Soutenir l’établissement de Repères Institutionnels; Institut National de Santé Publique Québec: Québec, QC, Canada, 2021; ISBN 978-2-550-89320-2.
  126. Alvesson, M.; Sandberg, J. The Problematizing Review: A Counterpoint to Elsbach and Van Knippenberg’s Argument for Integrative Reviews. J. Manag. Stud. 2020, 57, 1290–1304. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  127. Green, B.N.; Johnson, C.D.; Adams, A. Writing Narrative Literature Reviews for Peer-Reviewed Journals: Secrets of the Trade. J. Chiropr. Med. 2006, 5, 101–117. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  128. Greenhalgh, T.; Thorne, S.; Malterud, K. Time to Challenge the Spurious Hierarchy of Systematic over Narrative Reviews? Eur. J. Clin. Inve. 2018, 48, e12931. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  129. Washington, W.D.; Galizio, M. From Lab Bench to Park Bench: How Behavioral Science Affects the Field of Substance Abuse. Perspect. Behav. Sci. 2019, 42, 375–384. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. The value function [85,86].
Figure 1. The value function [85,86].
Behavsci 13 00437 g001
Table 1. The four attitudes towards risk [85].
Table 1. The four attitudes towards risk [85].
Coding DomainType of Probability
Low ProbabilitiesMedium and High Probabilities
GainsRisk seeking
(e.g., preference for a bet with a 1% chance of winning USD 5000 over a sure gain of USD 5)
Risk aversion
(e.g., preference for a sure gain of USD 500 over a bet with a 50% chance of winning USD 1000)
LossesRisk aversion
(e.g., preference for a sure loss of USD 5 over a bet with a 1% chance of losing USD 5000)
Risk seeking
(e.g., preference for a bet with a 50% chance of losing USD 1000 over a sure loss of USD 500)
Table 2. Choice under the certainty effect.
Table 2. Choice under the certainty effect.
DomainOutcome Probability of Occurrence
0% or 100% (Sure Outcome)Between ]0 and 100[% (Risky Outcome)
GainsPreferred choiceAvoided choice
LossesAvoided choicePreferred choice
Table 3. Material from Takeuchi et al. [92].
Table 3. Material from Takeuchi et al. [92].
Options Available to Participants
Option 1
50% chance of losing a fixed amount X
(2500, 5000, 10,000, or 15,000 yen depending on the trial)
50% chance of winning an amount Y
(between 0.5 × X and 10 × X )
Option 2
50% chance of losing 0 yen50% chance of winning 0 yen
Loss aversion parameter (λ)
calculated from the prospect theory of Kahneman and Tversky
Low aversionMedium aversionHigh aversion
0 < λ < 3.333.34 < λ < 6.666.67 < λ < 10
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Fontaine, M.; Lemercier, C.; Bonnaire, C.; Giroux, I.; Py, J.; Varescon, I.; Le Floch, V. Gambling and Aging: An Overview of a Risky Behavior. Behav. Sci. 2023, 13, 437. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs13060437

AMA Style

Fontaine M, Lemercier C, Bonnaire C, Giroux I, Py J, Varescon I, Le Floch V. Gambling and Aging: An Overview of a Risky Behavior. Behavioral Sciences. 2023; 13(6):437. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs13060437

Chicago/Turabian Style

Fontaine, Maylis, Céline Lemercier, Céline Bonnaire, Isabelle Giroux, Jacques Py, Isabelle Varescon, and Valérie Le Floch. 2023. "Gambling and Aging: An Overview of a Risky Behavior" Behavioral Sciences 13, no. 6: 437. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs13060437

APA Style

Fontaine, M., Lemercier, C., Bonnaire, C., Giroux, I., Py, J., Varescon, I., & Le Floch, V. (2023). Gambling and Aging: An Overview of a Risky Behavior. Behavioral Sciences, 13(6), 437. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs13060437

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop