Next Article in Journal
Canine Gallbladder Erosion/Ulcer and Hemocholecyst: Clinicopathological Characteristics of 14 Cases
Previous Article in Journal
DJAN: Deep Joint Adaptation Network for Wildlife Image Recognition
Previous Article in Special Issue
Attitudes towards the Potential Use of Aversive Geofencing Devices to Manage Wild Elephant Movement
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Testing the Effectiveness of the “Smelly” Elephant Repellent in Controlled Experiments in Semi-Captive Asian and African Savanna Elephants

1
WildAid, 220 Montgomery Street #1200, San Francisco, CA 94104, USA
2
Game Rangers International, Plot 2374, The Village, Leopards Hill Road, Lusaka 10101, Zambia
3
Golden Triangle Asian Elephant Foundation, 229 Moo 1, Chiang Saen, Chiang Rai 57150, Thailand
4
Department of National Parks and Wildlife, Ministry of Tourism, Chilanga 10101, Zambia
5
Department for Psychosomatic Medicine and Psychotherapy, University for Continuing Education Krems, 3500 Krems an der Donau, Austria
6
Amboseli Trust for Elephants, Langata, Nairobi 15135, Kenya
7
Durrell Institute of Conservation and Ecology, University of Kent, Canterbury CT2 7NZ, UK
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Joint senior authorship.
Animals 2023, 13(21), 3334; https://doi.org/10.3390/ani13213334
Submission received: 31 August 2023 / Revised: 12 October 2023 / Accepted: 19 October 2023 / Published: 26 October 2023
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Conservation Behavior Approaches to Elephant Management and Welfare)

Abstract

:

Simple Summary

Mitigating and reducing the impacts of elephant crop-raiding has become a major focus of conservation intervention. By observing the behaviour amongst two groups of semi-captive African and Asian elephants in Zambia and Thailand, we found that a novel olfactory crop-raiding mitigation method called the “smelly elephant repellent” elicited clear reactions from the elephants. However, unlike trials with wild elephants, the repellent did not prevent the elephants from entering areas or eating food protected by the solution. We found that elephant personality played a role in responses towards the repellent, as the individuals that entered the experimental plots were bolder and more curious individuals. Although captive environments provide controlled settings for experimental testing, the ecological validity of testing human–elephant conflict mitigation methods with captive wildlife should be strongly considered. Understanding animal behaviour is essential for improving human–elephant coexistence and for designing deterrence mechanisms, and the smelly elephant repellent may be a useful mitigation method when used in combination with other methods.

Abstract

Crop-raiding by elephants is one of the most prevalent forms of human–elephant conflict and is increasing with the spread of agriculture into wildlife range areas. As the magnitude of conflicts between people and elephants increases across Africa and Asia, mitigating and reducing the impacts of elephant crop-raiding has become a major focus of conservation intervention. In this study, we tested the responses of semi-captive elephants to the “smelly” elephant repellent, a novel olfactory crop-raiding mitigation method. At two trial sites, in Zambia and Thailand, African elephants (Loxodonta africana) and Asian elephants (Elephas maximus) were exposed to the repellent, in order to test whether or not they entered an area protected by the repellent and whether they ate the food provided. The repellent elicited clear reactions from both study groups of elephants compared to control conditions. Generalised linear models revealed that the elephants were more alert, sniffed more, and vocalised more when they encountered the repellent. Although the repellent triggered a response, it did not prevent elephants from entering plots protected by the repellent or from eating crops, unlike in trials conducted with wild elephants. Personality played a role in responses towards the repellent, as the elephants that entered the experimental plots were bolder and more curious individuals. We conclude that, although captive environments provide controlled settings for experimental testing, the ecological validity of testing human–elephant conflict mitigation methods with captive wildlife should be strongly considered. This study also shows that understanding animal behaviour is essential for improving human–elephant coexistence and for designing deterrence mechanisms. Appreciating personality traits in elephants, especially amongst “problem” elephants who have a greater propensity to crop raid, could lead to the design of new mitigation methods designed to target these individuals.

1. Introduction

Human expansion and habitat conversion have led to a decline in African and Asian elephant ecosystems [1,2]. This land use change has increased interactions and resource competition between humans and elephants [3]. The resultant human–elephant conflict is one of the greatest challenges facing conservation today, due to the significant costs incurred for both people and elephants [4,5,6].
Human–elephant conflict can have negative impacts on people through loss of crops, damage to property, loss of livestock, and, in some cases, human injury or loss of life [6,7]. This can have detrimental impacts on the food security of already impoverished farmers and may be increasingly compounded by climate change [8]. Indirect costs can also occur, including restriction of movements, disruption to daily routines, fear, and sleepless nights [9]. These impacts can play a significant role in eroding tolerance towards elephants, leading to retaliatory killing [10,11], impacting elephant populations and hampering wildlife conservation efforts [12,13].
Crop-raiding by elephants is one of the most prevalent forms of human–elephant conflict and is increasing with the spread of agriculture into wildlife range areas [14,15]. It is well documented that, at certain times of the year, the nutritional value of cultivated food crops is greater than that of natural forage [16,17,18,19], which is attractive to elephants [20]. However, crop-raiding poses a significant risk to elephants through attacks from farmers [11], and so elephants must weigh up the risk versus the nutritional gain from the crops. Due to this high-risk, high-gain strategy, in some areas, only male elephants have been reported to crop-raid [18,19,21,22]. This risk-taking behaviour of males could have evolved as a result of strong sexual selection for large body size and condition-dependent mating success in males [23]. Crop-raiding can lead to gains in body size for elephants, as cultivated food crops are highly nutritious [23]. Risk assessment behaviour has been documented in elephants around humans, as they move faster through dangerous areas where there is a risk of mortality [24,25], and they move at night when there is less risk [25,26].
As the magnitude of conflicts between people and elephants increases across Africa and Asia, mitigating and reducing the impacts of elephant crop-raiding has become a major focus of conservation intervention. A variety of short-term solutions have been developed, which include physical, visual, and acoustic interventions that deter elephants from entering farms and/or eating crops. Traditional, low-cost strategies include burning fires, using dogs to alert to elephant presence, and noise making, such as banging pots and pans and shouting [14]. More recently developed methods include wire, electric, chilli and honeybee fencing [14], metal strip fences [27], trenches, buffer cropping, sirens, predator playbacks, strobe lighting [28], soft virtual boundaries [29], and the smelly elephant repellent [30]. These strategies are often selected by farmers based on their affordability, accessibility, and traditional effectiveness [14]. However, effectiveness varies widely [14,31] and may depend on the landscape and variation in elephant behaviour and cognition [32]. This is of particular concern, as some individual elephants are learning to circumvent conflict mitigation strategies such as electric fences [33,34]. Given the high cognitive capabilities of elephants, including their ability to alter their behaviour in response to risk, understanding elephant behaviour is key to designing mitigation methods [35].
By taking into account an elephant’s perspective, we may be able to design more effective mitigation methods [35]. For example, elephants have an excellent sense of smell [36] and use olfaction to navigate their world, for foraging, and for social decision making [37,38]. The knowledge of this key sensory perception has led to more olfactory-based mitigation methods being designed [39]. These include: (1) using scent to mask the smell of ripening crops [40]; (2) using the chemical compound capsaicin in chilli, as chilli can cause irritation to elephants’ eyes and noses and stimulate olfactory receptors [41,42]; and (3) using a smell that elephants are averse to, e.g., bee pheromones [43] or predator scents [44].
The use of chilli pepper (Capsicum spp.) is one of the most widely tested olfactory-based mitigation methods. Various studies have trialled chilli-based methods, including fences of chilli oil-soaked cloths and chilli briquettes [41,45,46,47]. Chilli has been effective in some contexts (e.g., [48]), but in others has shown a low efficiency when compared to easier and cheaper methods such as community guarding [49]. Despite the positive impact that chilli can play in mitigating elephant damage to crops, the long-term sustainability of this method has been questioned due to the difficulty, expense, and labour required for its application.
Another olfactory-based mitigation method that includes chilli in its ingredients is the “smelly” elephant repellent, also known as the “smelly repellent” or just “repellent”, which is a novel foul-smelling liquid made of common natural ingredients [50]. The ingredients include chilli, garlic, ginger, neem leaves, cooking oil, dung, and rotten eggs. After the preparation of the solution, the mixture is left to mature for a strong, unpleasant odour to develop. This method was developed in Uganda and initially showed positive results as a domestic animal repellent, before being utilised as an olfactory deterrent for elephants. In trials with wild elephants, the smelly elephant repellent was sprayed directly onto crops or put into perforated bottles hung on a rudimentary fence around a crop field.
The smelly repellent showed high levels of effectiveness in trials on 40 farms in Uganda and Kenya. In Uganda, 82% of 309 attempted elephant crop-raiding incidents recorded at 30 farms on the northern boundary of Murchison Falls National Park were deterred. That is to say that elephants approached the farms and could have crop-raided, but were sufficiently put off by the smelly repellent so as to retreat without eating. In Kenya, the repellent deterred 63% of 24 attempted elephant crop-raiding incidents at 10 farms in the Tsavo Conservation Area, and there was a significant effect of the repellent on test sites compared to control sites [30]. The study highlighted the potential for the repellent to be a helpful crop-raiding mitigation tool for farmers, as the community also responded positively to using it. Moreover, the repellent is relatively cheap and quick to produce from ingredients readily available in most countries that elephants inhabit. It is important that mitigation methods are cheap, effective, and have community buy-in, as without this, uptake will not be successful [14,45,51,52].
Following the trials with wild African elephants, the next step was to test the smelly repellent on Asian elephants to determine the effectiveness and potential for use of this method in Asia. Given the caveats and challenges of conducting trials in the wild, such as the length of time required to gather statistically sound datasets and the impossibility of ruling out other deterring factors, we decided to carry out the trials with semi-captive elephants. Here, it was easier to control the environment and be able to fully observe the elephant behaviour when exposed to the repellent. Utilising two groups of semi-captive elephants, one in Asia and one in Africa, also gave us the chance to determine whether reactions to the repellent were species reactions or captive elephant reactions. Furthermore, as described above, individual learning and behaviour may also affect crop-raiding behaviours, and it is important to consider this for mitigation efforts [29,35]. Therefore, a captive setting also allowed for the testing of individual elephant traits, which may contribute to crop-raiding.
In this study, we aimed to determine the effectiveness of the smelly elephant repellent as an elephant crop-raiding deterrent. We presented semi-captive African and Asian elephants with a foraging opportunity with either the smelly repellent or a control condition (water) and recorded their behavioural responses. Additionally, personality traits were measured from each individual and compared with their crop-raiding behaviours. Based on a previous study with wild elephants [30], we predicted that elephants would be more likely to show a behavioural response, less likely to enter the plot, and less likely to eat in the repellent trials than in the control trials. We also predicted that personality would have an effect on likelihood to enter the plot and likelihood to eat the food in the repellent trials, but this was explorative to better understand these little-researched effects, and so we did not make specific predictions regarding the directionality of the results.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Study Sites

The smelly elephant repellent was tested at two sites between February and September 2021.

2.1.1. Thailand

The first site was at the Golden Triangle Asian Elephant Foundation (GTAEF), located in Chiang Saen in northern Thailand. The facility was home to 26 elephants that were rescued from the streets of Bangkok, the logging industry, or had been transferred from tourist trekking camps. The elephants are used as part of the elephant camp programmes at the Anantara Golden Triangle Elephant Camp and Resort and the Four Seasons Golden Triangle Tented Camp. The elephants are provided with access to natural habitat at all times, with artificial shelter provided during extreme weather or veterinary observation. They are fed from four to seven times a day on natural grasses and fruits and bathed two or three times a day. The elephant’s mahout (the daily caretaker who is often also the elephant’s owner), two full-time staff veterinarians, and senior management provide daily care and ensure that proper elephant welfare practice is in place. Details of the elephants that participated in this study can be found in Appendix A (Table A1).

2.1.2. Zambia

The second test site was at the Game Rangers International’s (GRI) Kafue Release Facility, located in Kafue National Park, western Zambia. GRI works in close partnership with the Department of National Parks and Wildlife (DNPW) to empower rangers and communities to conserve nature through natural resource protection, community outreach, and wildlife rescue. The wildlife rescue programme focuses on the rescue, rehabilitation, and release of elephants orphaned as a result of human–elephant conflict and poaching. The release facility at Kafue is the final stage of rehabilitation, where the aim is to provide the elephants with the most natural environment possible, in order that, over time, they gradually become competent and confident enough to live in the wild without the support of humans. The younger elephants spend their nights in a predator-proof area with shelter to ensure their safety. Once elephants are considered physically capable of defending themselves against predators, they are encouraged to remain outside of the outer boma (enclosure). Elephants are only encouraged to the outer boma for two hours over lunchtime, where they have access to pellets (for additional nutrition), have pools for drinking, and mud-bathing areas. The elephants are escorted on daily walks in the national park by elephant keepers and armed wildlife rangers. To prepare them for a life in the wild, during these walks, the orphaned elephants decide where they walk and feed, and if they interact with wild elephants. Details of the elephants that participated in this study can be found in Appendix A (Table A2).

2.2. Smelly Elephant Repellent

The smelly elephant repellent is made of a mixture of natural ingredients that are cooked together, matured, and then strained, producing a potent smelly liquid. The ingredients (chilli, garlic, ginger, cooking oil, eggs, neem leaves, and dung) were purchased from nearby towns or collected locally, and were pummelled before being cooked together and left to mature in sealed containers for approximately four weeks [50]. The production process of the repellent for both studies was conducted on-site by the GTAEF and GRI teams, and a previous study found that the scent of the repellent lasted for around 7–8 weeks before fading in wild conditions [30].

2.3. Trials and Data Collection

2.3.1. Thailand

In Thailand, we replicated a typical “farm” set up, using a 10 × 10 m plot of land in the grasslands of the organisation’s elephant camp. The “crop” was maize cobs or pieces of sugarcane distributed at roughly 1 m intervals around the edge of the plot (reachable from outside the fence in the fence conditions), another cob or cane layer further in, and at least two large piles of maize or sugarcane in the centre of the field. This distribution was to make the crop obvious to the elephants and replicate their normal feeding context. The plot was used for four different test and control conditions (Figure 1):
  • Repellent fence: The test fence with the repellent in plastic bottles. A fence line using rope was erected using 4–8 poles. Plastic bottles were hung onto the fence line at a distance of 1 m apart. The bottles were filled to roughly a quarter full with repellent and had holes punctured in them to allow the smell of the repellent to diffuse.
  • Water fence: The control fence with water in plastic bottles. The same process as above was applied, however, the bottles were quarter filled with water instead of repellent.
  • Repellent spray: The test crops were sprayed with repellent. The fence line around the plot was removed and the crops in the plot were dipped in the repellent. The ground was not sprayed so that the same plot could be used for each scenario.
  • Water spray: The control crops were sprayed with water. The crops in the unfenced plot were dipped in water.
Figure 1. Set up of the four different trial situations in Thailand.
Figure 1. Set up of the four different trial situations in Thailand.
Animals 13 03334 g001aAnimals 13 03334 g001b
For these trials, 13 female elephants in five groups of two to three individuals, based on long-term social bonds, were tested. Prior to testing, each elephant group was taken to the plot without any repellent or water in order for them to learn that food was available in this location. Each group was tested on each condition four times, meaning that the total number of trials across the different conditions was 12 for each elephant. The elephants were only tested on one condition per day, and the order of the condition type order was randomised.
Each trial was a maximum of 30 min long, and for each trial, the elephants were released into the grassland approximately 30 m from the plot. Distance markers were set up to allow the observer to measure how close the elephants got to the plot. An observer filmed the trial from behind the plot to record the reactions of the elephants to the fence/sprayed crops. The observer also recorded the following: (1) the behavioural reaction of the elephants to the plot (see below); (2) whether the elephants broke through the fence and entered the plot; and (3) whether the elephants ate the food. We observed and recorded the following behaviours: 1. Sniffing, 2. Alertness, 3. Vocalising, 4. Approaching the plot straight away, 5. Turning away from the plot immediately, 6. Strong reaction, 7. Did nothing, and 8. Any other behaviour of note. See Table A3 in the Appendix B for behaviour definitions.

2.3.2. Zambia

In Zambia, a different set up was used to fit the conditions of the facility. The orphaned elephants return from the bush to their boma (enclosure) every lunchtime and evening. Their diet is supplemented with browse and elephant pellets when they return, and this is usually spread out throughout the boma in different places every day so that the elephants learn to use their sense of smell to find it. We replicated a farm scenario using a 40 × 40 m plot of land inside the elephant boma, with the “crop” being pellets. The same number of cups of pellets as the elephants would usually have access to (n = 15) were placed inside the “farm” test area in the boma. A “no choice” condition, where pellets were only provided inside the test area, and not elsewhere in the boma, was also tested. The intention here was to create a highly motivating condition, much like crop-raiding in the wild, and so putting the repellent to an even stronger test. The 40 × 40 m farm plot was used for four different test and control conditions (Figure 2):
  • Repellent choice: The test pellets were sprayed with repellent and there was the usual choice of browse/pellets in the boma.
  • Water choice: The control pellets were sprayed with water and there was the usual choice of browse/pellets in the boma.
  • Repellent no-choice: The test pellets were sprayed with repellent and there was no choice of browse/pellets in the boma.
  • Water no-choice: The control pellets were sprayed with water and there was no choice of browse/pellets in the boma.
Figure 2. Set up of the four different trial situations in Zambia.
Figure 2. Set up of the four different trial situations in Zambia.
Animals 13 03334 g002
For each trial, up to 13 elephants entered the boma approximately 160 m from the test/control area. Marks on the ground allowed the observers to measure how close the elephants got to the 40 × 40 m farm plot. The elephants were given the entire duration that they were in the boma—approximately 2 h—for each trial. An observer filmed each session from the research tower, GoPro cameras were placed along the fence, still photographs were captured, and a minimum of two observers filled out a sheet recording the different reactions of the elephants to the pellets, specifically recording the same factors as those in the Thailand trial.

2.3.3. Personality Assessment

Elephant personality was assessed with the Elephant Personality Survey [53]. Two raters from each site, who had been working with the elephants for at least three years (Thailand: five and 23 years; Zambia: three and 14 years), rated the traits for each of the elephants across three domains: interactions with the physical environment; interactions with other elephants, and interactions with humans. Each trait was scored on a seven-point Likert scale (e.g., extremely shy, quite shy, slightly shy, neutral, slightly bold, quite bold, and extremely bold). We then selected seven traits to analyse in comparison with the behaviour in the trials: interactions with the physical environment—curiosity, confidence, playfulness, and fearless/timid, and interactions with humans—curiosity, aggressiveness, and shy/bold. These traits were chosen because studies have shown that bolder individuals are more likely to engage in human–animal conflict [54,55]. The scores from the Zambia sample were less varied than those from the Thai sample, and so to rule out rater effects, two further raters completed the survey and inter-rater reliability was assessed across all four raters for this site. Inter-rater reliability was assessed with the intraclass correlation coefficient using the irr package in R. Significant reliability was reached across all traits in both sites (p-values < 0.05), with ICC values ranging from 0.4 to 1.0. When the scores for an individual differed, the mean score was taken for the analyses.

2.3.4. Data Coding and Statistical Analysis

In Thailand, the data were collected live during the session by one observer, and 48% of the videos (n = 33) were also coded by a second coder (R.D.). In Zambia, the data were collected live during the session by two coders. The behavioural responses during the trials were coded categorically (yes/no) for occurrence across the whole trial.
To analyse the effects of condition and personality on the various behaviours, generalised linear models were run using the ‘glmer’ function in the lme4 package in R version 4.1.2 with the binomial family and backward elimination [56]. The full models were compared to the null models, without condition or personality included, to assess the effects of condition/personality on each dependent variable. The following construct depicts the basic model used for all the GLMMs:
Responseijk ~ conditioni + animalij + eijk
In this model example, “conditioni” is the fixed categorical effect of condition, “animalij” is the random animal (or animal within group for Thailand) effect with mean zero, and “eijk” is the random residual with mean zero.
For assessing personality traits, only data from the repellent trials were used and the models were split into two groups, such that the traits related to interactions with the physical environment were analysed together and interactions with humans were analysed together. For the personality models, sex was also included as a factor for the Zambia sample to control for this (in Thailand all elephants were female). For all the models, elephant ID and group were included as random factors for Thailand and only elephant ID was included as a random factor for the Zambia sample, as all individuals were part of the same group.

3. Results

3.1. Results

3.1.1. Thailand

In Thailand, n = 13 elephants participated in the trials (Appendix B, Table A3), with each trial lasting for 30 min. The results for the effects of the repellent fence, water fence, repellent spray, and water spray conditions on the elephant behaviour in the Thailand trial are presented in Table 1.
Higher likelihoods of sniffing (e.g., spray condition: 60% more likely), alertness (spray: 21% more likely), and vocalisations (spray: 31% more likely) were observed in the two repellent conditions compared to the two water conditions. The elephants were also less likely to approach the plot immediately and approached the plot and then turned away more often in the repellent conditions as compared to the water conditions. However, the repellent did not affect whether they ultimately entered the plot over the course of the whole session, but having a fence did, with the elephants being slightly less likely to enter in the two fence conditions than the spray conditions (e.g., 20% less likely to enter with the repellent fence than repellent spray). Having said that, the repellent spray did somewhat deter them from eating after entering (26% less likely to eat in repellent spray than water spray).

3.1.2. Zambia

In Zambia, n = 15 elephants participated in the trials (Appendix A, Table A2), with each trial lasting approximately two hours. The results for the effects of the repellent choice, repellent no-choice, water choice, and water no-choice conditions on the elephant behaviour in the Zambia trials are presented in Table 2.
The elephants in Zambia were more likely to sniff in the repellent no-choice condition (25% more likely than water no choice) and more likely to eat in the water no-choice condition (26% more likely than repellent no choice). There was an effect of condition on entering the plot, but this seemed to be more about whether they had choice or not, not whether there was repellent.
There was no effect of condition on alert behaviours, vocalisations, and approaching and turning away. Turning away immediately and strong reactions happened too rarely to analyse statistically, but it is notable that a strong reaction only occurred in repellent conditions.

3.2. Effect of Personality

3.2.1. Thailand

Generalised linear models revealed that elephants with a higher playfulness in the physical environment were significantly more likely to enter the plot in repellent trials (z = 2.05, p = 0.04; Figure 3). There were no effects of curiosity, fearfulness, or confidence in the physical environment on entering the plot (curiosity: z = −0.1, p = 0.92, fear: z = −0.83, p = 0.41, and confidence: z = 1.45, p = 0.15) or eating (curiosity: z = 1.57, p = 0.12, fear: z = −0.78, p = 0.44, and confidence: z = 1.00, p = 0.32), nor an effect of playfulness on likelihood to eat the food (z = −0.09, p = 0.93).
Regarding behaviour around humans, those rated as bolder around humans, although not statistically significant, did show a tendency to be more likely to enter the plot in repellent trials (z = 1.87, p = 0.06; Figure 4). There were no effects of aggressiveness or curiosity towards humans on likelihood to enter the plot (aggression: z = 0.07, p = 0.94, curiosity: z = −0.60, p = 0.55) or eat the food (aggression: z = 0.33, p = 0.74, curiosity: z = −0.18, p = 0.86), nor an effect of boldness on eating the food (z = 1.54, p = 0.12).

3.2.2. Zambia

Generalised linear models revealed that, in repellent trials, elephants with a higher playfulness in the physical environment had a tendency to be more likely to enter the plot (z = 1.67, p = 0.09). There were no effects of elephant sex, confidence, fear, or curiosity in the physical environment on entering the plot (sex: z = 0.24, p = 0.81, confidence: z = −0.52, p = 0.60, fear: z = 0.42, p = 0.67, and curiosity: z = −0.37, p = 0.71) or eating (sex: z = 1.14, p = 0.25, confidence: z = −0.59, p = 0.56, fear: z = 0.70, p = 0.48, and curiosity: z = −0.17, p = 0.87), nor an effect of playfulness on likelihood to eat (z = 0.13, p = 0.89).
In terms of personality with humans, elephants rated as more bold with humans were more likely to enter the plot than shy elephants (z = 2.04, p = 0.04; Figure 5) and there was a trend for elephants more curious around humans to be more likely to enter (z = 1.69, p = 0.09), but no effect of aggressiveness (z = −0.23, p = 0.82) or sex (z = −0.12, p = 0.90). Interestingly, boldness did not affect likelihood to eat the food (z = −0.15, p = 0.88) and nor did sex (z = 0.33, p = 0.74), but elephants more aggressive or more curious towards humans were significantly more likely to eat in repellent trials (aggressive: z = 96.44, p < 0.0001; Figure 6, curiosity: z = 25.25, p < 0.0001).

4. Discussion

The smelly elephant repellent triggered behavioural responses in the elephants in both Thailand and Zambia, indicating that the individuals were able to detect the repellent. However, the repellent did not prevent all the elephants from entering the plot (in Thailand) and/or eating the food sprayed with the repellent (in Thailand and Zambia). In Thailand, the fence deterred the elephants from entering the test plot as, when there was no fence present (during the spray conditions), the elephants entered the test plot more. In both Thailand and Zambia, even though the elephants ate the food during the repellent spray trials, they were less likely to eat compared to when the food was sprayed with water. In both species, the elephants behaviourally reacted more towards the repellent conditions compared to the water conditions, as they exhibited enhanced sniffing, alertness, and vocalisation behaviours. Anecdotal evidence from the observers suggests a dislike for the repellent, as some of the elephants were seen to wipe the repellent off the food (see Supplementary Materials, Videos S1–S3). This was not coded systematically in our observations, as we were not expecting to see this behaviour, and so it could have occurred more than was noted.
The results of this study, showing the elephants to enter the plot and eat the food, were surprising, as the smelly elephant repellent has successfully deterred wild elephants from entering crop fields in Kenya and Uganda [30]. However, this type of result has been documented previously when trialling the use of beehive fences as a human–elephant conflict mitigation deterrent. In the wild, elephants were deterred from entering farms due to beehive fences [57,58], but, with captive elephants, the beehives had no impact [59]. Thus, although captive environments provide controlled settings for experimental testing, the ecological validity of testing conflict mitigation methods with captive wildlife should be strongly considered [38,59,60,61]. This may be due to differences between captive and wild elephants in social and physical conditions, stress levels, and interactions with or exposure to humans.
The presence of a mahout/carer during the trials in both Thailand and Zambia could have impacted the elephants’ performances [62]. The working performance of captive elephants during novel situations has been documented to improve with mahout/carer presence. For example, in Myanmar, captive elephants were more likely to cross an unfamiliar barrier in the presence of their mahouts, and more so if they had known their mahout for over a year [62]. This presence may have made the elephants in this study feel more relaxed and safe to enter the unfamiliar test plot or eat the novel food. In Zambia, a feeling of safety was also previously anecdotally observed when spiky stones were being tested as a mitigation method and one of the elephants injured itself by walking straight over them, indicating that the elephant did not see the spikes as a threat (L. J. Olivier, pers. comm.). The captive setting in this study is very different to real life, despite our best attempts to recreate a real-world scenario. In the wild, entering a field of crops and consuming crops poses a significant risk to the survival of elephants through retaliatory injury by farmers [11]. However, the captive elephants in this study were not faced with this kind of threat and so there was no real risk for them to enter the test plot. In addition, captive elephants, in general, are not kept in natural social groups, which could impact their behaviour [35]. Thus, the lack of socio-ecological validity makes it very difficult to test behaviours associated with the conservation issues facing wild populations [35].
Finally, in captivity, individuals have more exposure to man-made objects and may be more used to human smells compared to wild individuals. This may result in less neophobia among captive animals compared to wild animals when exposed to a novel object and smell, like the set up in this experiment [63,64]. Nonetheless, behavioural effects of the repellent were still observed in our semi-captive samples, indicating the detection of and reaction to the repellent.
Despite previous findings that male elephants are more likely to crop-raid [18,19,21,22], our results in the Zambia sample showed that sex did not have a significant effect on behaviour when included in the same model as personality. Therefore, it seems that, in this sample, personality was more of a driver of behaviour around the repellent than sex. However, it is worth noting that in Zambia the elephants were younger, and so this result cannot be generalised to adults.
Personality differences between individuals, such as boldness towards novel scenarios and the ability to thrive in stressful captive environments, can impact performance in different situations [65,66]. This was seen in our study, as personality had an impact on whether the elephants entered the test plot or ate the food in the repellent trials. Both the Thai and Zambian elephants, two different species, showed similar effects of personality traits on repellent interactions, such as boldness and playfulness, with the bolder, more playful elephants being more likely to enter the plot. Boldness is related to the probability of exploring new things and neophilia, with highly neophilic animals being quick to approach and explore a novel object, while highly neophobic animals are slow to do so [60,67]. Wild animals are more neophobic than captive animals [60,61], suggesting that the repellent would be predicted to have a stronger effect on wild animals than on those in captivity.
These personality traits (boldness and playfulness) in wildlife are likely to play a role in determining whether some individuals are more or less likely to engage in risky behaviours that result in human–wildlife conflict [32,33,68]. Bolder animals are more likely to take risks. For example, bolder wild house sparrows are more likely to invade new areas and explore human-made objects [54]. Bolder coyotes are less likely to be scared by mitigation devices used to protect food [55]. It is likely that these traits will determine whether elephants make the decision to take considerable risks by entering farms and engaging with people by crop-raiding and breaking fences, and why other individuals avoid them by remaining inside protected areas [32,33,68]. Often, these risk-taking elephants are labelled as “problem” animals. However, depending on location, a small percentage of actual populations are “problem” individuals.

5. Conclusions

Understanding animal behaviour is essential for improving human–elephant coexistence and designing deterrence mechanisms. Appreciating personality traits in elephants, especially amongst “problem” elephants who have a greater propensity to crop-raid, could lead to the design of new mitigation methods designed to target these individuals. Moreover, the use of existing methods, such as the smelly elephant repellent, combined with other methods could be rotated at different times of the year to slow down learning and habituation [35].
Overall, both the African and Asian elephants in this study showed behavioural reactions to the smelly elephant repellent, and this was further affected by personality traits. Due to the potential differences between captive and wild elephants, caution has to be taken when testing human–elephant conflict mitigation methods, as the results may vary between the two contexts, as is evident when comparing this study to previous findings [30]. These results from captive settings can demonstrate whether elephants respond to the repellent. Future research is needed to understand the effectiveness of the smelly repellent in wild Asian elephants and African forest elephants (Loxodonta cyclotis).

Supplementary Materials

The following supporting information can be downloaded at: https://www.mdpi.com/article/10.3390/ani13213334/s1, Video S1: Thailand repellent bottle sniffing; Video S2: Thailand repellent cleaning; Video S3: Thailand repellent strong reaction.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, M.R.R. and L.N.T.; methodology, M.R.R., L.N.T., R.D., J.R. and L.J.O.; formal analysis, R.D.; investigation, J.R., L.Y., L.J.O., C.B. and I.B.N.; resources, J.R. and L.J.O.; data curation, R.D.; writing—original draft preparation, M.R.R. and L.N.T.; writing—review and editing, M.R.R., L.J.O., J.R., R.D. and L.N.T.; visualization, M.R.R., R.D. and L.N.T.; supervision, M.R.R. and L.N.T.; project administration, M.R.R. and L.N.T.; funding acquisition, M.R.R. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by WildAid.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was approved by the Ministry of Tourism, Department of National Parks and Wildlife (Zambia) and Golden Triangle Asian Elephant Foundation (Thailand)—2 December 2021, and No. 1514 from 1 February 2021.

Informed Consent Statement

Not applicable.

Data Availability Statement

The datasets presented in this article can be made available on request directed to the authors.

Acknowledgments

Thank you to the Golden Triangle Asian Elephant Foundation team who assisted with research set-up and preparation: Lamai Insumran, Nuttamon Chaichana, Prayat Dokmadua, Wattana Salagnarm, Atiphong Photijikjub, Niwat Paiwanniwet, Boonruen Yongrum and Watcharapon Priabsom. Thank you to Game Rangers International’s elephant keepers in Zambia, who assisted with this study: Batra Munkombwe, Hayes Matakala, Clifford Musungwa and Aaron Lisita, and to the Senior Research Assistant, Muriel Simukonda. Thank you to WildAid for its commitment to researching this human-elephant conflict mitigation method. Finally, we thank the anonymous reviewers for their positive and helpful feedback on this paper.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest. The funders had no direct role in the design of the study; in the collection, analyses, or interpretation of data; in the writing of the manuscript, or in the decision to publish the results.

Appendix A

Table A1. Elephant information: Thailand.
Table A1. Elephant information: Thailand.
No.ElephantAgeGroup#RF Trials#RS Trials#WF Trials#WS Trials
1Yuki3513333
2Poonlarb3513333
3Beau4223333
4Yui2523333
5Pleum2323333
6Dah1933333
7Pumpui4433333
8Jathong3043332
9Kamoon5043332
10Maemoo4753332
11Kamtoon1753221
12Maenoi1953332
13Lanna3363332
Notes: All elephants were female. Age = years. RF: repellent fence; RS: repellent spray; WF: water fence; and WS: water spray.
Table A2. Elephant information: Zambia.
Table A2. Elephant information: Zambia.
No.ElephantSexAge#RC Trials#RN Trials#WC Trials#WN Trials
1ChamilanduF153444
2KaseweF54233
3KavalamanjaF104443
4LudakaM42233
5MarambaM114443
6MkalivaF54443
7Mosi oa tunyaM104444
8MphamvuM84444
9MuchichiliM63343
10MulisaniM64434
11MusololeM104444
12Mutaanzi DavidM13443
13NkalaM84433
14RufunsaM103443
15TukoF32233
Notes: Age = years; RC: repellent choice; RN: repellent no choice; WC: water choice; and WN: water no choice.

Appendix B

Table A3. Behaviours coded.
Table A3. Behaviours coded.
BehaviourDescription
Sniffing behaviourOne or more of any of the following:
A Sniff-Toward: The trunk is held relatively straight and pointed in the direction of interest.
Periscope-sniff: The trunk lifted up in an s shape.
Hovering- Sniff: The trunk is held suspended over a particular scent on the ground or object of interest.
Alert behaviourOne or more of any of the following:
Head shake: An abrupt shaking of the head
Ears out: Fully spreads the ears (perpendicular, or almost, to its body).
Tail raised: The tail is raised so that it is no longer flat against the back legs.
VocalisingAny vocalisation (e.g., rumble, trumpet, or chirping).
Approached the plot straight awayWent within 5 m of the plot but then moved to be more than 5 m from the plot within 5 min.
Turned away from the plot immediatelyUpon release by the mahout, the elephant moved away from the plot within 5 min
Strong reactionThe elephant showed sniffing and/or alert and/or vocalisation, as well as turning away immediately.
Did nothingThe elephant did not move towards or away from the plot. They did not show any interest in the plot. They may have grazed in the area (not in the plot) or stood still.
OtherIt was noted if the elephant did any behaviour not included in this list.
Note: Sniffing and alert definitions taken from Poole and Granli, 2011 [69].

References

  1. De Silva, S.; Wu, T.; Nyhus, P.; Weaver, A.; Thieme, A.; Johnson, J.; Wadey, J.; Mossbrucker, A.; Vu, T.; Neang, T.; et al. Land-use change is associated with multi-century loss of elephant ecosystems in Asia. Sci. Rep. 2023, 13, 5996. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Wall, J.; Wittemyer, G.; Klinkenberg, B.; LeMay, V.; Blake, S.; Strindberg, S.; Henley, M.; Vollrath, F.; Maisels, F.; Ferwerda, J.; et al. Human footprint and protected areas shape elephant range across Africa. Curr. Biol. 2021, 31, 2437–2445. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. IUCN. IUCN SSC Guidelines on Human-Wildlife Conflict and Coexistence, 1st ed.; IUCN: Gland, Switzerland, 2021. [Google Scholar]
  4. Balmford, A.; Moore, J.L.; Brooks, T.; Burgess, N.; Hansen, L.A.; Williams, P.; Rahbek, C. Conservation conflicts across Africa. Science 2001, 291, 2616–2619. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Madden, F. Creating Coexistence between Humans and Wildlife: Global Perspectives on Local Efforts to Address Human–Wildlife Conflict. Hum. Dimens. Wildl. 2004, 9, 247–257. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Nyhus, P.J. Human–Wildlife Conflict and Coexistence. Annu. Rev. Environ. Resour. 2016, 41, 143–171. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Woodroffe, R.; Thirgood, S.; Rabinowitz, A. (Eds.) People and Wildlife Conflict or Coexistence? Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2005. [Google Scholar]
  8. Salerno, J.; Stevens, F.R.; Gaughan, A.E.; Hilton, T.; Bailey, K.; Bowles, T.; Cassidy, L.; Mupeta-Muyamwa, P.; Biggs, D.; Pricope, N.; et al. Wildlife impacts and changing climate pose compounding threats to human food security. Curr. Biol. 2021, 31, 5077–5085. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Barua, M.; Bhagwat, S.A.; Jadhav, S. The hidden dimensions of human-wildlife conflict: Health impacts, opportunity and transaction costs. Biol. Conserv. 2013, 157, 309–316. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Choudhury, A. Human–Elephant Conflicts in Northeast India. Hum. Dimens. Wildl. 2004, 9, 261–270. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Mijele, D.; Obanda, V.; Omondi, P.; Soriguer, R.C.; Gakuya, F.; Otiende, M.; Hongo, P.; Alasaad, S. Spatio-temporal distribution of injured elephants in masai mara and the putative negative and positive roles of the local community. PLoS ONE 2013, 8, e71179. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Dickman, A.J. Complexities of conflict: The importance of considering social factors for solving human-wildlife conflict. Anim. Conserv. 2010, 13, 458–466. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Pooley, S.; Barua, M.; Beinart, W.; Dickman, A.; Holmes, G.; Lorimer, J.; Loveridge, A.J.; Macdonald, D.W.; Marvin, G.; Redpath, S.; et al. An interdisciplinary review of current and future approaches to improving human–predator relations. Conserv. Biol. 2017, 31, 513–523. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Shaffer, L.J.; Khadka, K.K.; Van Den Hoek, J.; Naithani, K.J. Human–elephant conflict: A review of current management strategies and future directions. Front. Ecol. Evol. 2019, 6, 235. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Wettasin, M.; Chaiyarat, R.; Youngpoy, N.; Jieychien, N.; Sukmasuang, R.; Tanhan, P. Environmental factors induced crop raiding by wild Asian elephant (Elephas maximus) in the Eastern Economic Corridor, Thailand. Sci. Rep. 2023, 13, 13388. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Branco, P.S.; Merkle, J.A.; Pringle, R.M.; Pansu, J.; Potter, A.B.; Reynolds, A.; Stalmans, M.; Long, R.A. Determinants of elephant foraging behaviour in a coupled human natural system: Is brown the new green? J. Anim. Ecol. 2019, 88, 780–792. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Rode, K.D.; Chiyo, P.I.; Chapman, C.A.; McDowell, L.R. Nutritional ecology of elephants in Kibale National Park, Uganda, and its relationship with crop raiding behaviour. J. Trop. Ecol. 2006, 22, 441–449. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Sukumar, R. Ecology of the Asian elephant in southern India II: Feeding habits and crop-raiding patterns. J. Trop. Ecol. 1990, 6, 33–53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Sukumar, R.; Gadgil, M. Male-female differences in foraging on crops by Asian elephants. Anim. Behav. 1988, 36, 1233–1235. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Pokharel, S.S.; Singh, B.; Seshagiri, P.B.; Sukumar, R. Lower levels of glucocorticoids in crop-raiders: Diet quality as a potential ‘pacifier’ against stress in free-ranging Asian elephants in a human-production habitat. Anim. Conserv. 2019, 22, 177–188. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Chiyo, P.I.; Cochrane, E.P. Population structure and behaviour of crop-raiding elephants in Kibale National Park, Uganda. Afr. J. Ecol. 2005, 43, 233–241. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Osborn, F.V. Seasonal variation of feeding patterns and food selection by crop-raiding elephants in Zimbabwe. Afr. J. Ecol. 2004, 42, 322–327. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Chiyo, P.I.; Lee, P.C.; Moss, C.J.; Archie, E.A.; Hollister-Smith, J.A.; Alberts, S. No risk, no gain: Effects of crop raiding and genetic diversity on body size in male elephants. Behav. Ecol. 2011, 22, 552–558. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Douglas-Hamilton, I.; Krink, T.; Vollrath, F. Movements and corridors of African elephants in relation to protected areas. Naturwissenschaften 2005, 92, 158–163. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Graham, M.D.; Douglas-Hamilton, I.; Adams, W.M.; Lee, P.C. The movement of African elephants in a human-dominated land-use mosaic. Anim. Conserv. 2009, 12, 445–455. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Ihwagi, F.W.; Skidmore, A.K.; Wang, T.; Bastille-Rousseau, G.; Toxopeus, A.G.; Douglas-Hamilton, I. Poaching lowers elephant path tortuosity: Implications for conservation. J. Wildl. Manag. 2019, 83, 1022–1031. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Von Hagen, R.L.; Kasaine, S.; Githiru, M.; Amakobe, B.; Mutwiwa, U.N.; Schulte, B.A. Metal strip fences for preventing African elephant (Loxodonta africana) crop foraging in the Kasigau Wildlife Corridor, Kenya. Afr. J. Ecol. 2021, 59, 293–298. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Adams, T.S.; Mwezi, I.; Jordan, N.R. Panic at the disco: Solar-powered strobe light barriers reduce field incursion by African elephants Loxodonta africana in Chobe District, Botswana. Oryx 2021, 55, 739–746. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. La Grange, M.; Matema, C.; Nyamukure, B.; Hoare, R. The Virtual Fence Dynamic: A Breakthrough for Low-Cost and Sustainable Mitigation of Human-Elephant Conflict in Subsistence Agriculture? Front. Conserv. Sci. 2022, 3, 863180. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Tiller, L.N.; Oniba, E.; Opira, G.; Brennan, E.J.; King, L.E.; Ndombi, V.; Wanjala, D.; Robertson, M.R. “Smelly” Elephant Repellent: Assessing the Efficacy of a Novel Olfactory Approach to Mitigating Elephant Crop Raiding in Uganda and Kenya. Diversity 2022, 14, 509. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Kiffner, C.; Schaal, I.; Cass, L.; Peirce, K.; Sussman, O.; Grueser, A.; Wachtel, E.; Adams, H.; Clark, K.; Konig, H.J.; et al. Perceptions and realities of elephant crop raiding and mitigation methods. Conserv. Sci. Pract. 2021, 3, 372. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Mumby, H.S.; Plotnik, J.M. Taking the elephants’ perspective: Remembering elephant behavior, cognition and ecology in human- elephant conflict mitigation. Front. Ecol. Evol. 2018, 6, 122. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Barrett, L.P.; Stanton, L.; Benson-Amram, S. The cognition of ‘nuisance’ species. Anim. Behav. 2019, 147, 167–177. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Mutinda, M.; Chenge, G.; Gakuya, F.; Otiende, M.; Omondi, P.; Kasiki, S.; Soriguer, R.C.; Alasaad, S. Detusking fence-breaker elephants as an approach in human-elephant conflict mitigation. PLoS ONE 2014, 9, e91749. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Plotnik, J.M.; Jacobson, S.L. A “thinking animal” in conflict: Studying wild elephant cognition in the shadow of anthropogenic change. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 2022, 46, 101148. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Niimura, Y.; Matsui, A.; Touhara, K. Extreme expansion of the olfactory receptor gene repertoire in African elephants and evolutionary dynamics of orthologous gene groups in 13 placental mammals. Genome Res. 2014, 24, 1485–1496. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Bates, L.A.; Sayialel, K.N.; Njiraini, N.W.; Poole, J.H.; Moss, C.J.; Byrne, R.W. African elephants have expectations about the locations of out-of-sight family members. Biol. Lett. 2008, 4, 34–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Plotnik, J.M.; Brubaker, D.L.; Dale, R.; Tiller, L.N.; Mumby, H.S.; Clayton, N.S. Elephants have a nose for quantity. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2019, 116, 12566–12571. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Schulte, B.A.; LaDue, C.A. The Chemical Ecology of Elephants: 21st Century Additions to Our Understanding and Future Outlooks. Animals 2021, 11, 2860. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Santiapillai, C.; Read, B. Would masking the smell of ripening paddy-fields help mitigate human–elephant conflict in Sri Lanka? Oryx 2010, 44, 509–511. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Osborn, F.V.; Rasmussen, L.E.L. Evidence for the effectiveness of an oleo-resin capsicum aerosol as a repellent against wild elephants in Zimbabwe. Pachyderm 1995, 20, 55–64. [Google Scholar]
  42. Osborn, F.V. Capsicum oleoresin as an elephant repellent: Field trials in the communal lands of Zimbabwe. J. Wildl. Manag. 2002, 66, 674–677. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Wright, M.G.; Spencer, C.; Cook, R.M.; Henley, M.D.; North, W.; Mafra-Neto, A. African bush elephants respond to a honeybee alarm pheromone blend. Curr. Biol. 2018, 28, 778–780. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  44. Valenta, K.; Schmitt, M.H.; Ayasse, M.; Nevo, O. The sensory ecology of fear: African elephants show aversion to olfactory predator signals. Conserv. Sci. Pract. 2021, 3, 333. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Osborn, F.; Parker, G. Community-based methods to reduce crop loss to elephants: Experiments in the communal lands of Zimbabwe. Pachyderm 2002, 33, 32–38. [Google Scholar]
  46. Parker, G.E.; Osborn, F.V. Investigating the potential for chilli Capsicum spp. to reduce human-wildlife conflict in Zimbabwe. Oryx 2006, 40, 343–346. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Pozo, R.A.; Coulson, T.; McCulloch, G.; Stronza, A.; Songhurst, A. Chilli briquettes modify the temporal behaviour of elephants, but not their numbers. Oryx 2017, 53, 100–108. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Karidozo, M.; Osborn, F.V. Community based conflict mitigation trials: Results of field test of chilli as an elephant deterrent. Biodivers. Endanger. Species 2014, 3, 144. [Google Scholar]
  49. Hedges, S.; Gunaryadi, D. Chilli-based elephant deterrents. Oryx 2009, 44, 139–146. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Oniba, E.; Robertson, M.R. Trialling a new scent-based repellent to mitigate elephant crop-raiding around Murchison Falls National Park, Uganda. Pachyderm 2019, 60, 123–125. [Google Scholar]
  51. Snyder, K.D.; Renstch, D. Rethinking assessment of success of mitigation strategies for elephant-induced crop damage. Conserv. Biol. 2020, 34, 829–842. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Sitati, N.W.; Walpole, M.J. Assessing farm-based measures for mitigating human-elephant conflict in Transmara District, Kenya. Oryx 2006, 40, 279–286. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Horback, K.M.; Miller, L.J.; Kuczaj II, S.A. Personality assessment in African elephants (Loxodonta africana): Comparing the temporal stability of ethological coding versus trait rating. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2013, 149, 55–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Quesada, J.; Chavez-Zichinelli, C.A.; Garcia-Arroyo, M.; Yeh, P.J.; Guevara, R.; Izquierdo-Palma, J.; MacGregor-Fors, I. Bold or shy? Examining the risk-taking behavior and neophobia of invasive and non-invasive house sparrows. Anim. Biodivers. Conserv. 2022, 45, 97–106. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Darrow, P.A.; Shivik, J.A. Bold, shy, and persistent: Variable coyote response to light and sound stimuli. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2009, 116, 82–87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Bates, D.; Mächler, M.; Bolker, B.; Walker, S. Fitting Linear Mixed-Effects Models Using lme4. J. Stat. Softw. 2015, 67, 1–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. King, L.E.; Lala, F.; Nzumu, H.; Mwambingu, E.; Douglas-Hamilton, I. Beehive fences as a multidimensional conflict-mitigation tool for farmers coexisting with elephants. Conserv. Biol. 2017, 31, 743–752. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Van de Water, A.; King, L.E.; Arkajak, R.; Arkajak, J.; van Doormaal, N.; Ceccarelli, V.; Sluiter, L.; Doornwaard, S.M.; Praet, V.; Owen, D.; et al. Beehive fences as a sustainable local solution to human-elephant conflict in Thailand. Conserv. Sci. Pract. 2020, 2, e260. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Dror, S.; Harich, F.; Duangphakdee, O.; Savini, T.; Pogány, Á.; Roberts, J.; Geheran, J.; Treydte, A.C. Are Asian elephants afraid of honeybees? Experimental studies in northern Thailand. Mamm. Biol. 2020, 100, 355–363. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Benson-Amram, S.; Weldele, M.L.; Holekamp, K.E. A comparison of innovative problem-solving abilities between wild and captive spotted hyaenas, Crocuta crocuta. Anim. Behav. 2013, 85, 349–356. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. McCune, K.B.; Jablonski, P.; Lee, S.I.; Ha, R.R. Captive jays exhibit reduced problem-solving performance compared to wild conspecifics. R. Soc. Open Sci. 2019, 6, 181311. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Liehrmann, O.; Crawley, J.A.; Seltmann, M.W.; Feillet, S.; Nyein, U.K.; Aung, H.H.; Htut, W.; Lahdenperä, M.; Lansade, L.; Lummaa, V. Handler familiarity helps to improve working performance during novel situations in semi-captive Asian elephants. Sci. Rep. 2021, 11, 15480. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Huber, L.; Gajdon, G.K. Technical intelligence in animals: The kea model. Anim. Cogn. 2006, 9, 295–305. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  64. Van de Waal, E.; Bshary, R. Contact with human facilities appears to enhance technical skills in wild vervet monkeys (Chlorocebus aethiops). Folia Primatol. 2019, 81, 282–291. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Biro, P.A.; Dingemanse, N.J. Sampling bias resulting from animal personality. Trends Ecol. Evol. 2008, 24, 66–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  66. Sih, A.; Del Giudice, M. Linking behavioural syndromes and cognition: A behavioural ecology perspective. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B 2012, 367, 2762–2772. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  67. Day, R.L.; Coe, R.L.; Kendal, J.R.; Laland, K.N. Neophilia, innovation and social learning: A study of intergeneric differences in callitrichid monkeys. Anim. Behav. 2003, 65, 559–571. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Goldenberg, S.Z.; Douglas-Hamilton, I.; Daballen, D.; Wittemyer, G. Challenges of using behavior to monitor anthropogenic impacts on wildlife: A case study on illegal killing of African elephants. Anim. Conserv. 2017, 20, 215–224. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Poole, J.H.; Granli, P. Signals, Gestures, and Behavior of African Elephants. In The Amboseli Elephants; Moss, C.J., Ed.; University of Chicago Press: Chicago, IL, USA, 2011; pp. 109–124. [Google Scholar]
Figure 3. Likelihood of entering the plot according to playfulness in the physical environment (higher score = higher playfulness) in the Thai sample.
Figure 3. Likelihood of entering the plot according to playfulness in the physical environment (higher score = higher playfulness) in the Thai sample.
Animals 13 03334 g003
Figure 4. Likelihood of entering the plot according to boldness towards humans (higher score = higher boldness) in the Thai sample.
Figure 4. Likelihood of entering the plot according to boldness towards humans (higher score = higher boldness) in the Thai sample.
Animals 13 03334 g004
Figure 5. Likelihood of entering the plot according to boldness towards humans (higher score = higher boldness) in the Zambian sample.
Figure 5. Likelihood of entering the plot according to boldness towards humans (higher score = higher boldness) in the Zambian sample.
Animals 13 03334 g005
Figure 6. Likelihood of eating the food according to aggressiveness towards humans (higher score = higher aggressiveness) in the Zambian sample.
Figure 6. Likelihood of eating the food according to aggressiveness towards humans (higher score = higher aggressiveness) in the Zambian sample.
Animals 13 03334 g006
Table 1. Percentage of elephants showing each behaviour, according to condition, in Thailand.
Table 1. Percentage of elephants showing each behaviour, according to condition, in Thailand.
BehaviourRepellent FenceWater FenceRepellent SprayWater SprayModel p-Value
Sniffing74.3660.5397.3737.50p < 0.0001
Alert35.9015.7921.050.00p = 0.0001
Vocalisation46.1542.1150.0018.75p = 0.02
Approach immediate66.6781.58100.00100.00p < 0.0001
Approach then turn58.9734.2163.1631.25p = 0.007
Turn away immediate5.132.637.890.00p > 0.05
Strong reaction2.562.6313.160.00p = 0.04
Enter plot79.4989.47100.00100.00p < 0.0001
Eat82.0582.8674.29100.00p = 0.0009
Table 2. Percentage of elephants showing each behaviour, according to condition, in Zambia.
Table 2. Percentage of elephants showing each behaviour, according to condition, in Zambia.
BehaviourRepellent ChoiceRepellent No ChoiceWater ChoiceWater No ChoiceModel p-Value
Sniffing53.8583.0265.4558.00p = 0.005
Alert34.6226.4229.0932.00p > 0.05
Vocalisation19.2320.7514.5514.00p > 0.05
Approach immediate36.5426.4218.1836.00p = 0.03
Approach then turn1.921.895.456.00p > 0.05
Turn away immediate0.003.773.640.00-
Strong reaction0.001.890.000.00-
Enter plot29.4166.0423.6448.00p < 0.0001
Eat21.5733.9634.5560.00p < 0.0001
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Robertson, M.R.; Olivier, L.J.; Roberts, J.; Yonthantham, L.; Banda, C.; N’gombwa, I.B.; Dale, R.; Tiller, L.N. Testing the Effectiveness of the “Smelly” Elephant Repellent in Controlled Experiments in Semi-Captive Asian and African Savanna Elephants. Animals 2023, 13, 3334. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani13213334

AMA Style

Robertson MR, Olivier LJ, Roberts J, Yonthantham L, Banda C, N’gombwa IB, Dale R, Tiller LN. Testing the Effectiveness of the “Smelly” Elephant Repellent in Controlled Experiments in Semi-Captive Asian and African Savanna Elephants. Animals. 2023; 13(21):3334. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani13213334

Chicago/Turabian Style

Robertson, Marion R., Lisa J. Olivier, John Roberts, Laddawan Yonthantham, Constance Banda, Innocent B. N’gombwa, Rachel Dale, and Lydia N. Tiller. 2023. "Testing the Effectiveness of the “Smelly” Elephant Repellent in Controlled Experiments in Semi-Captive Asian and African Savanna Elephants" Animals 13, no. 21: 3334. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani13213334

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop