Next Article in Journal
Comparing Older Parents’ and Adult Children’s Fear of Falling and Perceptions of Age-Friendly Home Modification: An Integration of the Theories of Planned Behavior and Protection Motivation
Previous Article in Journal
Effects of Low-Intensity Aerobic Exercise on Neurophysiological and Behavioral Correlates of Cognitive Function
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

The Role of Alexithymia and Impulsivity in Male Victims and Perpetrators of Intimate Partner Violence

by
Stefania Mannarini
1,2,
Federica Taccini
2,3,* and
Alessandro Alberto Rossi
1,2
1
Department of Philosophy, Sociology, Education, and Applied Psychology, Section of Applied Psychology, University of Padua, 35131 Padua, Italy
2
Centre for Intervention and Research on Family (CIRF)—Department of Philosophy, Sociology, Education, and Applied Psychology, Section of Applied Psychology, University of Padua, 35131 Padua, Italy
3
Department of Developmental Psychology and Socialisation, University of Padua, 35131 Padua, Italy
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Behav. Sci. 2023, 13(5), 402; https://doi.org/10.3390/bs13050402
Submission received: 14 February 2023 / Revised: 27 April 2023 / Accepted: 4 May 2023 / Published: 11 May 2023

Abstract

:
(1) Background: Alexithymia and impulsivity appear to represent risk factors for violence perpetration, while mixed results are present with respect to victimization experience. In light of this, the purpose of this study was to compare the roles of both alexithymia and impulsivity among three different samples of men: men who experienced partner victimization (IPVV); male intimate partner violence perpetrators (IPVP); and men from the general population (CG). (2) Methods: Participants were recruited from specialized centers in Italy. A profile analysis was conducted. (3) Results: The results showed that IPVV presented alexithymia and impulsivity comparable to the CG. Furthermore, differences were found between victims and perpetrators in terms of impulsivity and alexithymia. The IPVP group had higher levels of both impulsivity and alexithymia in comparison to IPVV. Moreover, the perpetrators exhibited significantly higher levels of alexithymia compared to the CG. However, despite the medium Cohen’s d (d = 0.441) resulting from the analyses, IPVP’s level of impulsivity was not statistically different from the CG. (4) Conclusion: Alexithymia and impulsivity appear to play a key role in violent behaviors and should be the focus of psychological interventions with perpetrators.

1. Introduction

Intimate partner violence (IPV) refers to any conduct that causes physical, sexual, or psychological harm to individuals in a romantic relationship [1]. IPV perpetrated by men is highly prevalent throughout the world, with around 26% of women experiencing physical and/or sexual violence from a current or former male partner at least once in their lives since the age of 15 [2,3,4,5,6]. Regarding male victims, Kolbe and colleagues showed that the prevalence rates of domestic physical abuse against men ranged from around 3.4% to 20.3% [7]. In most cases, these men had also been violent toward their partners [7,8].
Alexithymia and impulsivity have been identified in the literature as risk factors for violence perpetration [9,10]. Alexithymia corresponds to difficulties in identifying, naming, cognitive processing, and regulating emotions. Moreover, people with clinical levels of alexithymia can present an externally oriented style of thinking and struggle to discriminate their subjective feelings from bodily ones of emotional arousal [11]. The literature shows that higher levels of alexithymia are linked to aggressive behaviors [12,13]. In this regard, Strickland and colleagues investigated the alexithymia levels of three different groups of male participants: 79 general violent perpetrators, 31 IPV perpetrators (IPVP), and 80 men for the control group—using the Toronto Alexithymia Scale-20 (TAS-20), which is the most famous self-report of alexithymia [12]. The results showed that general violent perpetrators and IPVP reported statistically significant higher levels of alexithymia compared to the general population in total TAS-20 scores and in two of three subscales of this self-report (difficulty identifying and describing emotions but not externally oriented thinking) [12]. Consequently, aggression may represent a strategy that people with alexithymia use to regulate emotions they cannot express verbally [12,14].
Mixed results are present in IPVV alexithymia levels [15,16,17,18]. In fact, previous research showed the presence of statistically significant higher levels of alexithymia among IPVV compared to individuals in the general population and that alexithymia appeared to play a role in the development of posttraumatic symptoms [18]. However, other research contradicts these results, showing no statistically significant differences in alexithymia levels between these groups [9]. Despite these results, high levels of alexithymia appear to be present in people with antisocial personality disorder [19], in violent forensic psychiatric outpatients [14], in abusive partners [9,12], and in adolescent perpetrators [20]. Additionally, alexithymia is associated with some intrapersonal problems, such as increased physiological arousal [21] and impulsivity [22,23].
Impulsivity refers to the tendency to display behavior that is not well thought through or reflected or that does not consider the consequences involved [24]. Stanford and colleagues suggested a three-factor model of impulsivity: (1) Cognitive Impulsiveness—based on quick choices and decisions; (2) Motor Impulsiveness—based on actions and behaviors conducted without thinking things through; and (3) Non-Planning Impulsiveness—which is based on a lower tendency to make plans ahead [25]. Impulsive behaviors seem to be present in different psychiatric disorders, such as depression, bipolar disorder, borderline personality disorder, antisocial personality disorder, attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), as well as suicide attempts and they appear to be present in criminal perpetrators as well [25,26,27,28]. Moreover, impulsivity is related to aggressive behaviors e.g., [26,27,29,30]), which can be directed toward the self (self-harm) or against others. In this regard, several studies have investigated impulsivity in perpetrators e.g., [29,31,32,33,34,35]). Specifically, the study by Tharp and colleagues with 121 IPVP with alcohol use disorder showed that impulsiveness predicted sexual and psychological violence [33]. Furthermore, the study by Stuart and Holtzworth-Munroe showed that IPVP had statistically significant higher levels of impulsiveness than non-violent men [35]. However, only a few studies have investigated the role of impulsivity in victims of violence e.g., [36,37,38]), and the results in this regard are mixed. Some studies indicate that victims exhibit levels of impulsivity similar to those who have not experienced violence [36]. Conversely, other research suggests that impulsivity, in combination with substance abuse, can increase the likelihood of experiencing further sexual assault [38].
Considering the relevance that both impulsivity and alexithymia appear to have in the victimization phenomenon, this study aims to use a profile analysis to evaluate and compare the contribution of alexithymia and impulsivity to IPV perpetration and victimization in men. Based on previous research, we predict that IPVP will exhibit significantly higher levels of alexithymia and impulsivity compared to the general population and the IPVV group. In addition, we hypothesize that IPVV will demonstrate levels of alexithymia and impulsivity similar to those of the general population.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Procedure

Each participant was recruited individually: a research survey assessing demographic information and psychological variables with self-report questionnaires was administered in person to each participant.
In line with previous research [9], male IPVP and male IPVV were recruited from specialized centers for the treatment of perpetrators of IPV and victims, respectively. The survey was administered within 2 to 6 months of their arrival at the centers. All the centers were in the north of Italy. Lastly, according to previous studies [9], a control group (CG) was randomly enrolled from the general population of Italy through personal invitations and advertisements in universities, libraries, supermarkets, cafes, etc.
Inclusion criteria for the overall sample (general inclusion criteria) consisted of the following: being a native Italian speaker, being over 18 years old, being in a relationship or having ended the relationship at least three months before the assessment, and having the ability to complete the assessment procedure. Whereas inclusion criteria for both the IPVP and the IPVV group were applied (specific inclusion criteria). Men should have exclusively acted (IPVP group) or suffered (IPVV group) at least one episode of IPV within the last 6–18 months. Men who had both suffered from and acted on IPV were excluded from the study. On the contrary, men from the CG should never have acted or been subjected to any kind of IPV.

2.2. Sample Size Calculation

The required sample size for testing the hypothesis of the present study was computed a priori. The G*Power software was used [39]. First, considering the main analysis of this research (see Profile Analysis), the multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA, global effect) family of statistics was chosen. Second, a single independent variable with three levels was specified, i.e., the three groups of men. Third, four different variables (dependent variables) were specified—i.e., four response variables (see the ‘instruments’ section) [9,40,41,42]. Fourth, the a priori effect size statistic was set at small values [43,44]: Pillai’s trace (V) was set to 0.25—resulting in a small effect size: f2(V) = 0.111 [43,45]. Fifth, the Type I error (α) was set at 0.05 (two-sided), and the power (1—β) was set at 0.80 [43]. The results showed that there is more than 85% chance of correctly rejecting the null hypothesis of no significant effect of the model with an overall sample of 63 participants—21 participants per group.

2.3. Participants

An initial sample of 90 men was contacted. However, 14 men did not complete the procedure: 7 men IPVP, 3 men IPVV, and one man from the CG.
Thus, the final sample was composed of 76 participants. Men in IPVP were 25 and aged from 30 to 68 years (mean = 46.88, SD = 9.58). Forty percent were separated/divorced; 32% were married; 20% were in a relationship; and 8% were single. Men in the CG were 30 and aged from 27 to 65 years (mean = 45.30, SD = 12.70). 43.3% were married; 23.4% were separated/divorced; 20% were single; 13.3% were in a relationship; and 8% were single. Men in IPVV were 21 and aged from 24 to 54 years (mean = 41.24, SD = 8.53): 38.1% were separated/divorced; 28.6% were single; 23.8% were in a relationship; and 9.5% were married.

2.4. Measures

The demographic and clinical information form was used to collect: age, kind of interpersonal violence (acted or suffered), and actual relationship status.

2.4.1. Toronto Alexithymia Scale 20 (TAS-20)

The TAS-20 [46] is one of the most widely used self-report questionnaires for the assessment of alexithymia. The TAS-20 was used in several contexts related to IPV—for example, with men/women who acted in IPV as well as with victims [15,47]. The TAS-20 evaluates the three main facets of alexithymia [11]: (A) difficulties in identifying feelings (DIF); (B) difficulties in describing feelings to other people (DDF); and (C) externally oriented thinking (EOT). It consists of 20 items on a 5-point Likert-type scale, with higher scores reflecting higher levels of the measured facet. In this study, the Italian version of the TAS-20 was used, which provided good internal consistency. Total scores: Cronbach’s Alpha = 0.858; DIF: Cronbach’s Alpha = 0.860; DDF: Cronbach’s Alpha = 0.802; and EOT: Cronbach’s Alpha = 0.601.

2.4.2. Barratt Impulsiveness Scale (BIS-11)

The BIS-11 [25,48,49] is one of the most widely used self-report questionnaires for the assessment of impulsivity. The BIS-11 was used in settings associated with IPV—such as with men/women who acted IPV as well as in victims [32,33,34]. The BIS-11 evaluates the three main facets of impulsivity: (A) attentional impulsiveness, (B) motor impulsiveness, and (C) no-planning impulsiveness. However, a general total score was strongly assumed [49]. It consists of 30 items on a 4-point Likert-type scale, with higher scores reflecting higher levels of impulsivity. In this study, the Italian version of the BIS-11 was used, and it provided good internal consistency. Total score: Cronbach’s Alpha = 0.702.

2.5. Statistical Analyses

Data analysis was performed using JASP and R software and the following packages: ‘esvis’ [50], ‘ggplot2′ [51], ‘psych’ [52], and ‘tidyverse’ [53].
First, preliminary analyses [42] were performed: normality, linearity, multicollinearity, and homogeneity of covariance matrices.
Second, a profile analysis (PA) was performed [54]. PA is a specific multivariate approach to testing mean differences (MANOVA family of statistics) that allows to quantify the extent to which groups of individuals (three groups: independent variables) revealed different profiles on variables implied in IPV (four constructs: dependent variables)—computing the amount of difference among profiles [40,55,56,57,58]. According to the guidelines, before performing PA, all dependent variables were rescaled into z-scores [40,42]. PA provides three specific statistics: (I) parallelism; (II) level equality; and (III) flatness [41,42,58,59]. (A) Parallelism assesses whether two profiles are analogous and symmetrical (parallel) among different groups (namely, between-subject general statistics). (B) Level equality refers to the degree of similarity (equal means scores) of all of the dependent variables across groups—a general between-subject statistic. To test the level of equality, several focused comparisons between groups were performed [60]. (C) Flatness determines (within each profile) whether each variable yielded a similar response to the other variables—a general within-subjects statistic [42]. To test flatness, several focused repeated measures comparisons were also performed—separately—for each group (within-group effect).
Bonferroni’s correction was applied. Partial eta-square (η2p) and Cohen’s d were used to quantify the difference in multiple and pairwise comparisons, respectively, with the following benchmarks: small (η2p: 0.011 to 0.059; d: 0.20 to 0.49), moderate (η2p: 0.060 to 0.139; d: 0.50 to 0.79), and large (η2p > 0.140; d > 0.80) [42,43]. In addition, it is important to note that due to the small sample size, differences in means between groups are not likely to be statistically significant; therefore, the results will be interpreted using effect sizes.

3. Results

3.1. Preliminary Analyses: Controlling Model Assumptions

Preliminary analyses revealed that each psychological variable was normally distributed (univariate normality) and that the bivariate relationships among each pair of dependent variables were almost linear (a scatter matrix that revealed no curvilinear relationships). At the same time, Pearson’s bivariate correlation coefficients showed that the bivariate relationships did not exceed the critical cut-off of |0.80|. Moreover, tolerance and variance inflation factor (VIF) statistics showed the absence of multicollinearity. The results are reported in Table 1.
Finally, the Box’s M test (homogeneity of variance-covariance matrices) resulted in being statistically significant (M = 35.759, F = 1.642, p < 0.035)—due to its overpower related to groups with equal size, as in this case [9,54]. Thus, the PA was carried out [42,60].

3.2. Preliminary Analyses: Assessment of General Alexithymia Levels among Groups

As an additional preliminary analysis, general levels of alexithymia were investigated among the three groups of individuals. ANOVA revealed statistically significant differences between the three groups: F = 6.839, p = 0.001, and η2p = 0.158 (large effect size). Furthermore, specific between-group comparisons were investigated. A statistically significant difference was found between IPVV (M = 40.24, SD = 11.238) and IPVP (M = 53.72, SD = 12.068): t = −3.606, p = 0.002, d = −1.07 (large effect size). Moreover, a statistically significant difference was found between the IPVP (M = 53.72, SD = 12.068) and the CG (M = 45.17, SD = 13.926): t = 2.501, p = 0.044, d = 0.68 (moderate effect size). Lastly, no statistically significant differences were found between IPVV (M = 40.24, SD = 11.238) and CG (M = 45.17, SD = 13.926): t = −1.372, p = 0.523, d = −0.39 (small effect size).

3.3. Profile Analysis: Parallelism

A statistically significant effect of the group of participants (IPVV vs. IPVP vs. CG) was found on variables related to IPV, revealing the presence of parallelism among profiles: Pillai’s V = 0.052, F = 0.637, p = 0.700, η2p = 0.026 (small effect size). Figure 1 represents the presence of parallelism.

3.4. Profile Analysis: Level Equality—Between-Group Differences

A statistically significant between-group effect was found: F = 6.552, p = 0.002. This result confirmed that the three groups were, on average, different.
The multivariate pairwise focused contrast between IPVV and IPVP showed a statistically significant multivariate effect: Pillai’s V = 0.268, F = 3.747, p = 0.011, η2p = 0.268 (large effect size). Moreover, multivariate pairwise focused contrast between the IPVV and the CG showed a non-statistically significant multivariate effect: Pillai’s V = 0.077, F = 0.959, p = 0.439, η2p = 0.077 (small effect size). Lastly, a multivariate pairwise focused contrast between IPVP and the CG showed a non-statistically significant multivariate effect: Pillai’s V = 0.125, F = 1.792, p = 0.145, η2p = 0.125 (moderate effect size). Specific between-group comparisons were reported in Table 2 and Figure 2.

3.5. Profile Analysis: Flatness—Within-Group Differences

A non-statistically significant within-group effect was found: F = 0.498, p = 0.783, η2p = 0.013 (small effect size).
Considering IPVV individuals, a non-statistically significant multivariate effect was found: Pillai’s V = 0.063, F = 0.401, p = 0.754, η2p = 0.063 (moderate effect size). Considering IPVP individuals, a non-statistically significant multivariate effect was found: Pillai’s V = 0.110, F = 905, p = 0.455, η2p = 0.110 (moderate effect size). Lastly, considering individuals from the CG, a non-statistically significant multivariate effect was found: Pillai’s V = 0.036, F = 0.341, p = 0.769, η2p = 0.036 (small effect size). Detailed results—bivariate comparisons—are reported in Table 3 and Figure 3.

4. Discussion

The objective of the present study was to investigate alexithymia and impulsivity in victims and perpetrators of IPV. To reach this objective, a profile analysis was conducted among three different samples of men: men who experienced partner victimization, male perpetrators, and men from the general population.
First, the results showed a statistically significant correlation between impulsivity and alexithymia. Consequently, people who struggle to identify and label their own and others’ feelings and emotions can use impulsive behaviors (such as impulsive aggression) to cope with these difficulties [12,14,26,29]. This is in line with previous literature on the matter, which shows that the ability of a person to identify and, then, categorize negative emotional experiences moderates the relationship between anger and aggressive behaviors [61].
Furthermore, the results confirmed our hypotheses, showing differences between IPVV and IPVP. Specifically, perpetrators had higher levels of impulsivity and alexithymia compared to victims. This is in line with previous literature showing that both impulsivity and alexithymia appear to be related to higher levels of aggression [27,29,62]. In this regard, the study by Garofalo and colleagues showed that emotion dysregulation and impulsivity mediated the relationship between alexithymia and aggression. Consequently, a person’s ability to regulate emotions and control impulsive behaviors seems to play a key role in the relationship between alexithymic difficulties and aggressive behavior [62]. Furthermore, alexithymia represents a potential risk factor for reoffending in intimate relationships and for dropping out of psychotherapeutic support [63]. Therefore, this result contributes to highlighting the importance of considering the role of alexithymia and impulsivity in understanding IPV and developing effective interventions for IPVP. The findings suggest that addressing these psychological constructs in the early stages of the psychological intervention with IPVP can play a key role in reducing aggression, improving psychotherapeutic outcomes, and reducing drop-out rates.
In addition, in line with our hypothesis, the between-group comparison showed statistically significant differences between IPVP and CG in alexithymia levels. Specifically, the perpetrators had higher levels of difficulty describing and identifying feelings and emotions. This aligns with previous research indicating that individuals with alexithymic traits can struggle with empathizing with others and understanding their feelings [9,64]. As a result, this may cause difficulties in their ability to control their behaviors, leading to more violent behaviors toward their intimate partners [47].
Several studies have pointed out that alexithymia is related to difficulties in interpersonal relationships, such as empathizing with others’ emotions [65,66], which, in this case, may contribute to further violence. It is important to note that IPV is often characterized by a cycle of violence, in which violence and abuse escalate over time [67]. This implies that the more perpetrators struggle to understand their partners’ emotions and express their own, the more likely they are to escalate their violent behaviors toward their partners. These findings highlight the importance of addressing alexithymic difficulties in the early stages of psychological interventions with IPVP. By doing so, it may be possible to prevent the cycle of violence from escalating and reduce the risk of reoffending. Contrary to expectations, the CG did not differ statistically significantly from the IPVP in the impulsivity level. This result may reflect the small sample size. In fact, the analysis resulted in a medium Cohen’s d value (d = 0.441), indicating a relationship between impulsivity and aggression. This suggests that individuals who struggle with mentalizing affective states may be more likely to use maladaptive strategies, such as aggressive behaviors [68,69].
In conclusion, our hypotheses were supported by the findings and are consistent with some of the literature (e.g., [9]), indicating that IPVV did not exhibit alexithymic difficulties. Additionally, their impulsivity levels were comparable to those of the CG. This outcome emphasizes that these psychological factors may not contribute to the experience of victimization; hence, they should not be the primary focus of interventions with victims of violence. Instead, interventions should primarily target perpetrators who have been found to have higher levels of alexithymia and impulsivity.

4.1. Implications for Practice

These results contribute to the suggestion that alexithymia should be evaluated and addressed in rehabilitation programs for violence perpetrators. In fact, the literature shows that perpetrators could benefit from training in emotional identification and labeling skills and anger management techniques [12,63]. Additionally, group therapy could be implemented in these cases, as patients with higher levels of alexithymia could perceive it as less demanding from an emotional and relational point of view [70,71]. Moreover, according to the results, impulsivity should also be addressed in clinical interventions with IPVP. For example, cognitive-behavioral therapy techniques such as self-control training, problem-solving skills, and coping skills appear to be one of the focuses of intervention [72,73].
In summary, psychological interventions for IPV perpetrators to be successful should target the perpetrators’ lack of ability to identify and express emotions and control their impulsive behaviors.

4.2. Limitations

Some limitations of this study must be acknowledged. First, a cross-sectional study design was implemented in this study. Although cross-sectional studies are widely used, they suffer from a number of flaws, such as simultaneous measurement of exposure and outcome. This often makes causality difficult to pin down. [74]. Second, the sample size was small, even though it was still sufficient for the statistical analysis conducted in the present manuscript.
In future studies, it would be beneficial to increase the sample size and examine the role of emotion regulation, in line with previous research. Additionally, it is recommended to conduct studies on a national level and include cross-cultural comparisons.

5. Conclusions

The present study aimed to investigate alexithymia and impulsivity in victims and perpetrators of IPV. A profile analysis was conducted. The results showed that IPVV presented levels of alexithymia and impulsivity comparable to the general population. In contrast, IPVP showed difficulties in identifying and describing emotions and higher levels of impulsivity. Therefore, these two psychological constructs may play a key role in violent behaviors and should be targeted in the clinical setting.

Author Contributions

S.M. conceived the study. S.M. collected the data. A.A.R. performed statistical analyses, displayed tables and figures, and wrote part of the first draft. F.T. wrote part of the first draft. S.M. provided important intellectual revisions. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was carried out according to the ethical standards of the University of Padua. All procedures performed in the study were carried out following the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and the Declaration of Helsinki and its subsequent amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

Due to privacy restrictions, the dataset used in the current study is available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Krug, G.E.; Dahlberg, L.L.; Mercy, A.J.; Zwi, B.A.; Lozano, R. World Report on Violence and Health; World Health Organization: Geneva, Switzerland, 2002. [Google Scholar]
  2. WHO. Global, Regional and National Prevalence Estimates for Intimate Partner Violence against Women and Global and Regional Prevalence Estimates for Non-Partner Sexual Violence against Women; World Health Organization: Geneva, Switzerland, 2021. [Google Scholar]
  3. Taccini, F.; Rossi, A.A.; Mannarini, S. Women’s EmotionS, Trauma and EmpowErMent (W-ES.T.EEM) study protocol: A psychoeducational support intervention for victims of domestic violence—A randomised controlled trial. BMJ Open 2022, 12, e060672. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Taccini, F.; Rossi, A.A.; Mannarini, S. Intergenerational Transmission of Relational Styles: Current Considerations. Front. Psychol. 2021, 12, 672961. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Mannarini, S.; Balottin, L.; Munari, C.; Gatta, M. Assessing conflict management in the couple: The definition of a latent dimension. Fam. J. 2017, 25, 13–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Taccini, F.; Mannarini, S. An Attempt to Conceptualize the Phenomenon of Stigma toward Intimate Partner Violence Survivors: A Systematic Review. Behav. Sci. 2023, 13, 194. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Kolbe, V.; Büttner, A. Domestic Violence Against Men-Prevalence and Risk Factors. Dtsch. Arztebl. Int. 2020, 117, 534–541. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  8. Trombetta, T.; Rollè, L. Intimate Partner Violence Perpetration Among Sexual Minority People and Associated Factors: A Systematic Review of Quantitative Studies. Sex. Res. Soc. Policy 2022. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Mannarini, S.; Taccini, F.; Rossi, A.A. Women and violence: Alexithymia, relational competence and styles, and satisfaction with life: A comparative profile analysis. Behav. Sci. 2021, 11, 147. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Tharp, A.T.; DeGue, S.; Valle, L.A.; Brookmeyer, K.A.; Massetti, G.M.; Matjasko, J.L. A systematic qualitative review of risk and protective factors for sexual violence perpetration. Trauma Violence Abus. 2013, 14, 133–167. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Bagby, R.M.; Parker, J.D.; Taylor, G.J. The twenty-item Toronto Alexithymia Scale—I. Item selection and cross-validation of the factor structure. J. Psychosom. Res. 1994, 38, 23–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Strickland, J.; Parry, C.L.; Allan, M.M.; Allan, A. Alexithymia among Perpetrators of Violent Offences in Australia: Implications for Rehabilitation. Aust. Psychol. 2017, 52, 230–237. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Mannarini, S.; Balottin, L.; Palmieri, A.; Carotenuto, F. Emotion regulation and parental bonding in families of adolescents with internalizing and externalizing symptoms. Front. Psychol. 2018, 9, 1493. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Hornsveld, R.H.J.; Kraaimaat, F.W. Alexithymia in Dutch violent forensic psychiatric outpatients. Psychol. Crime Law 2012, 18, 833–846. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Craparo, G.; Gori, A.; Petruccelli, I.; Cannella, V.; Simonelli, C. Intimate partner violence: Relationships between alexithymia, depression, attachment styles, and coping strategies of battered women. J. Sex. Med. 2014, 11, 1484–1494. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Yelsma, P. Affective Orientations of Perpetrators, Victims, and Functional Spouses. J. Interpers. Violence 2016, 11, 141–161. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Anuk, D.; Bahadır, G. The association of experience of violence and somatization, depression, and alexithymia: A sample of women with medically unexplained symptoms in Turkey. Arch. Women’s Ment. Health 2018, 21, 93–103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Signorelli, M.; Fusar-Poli, L.; Arcidiacono, E.; Caponnetto, P.; Aguglia, E. Depression, PTSD and alexithymia in victims of intimate partner violence: A case-control study. Arc. Clin. Psychiatr. 2020, 47, 45–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. De Rick, A.; Vanheule, S. Alexithymia and DSM-IV personality disorder traits in alcoholic inpatients: A study of the relation between both constructs. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2007, 43, 119–129. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Zimmermann, G. Delinquency in male adolescents: The role of alexithymia and family structure. J. Adolesc. 2006, 29, 321–332. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Lumley, M.A.; Neely, L.C.; Burger, A.J. The assessment of alexithymia in medical settings: Implications for understanding and treating health problems. J. Pers. Assess. 2007, 89, 230–246. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Shishido, H.; Gaher, R.M.; Simons, J.S. I don’t know how I feel, therefore I act: Alexithymia, urgency, and alcohol problems. Addict. Behav. 2013, 38, 2014–2017. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Herman, A.M.; Pilcher, N.; Duka, T. Deter the emotions: Alexithymia, impulsivity and their relationship to binge drinking. Addict. Behav. Rep. 2020, 12, 100308. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. Huang, S.; Zhu, Z.; Zhang, W.; Chen, Y.; Zhen, S. Trait impulsivity components correlate differently with proactive and reactive control. PLoS ONE 2017, 12, e0176102. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Stanford, M.S.; Mathias, C.W.; Dougherty, D.M.; Lake, S.L.; Anderson, N.E.; Patton, J.H. Fifty years of the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale: An update and review. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2009, 47, 385–395. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Moeller, F.G.; Barratt, E.S.; Dougherty, D.M.; Schmitz, J.M.; Swann, A.C. Psychiatric aspects of impulsivity. Am. J. Psychiatry 2001, 158, 1783–1793. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  27. Martin, S.; Zabala, C.; Del-Monte, J.; Graziani, P.; Aizpurua, E.; Barry, T.J.; Ricarte, J. Examining the relationships between impulsivity, aggression, and recidivism for prisoners with antisocial personality disorder. Aggress. Violent Behav. 2019, 49, 101314. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Lawrence, K.A.; Allen, J.S.; Chanen, A.M. Impulsivity in borderline personality disorder: Reward-based decision-making and its relationship to emotional distress. J. Personal. Disord. 2010, 24, 786–799. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  29. Swogger, M.T.; Walsh, Z.; Kosson, D.S. Domestic violence and psychopathic traits: Distinguishing the antisocial batterer from other antisocial offenders. Aggress. Behav. 2007, 33, 253–260. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Dolan, M.; Anderson, I.M.; Deakin, J.F.W. Relationship between 5-HT function and impulsivity and aggression in male offenders with personality disorders. Br. J. Psychiatry 2001, 178, 352–359. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Stanford, M.S.; Houston, R.J.; Baldridge, R.M. Comparison of impulsive and premeditated perpetrators of intimate partner violence. Behav. Sci. Law 2008, 26, 709–722. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Helfritz, L.E.; Stanford, M.S.; Conklin, S.M.; Greve, K.W.; Villemarette-Pittman, N.R.; Houston, R.J. Usefulness of Self-Report Instruments in Assessing Men Accused of Domestic Violence. Psychol. Rec. 2006, 56, 171–180. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Tharp, A.T.; Schumacher, J.A.; Samper, R.E.; McLeish, A.C.; Coffey, S.F. Relative Importance of Emotional Dysregulation, Hostility, and Impulsiveness in Predicting Intimate Partner Violence Perpetrated by Men in Alcohol Treatment. Psychol. Women Q. 2013, 37, 51–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  34. Di Piazza, L.; Kowal, C.; Hodiaumont, F.; Blavier, A. Intimate partner violence: Perseverance vs Dropout from treatment programs targeting male batterers. In Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Belgian Association for Psychological Science, Anvers, Belgium, 24 May 2016. [Google Scholar]
  35. Stuart, G.L.; Holtzworth-Munroe, A. Testing a Theoretical Model of the Relationship Between Impulsivity, Mediating Variables, and Husband Violence. J. Fam. Violence 2005, 20, 291–303. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Molero Jurado, M.D.M.; Pérez-Fuentes, M.D.C.; Simón Márquez, M.D.M.; Barragán Martín, A.B.; Sisto, M.; Gázquez Linares, J.J. Relationship Between Impulsivity, Sensation-Seeking, and Drug Use in Aggressors and Victims of Violence. Front. Psychol. 2020, 11, 600055. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  37. Mullet, N.; Hawkins, L.G.; Tuliao, A.P.; Snyder, H.; Holyoak, D.; McGuire, K.C.; Earl, A.K.; McChargue, D. Early Trauma and Later Sexual Victimization in College Women: A Multiple Mediation Examination of Alexithymia, Impulsivity, and Alcohol Use. J. Interpers. Violence 2022, 37, Np18194–Np18214. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  38. Messman-Moore, T.L.; Ward, R.M.; Zerubavel, N. The role of substance use and emotion dysregulation in predicting risk for incapacitated sexual revictimization in women: Results of a prospective investigation. Psychol. Addict. Behav. 2013, 27, 125–132. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  39. Faul, F.; Erdfelder, E.; Buchner, A.; Lang, A.G. Statistical power analyses using G*Power 3.1: Tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behav. Res. Methods 2009, 41, 1149–1160. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Bulut, O.; Desjardins, C.D. Profile Analysis of Multivariate Data: A Brief Introduction to the ProfileR Package; University of Minnesota: Minneapolis, MN, USA, 2020; pp. 1–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Schmit, M.K.; Oller, M.L.; Tapia-Fuselier, J.L.; Schmit, E.L. A Holistic Client Functioning Profile Comparison of People with Serious Mental Illness. J. Couns. Dev. 2020, 98, 3–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Tabachnick, B.G.; Fidell, L.S. Using Multivariate Statistics; Pearson: Harlow, UK, 2014. [Google Scholar]
  43. Cohen, J. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences; Lawrence Erlbaum Associates: Mahwah, NJ, USA, 1988. [Google Scholar]
  44. Pillai, K.C.S. Some New Test Criteria in Multivariate Analysis. Ann. Math. Statist. 1955, 26, 117–121. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Eid, M.; Gollwitzer, M.; Schmitt, M. Statistik und Forschungsmethoden, 5th ed.; Beltz: Basel, Switzerland, 2017. [Google Scholar]
  46. Bressi, C.; Taylor, G.; Parker, J.; Bressi, S.; Brambilla, V.; Aguglia, E.; Allegranti, I.; Bongiorno, A.; Giberti, F.; Bucca, M.; et al. Cross validation of the factor structure of the 20-item Toronto Alexithymia Scale: An Italian multicenter study. J. Psychosom. Res. 1996, 41, 551–559. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Leshem, R.; van Lieshout, P.H.H.M.; Ben-David, S.; Ben-David, B.M. Does emotion matter? The role of alexithymia in violent recidivism: A systematic literature review. Crim. Behav. Ment. Health 2019, 29, 94–110. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Fossati, A.; Di Ceglie, A.; Acquarini, E.; Barratt, E.S. Psychometric properties of an Italian version of the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale-11 (BIS-11) in nonclinical subjects. J. Clin. Psychol. 2001, 57, 815–828. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  49. Patton, J.H.; Stanford, M.S.; Barratt, E.S. Factor structure of the barratt impulsiveness scale. J. Clin. Psychol. 1995, 51, 768–774. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  50. Anderson, D. esvis: Visualization and Estimation of Effect Sizes; Version 0.3.1; 2020. Available online: https://github.com/datalorax/esvis (accessed on 27 April 2023).
  51. Wickham, H. ggplot2: Elegant Graphics for Data Analysis; Springer: New York, NY, USA, 2016. [Google Scholar]
  52. Revelle, W. psych: Procedures for Personality and Psychological Research; Northwestern University: Evanston, IL, USA, 2018. [Google Scholar]
  53. Wickham, H.; Averick, M.; Bryan, J.; Chang, W.; McGowan, L.D.; François, R.; Grolemund, G.; Hayes, A.; Henry, L.; Hester, J.; et al. Welcome to the tidyverse. J. Open Source Softw. 2019, 4, 1686. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Rossi, A.A.; Pietrabissa, G.; Tagliagambe, A.; Scuderi, A.; Montecchiani, L.; Castelnuovo, G.; Mannarini, S.; Dalla Ragione, L. Many facets of eating disorders: Profiling key psychological features of anorexia nervosa and binge eating disorder. Behav. Sci. 2023, 13, 276. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Bulut, O.; Davison, M.L.; Rodriguez, M.C. Estimating Between-Person and Within-Person Subscore Reliability with Profile Analysis. Multivar. Behav. Res. 2017, 52, 86–104. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  56. Bulut, O.; Desjardins, C.D. profileR: Profile Analysis of Multivariate Data in R; R package version 0.3-5; 2018. Available online: https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/profileR/index.html (accessed on 27 April 2023).
  57. Davison, M.L.; Davenport, E.C.J. Identifying criterion-related patterns of predictor scores using multiple regression. Psychol. Methods 2002, 7, 468–484. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  58. Schmit, M.K.; Watson, J.C.; Schmit, E.L. Using Profile Analysis in Counseling Outcome Research. Couns. Outcome Res. Eval. 2019, 10, 94–105. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Watkins, M.W.; Glutting, J.J.; Youngstrom, E.A. Issues in Subtest Profile Analysis. In Contemporary Intellectual Assessment: Theories, Tests, and Issues; The Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 2005; pp. 251–268. [Google Scholar]
  60. Howell, D.C. Statistical Methods for Psychology; Wadsworth, Cengage Learning: Belmont, CA, USA, 2013. [Google Scholar]
  61. Pond Jr, R.S.; Kashdan, T.B.; DeWall, C.N.; Savostyanova, A.; Lambert, N.M.; Fincham, F.D. Emotion differentiation moderates aggressive tendencies in angry people: A daily diary analysis. Emotion 2012, 12, 326–337. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Garofalo, C.; Velotti, P.; Zavattini, G.C. Emotion regulation and aggression: The incremental contribution of alexithymia, impulsivity, and emotion dysregulation facets. Psychol. Violence 2018, 8, 470–483. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Romero-Martínez, Á.; Lila, M.; Martínez, M.; Pedrón-Rico, V.; Moya-Albiol, L. Improvements in Empathy and Cognitive Flexibility after Court-Mandated Intervention Program in Intimate Partner Violence Perpetrators: The Role of Alcohol Abuse. Int. J. Env. Res. Public Health 2016, 13, 394. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Louth, S.M.; Hare, R.D.; Linden, W. Psychopathy and alexithymia in female offenders. Can. J. Behav. Sci./Rev. Can. Des Sci. Du Comport. 1998, 30, 91–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Zarei, J.; Besharat, M.a. Alexithymia and interpersonal problems. Procedia -Soc. Behav. Sci. 2010, 5, 619–622. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Valdespino, A.; Antezana, L.; Ghane, M.; Richey, J.A. Alexithymia as a Transdiagnostic Precursor to Empathy Abnormalities: The Functional Role of the Insula. Front Psychol 2017, 8, 2234. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  67. Walker, L. Battered woman syndrome: Empirical findings. Ann. NY Acad. Sci. 2006, 1087, 142–157. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  68. Fossati, A.; Acquarini, E.; Feeney, J.A.; Borroni, S.; Grazioli, F.; Giarolli, L.E.; Franciosi, G.; Maffei, C. Alexithymia and attachment insecurities in impulsive aggression. Attach. Hum. Dev. 2009, 11, 165–182. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Bateman, A.; Fonagy, P. Mentalization-based treatment. Psychoanal. Inq. 2013, 33, 595–613. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Cameron, K.; Ogrodniczuk, J.; Hadjipavlou, G. Changes in Alexithymia Following Psychological Intervention: A Review. Harv. Rev. Psychiatry 2014, 22, 162–178. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Mannarini, S. A method for the definition of a self-awareness behavior dimension with clinical subjects: A latent trait analysis. Behav. Res. Methods 2009, 41, 1029–1037. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Shorey, R.C.; Brasfield, H.; Febres, J.; Stuart, G.L. The association between impulsivity, trait anger, and the perpetration of intimate partner and general violence among women arrested for domestic violence. J. Interpers. Violence 2011, 26, 2681–2697. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  73. Mannarini, S.; Boffo, M.; Balottin, L. Beliefs about the patient’s role in the psychotherapeutic relationship: A latent trait perspective. TPM Test Psychom. Methodol. Appl. Psychol. 2013, 20, 277–294. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  74. Wang, X.; Cheng, Z. Cross-Sectional Studies: Strengths, Weaknesses, and Recommendations. Chest 2020, 158, S65–S71. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Figure 1. Plot of the profile analysis.
Figure 1. Plot of the profile analysis.
Behavsci 13 00402 g001
Figure 2. Between-group means comparison. Values are expressed as z-scores. Note: Independent samples of Cohen’s ds were reported.
Figure 2. Between-group means comparison. Values are expressed as z-scores. Note: Independent samples of Cohen’s ds were reported.
Behavsci 13 00402 g002
Figure 3. Between-group means comparison. Values are expressed as z-scores. Note: Dependent samples of Cohen’s ds were reported.
Figure 3. Between-group means comparison. Values are expressed as z-scores. Note: Dependent samples of Cohen’s ds were reported.
Behavsci 13 00402 g003
Table 1. Descriptive statistics of questionnaires (row scores) and correlations between variables.
Table 1. Descriptive statistics of questionnaires (row scores) and correlations between variables.
Descriptive StatisticsCorrelationsCollinearity
MSDSkK12345TVIF
1TAS-20 46.6213.5760.391−0.696- --
2DIF15.667.0510.698−0.3110.896 ***- 0.4072.460
3DDF13.094.7810.399−0.5460.856 ***0.723 ***- 0.4622.164
4EOT17.874.7870.185−0.3380.681 ***0.347 **0.363 **- 0.8471.181
5BIS-1159.539.700−0.030−0.3410.530 ***0.548 ***0.433 ***0.264 *-0.6921.445
Note: * p < 0.050; ** p < 0.010; *** p < 0.001. M = mean; SD = standard deviation; Sk = skewness; K = kurtosis; T = tolerance; VIF = Variance Inflaction Factor. TAS-20 = TAS-20 Total score; DIF = difficulties in identifying feelings; DDF = difficulties in describing feelings to other people; EOT = externally oriented thinking; BIS-11 = Barratt Impulsiveness Scale-11.
Table 2. Between-group comparisons.
Table 2. Between-group comparisons.
IPVVIPVPCGIPVV vs. IPVPIPVP vs. CGIPVV vs. CG
MSDMSDMSDtdtdtd
1DIF13.336.62119.165.64014.377.472−2.946 *−0.8722.649 *0.717−0.544−0.155
2DDF10.863.22915.084.34913.005.408−3.136 **−0.9281.689 *0.457−1.656−0.471
3EOT16.053.82719.484.47317.805.288−2.489 *−0.7371.3320.361−1.322−0.376
4BIS-1157.2910.06162.6810.00758.478.792−1.907−0.5641.6280.441−0.434−0.124
Note: * pbonf < 0.050; ** pbonf < 0.010; IPVV = IPV victims; IPVP = IPV perpetrators; CG = Control group; M = mean; SD = standard deviation; t = independent sample t-test; d = Cohen’s d (effect size); DIF = difficulties in identifying feelings; DDF = difficulties in describing feelings to other people; EOT = externally oriented thinking; BIS-11 = Barratt Impulsiveness Scale-11.
Table 3. Within-group comparisons.
Table 3. Within-group comparisons.
IPVVIPVPCG
tdtdtd
DIFDDF0.5950.1580.4470.088−0.767−0.155
EOT0.2190.0580.8840.173−0.790−0.160
BIS-11−0.426−0.1130.9480.186−0.346−0.070
DDFEOT−0.376−0.1000.4370.086−0.023−0.005
BIS-11−1.020−0.2710.5010.0980.4210.085
EOTBIS-11−0.644−0.1710.0640.0130.4440.090
Note: IPVV = IPV victims; IPVP = IPV perpetrators; CG = Control group; M = mean; SD = standard deviation; t = dependent sample t-test; d = Cohen’s d for paired sample (effect size); DIF = difficulties in identifying feelings; DDF = difficulties in describing feelings to other people; EOT = externally oriented thinking; BIS-11 = Barratt Impulsiveness Scale-11.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Mannarini, S.; Taccini, F.; Rossi, A.A. The Role of Alexithymia and Impulsivity in Male Victims and Perpetrators of Intimate Partner Violence. Behav. Sci. 2023, 13, 402. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs13050402

AMA Style

Mannarini S, Taccini F, Rossi AA. The Role of Alexithymia and Impulsivity in Male Victims and Perpetrators of Intimate Partner Violence. Behavioral Sciences. 2023; 13(5):402. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs13050402

Chicago/Turabian Style

Mannarini, Stefania, Federica Taccini, and Alessandro Alberto Rossi. 2023. "The Role of Alexithymia and Impulsivity in Male Victims and Perpetrators of Intimate Partner Violence" Behavioral Sciences 13, no. 5: 402. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs13050402

APA Style

Mannarini, S., Taccini, F., & Rossi, A. A. (2023). The Role of Alexithymia and Impulsivity in Male Victims and Perpetrators of Intimate Partner Violence. Behavioral Sciences, 13(5), 402. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs13050402

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop